4,615
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Special issue articles

Towards an international understanding of the power of celebrity persuasions: a review and a research agenda

Pages 486-504 | Received 14 Aug 2013, Accepted 29 Jan 2015, Published online: 23 Sep 2015

Abstract

Research into advertising using celebrity has been undertaken for nearly 40 years. It has principally used surveys and experiments to explore how consumers respond to celebrity advertisements. A recent meta-study of 32 papers has demonstrated that different populations respond in different ways to celebrity endorsements. Specifically, both US subjects and college students are more likely to respond in a significant way to the presence of celebrity than subjects who are not from the US, or who are not studying at college. Given that the nationality and student status of subjects matter, this article explores the make up of the samples that have been used to examine celebrity advertising. The article finds that these samples are not representative of US populations (because so many are students), nor of populations outside the US (because so few live beyond it). Furthermore, the history of dominance of US-based student samples, and the citation practices which keep them circulating in academia, suggests that theories of celebrity advertising have for a long time been excessively influenced by ideas tested on this unrepresentative group. This fact will limit the applicability of research into celebrity advertising to the wider world. I explore whether this matters, and how deficiencies might be addressed in further research.

Introduction

The effect of celebrity on different consumer groups has provoked continual interest in advertising and marketing journals. In US markets, advertising using celebrities represents a significant minority of all television advertising (nearly 10% of television advertising in 2002/03; Choi et al. Citation2005). Moreover, it has provided some phenomenal success stories (Pringle Citation2004). Regardless of whether celebrity advertising always works so well, the fact that it can succeed remarkably whets the appetite of advertisers.

It is important to distinguish between how celebrity advertising works with consumers and how people think it works. There is often a great deal of faith in the market in the power of celebrity advertising. When rumours of Michael Jordan’s return to the National Basketball Association hit Wall Street in 1995, firms whose products enjoyed his endorsement enjoyed a 2% increase in their value – worth in total almost $1 billion (Mathur et al. Citation1997). This was a good return on the £32 million Jordan was being paid annually to endorse products in 1993. Similarly, when firms announce celebrity endorsements in advertising campaigns before the campaigns begin, their stock value tends to rise (Agrawal and Kamakura Citation1995).

Such faith in celebrity advertising tells us that it can reap dividends. But it does not tell us how celebrity advertising works with consumers. Researchers have used four main theories to explain the effectiveness of celebrity advertising:

  1. The ‘source credibility’ model suggests that celebrity endorsements work because of the credibility of the celebrity as a spokesperson for the product. Credibility here has three elements – attractiveness, trustworthiness and expertise (Hovland and Weiss Citation1951, Ohanian Citation1991).

  2. The ‘source attractiveness’ model explores one of the dimensions of the credibility model – attractiveness – in more depth, given the power of beauty also to connote intelligence and believability (Friedman et al. Citation1976, Kahle and Homer Citation1985).

  3. The ‘product match-up hypothesis’ examines the fit between the image of the celebrity and the product. This extends ideas developed in the first two theories to consider how the qualities of the celebrity map onto the qualities of the product they are endorsing (Kamins Citation1990, Kamins and Gupta Citation1994).

  4. Finally there is the more complicated ‘meaning transfer model’ developed by Grant McCracken in the late 1980s (McCracken Citation1989). His theory posits that the socially ascribed meaning (what we would call now aspects of their brand) which public figures build up over the course of their career is transferred to the product through endorsements. If consumers identify with the celebrity, or at least recognise their socially ascribed meaning, then that meaning will be transferred to the products being endorsed, and from the product to the consumer by the consumer when she purchases it.

Research based on these theories into how celebrity advertising works on different consumer groups has now been undertaken for the best part of 40 years. It is useful at this juncture to consider what has been learnt from this research in combination. There have been few published reviews. One was produced by Erdogan (Citation1999), and a meta-analysis was conducted by Amos et al (Citation2008) which I discuss below. Both were concerned with what could be concluded from the findings of previous studies with respect to the predictions of the theories summarised above.

This article provides a different sort of overview. My concern is to examine the methods used to test these theories, and specifically the samples used. In this article I first explore why these samples matter. Then I consider what the empirical basis of research used to test theories of celebrity advertising has been. I discuss the implications for the biases found and consider how they might be addressed with further research into celebrity advertising.

Samples and methods in celebrity advertising research

Research into celebrity has almost exclusively used quantitative methods to explore the impact of celebrity endorsements. Qualitative studies exist (Tantiseneepong et al. Citation2012), but are rare. This is in keeping with other marketing research. A recent review of nearly 2600 papers published between 2003 and 2009 in the nine leading marketing journals found only 99 that had used qualitative and quantitative methods (Harrison and Reilly Citation2011). Furthermore, the bibliographies of quantitative studies do not lead back to qualitative research. Journals such as Qualitative Market Research do not feature in their references. The insights of qualitative research have rarely been used by researchers of celebrity advertising.

With respect to the quantitative methods applied, two have been used most frequently. Experimental approaches present advertisements with or without celebrity to different groups of people and explore their responses. Surveys ask samples of consumers their opinions about celebrity advertising. Both methods have tended to involve large sample sizes; the mean sample size of the studies reviewed here is 225. There have also been proxy measures of advertising effectiveness which have looked at how celebrity advertising altered company share values, and surveys of business leaders’ opinions about celebrity advertising. These again have used quantitative methods.

What does this accumulation of quantitative data tell us about celebrity advertising? In an important paper, Clinton Amos and colleagues conducted a meta-review on 32 studies (culled from 87 on the topic) to identify the most significant effects across the published literature (Amos et al. Citation2008). A significant effect indicates that the subjects examined responded in some significant way to the presence of the celebrity in the advert, either positively or negatively. An insignificant effect indicated that the presence of the celebrity did not appear to make much difference to subjects’ thinking. The meta-study showed, perhaps unsurprisingly, that credible, attractive and expert celebrities are more effective than those who are not. The most important factor was negative information about a celebrity, which was a powerful predictor of endorsements not working.

However, Amos et al.’s meta-study also identified two other, perhaps more interesting, variables which affected the outcome of the studies. They observed that studies of students are much more likely to find significant celebrity effects than studies of non-student populations, and that studies of US consumers are more likely to observe significant effects of celebrity endorsement than consumers living in other parts of the world (Amos et al. Citation2008, pp. 221 and 226). Specifically they observe that of the 185 significant effects of celebrity advertising on consumers observed across their survey, 73% derived from US samples and 63% could be attributed to student populations. Of the 81 insignificant effects of celebrity advertising on consumers, 66% were observed in non-US populations and 71% among non-students.

I have redrawn the relevant part of Amos et al.’s table using revised data supplied by the authors (; Amos, pers. comm., 5 October 2012). This shows the distribution of effects by population group. It is clear that student populations are highly likely to respond significantly to celebrity advertisements because 83% of effects are significant among student samples (). Non-students are more evenly split: 53% of effects are significant. In US populations, 80% of effects are significant; in non-US populations, only 43% are. In both cases these differences in distribution are statistically significant (chi-square test results are reported in the footnote to ). Or in plain English, US populations and student populations are likely to respond in significant ways to celebrity advertisements. The presence of celebrity in an advertisement appears to affect the way they think or behave. Non-US populations are unlikely to respond in significant ways, whereas non-student populations are more equivocal.

Table 1. Distribution of source effects from Amos et al. (Citation2008).

It is unfortunately not possible to explore how populations who are neither from the US or who are not students compare with the effects demonstrated by populations who are both from the US and who are students. The relevant SPSS file required to produce those data cannot now be located (Amos, pers. comm., 5 October 2012). Nevertheless it is clear that the two results do exist separately from each other, and would be likely to accentuate each other. In both US and non-US samples a majority of the sample were students, so we cannot explain the difference between countries according to their student status. Similarly for both students and non-students a majority of the sample were from the US, so we cannot explain differences according to nationality.

These differences are important. They matter because if quantitative methods such as experiments and surveys are used to test theories, then we have to be sure that the samples used are representative of a broader population. This is a prerequisite of the quantitative methods used thus far. If they are not representative of a broader population, then we cannot safely extrapolate to other groups. US students are not representative of the rest of the US with respect to their response to celebrity, because students respond differently from non-students. Nor are US subjects representative of populations from different countries for the same reason. Theories of celebrity advertising which want to be relevant for these other consumer groups will need to be tested on different, more appropriate, populations.

If research into celebrity endorsements has been using inappropriate samples, then it is possible that models of celebrity advertising are being built on false pretenses. Advertising and marketing professionals, and students on advertising and marketing courses will read papers about the power and effectiveness (or otherwise) of celebrity advertising but be unable to determine rigorously which population groups it is appropriate to apply those theories to. There is a risk of over-extrapolating the significance of celebrity advertising’s effects by testing theories of celebrity advertising on populations who respond more to celebrity than do other groups.

It is therefore quite important to quantify more precisely how many of the people asked about celebrity advertising in surveys or experiments are students or based in the US, and how many are that more common type of consumer – people who are not students or who are not living in the US. We need, in other words, to explore in detail the make-up of the samples that lead to these publications. That task provides the main substance of this article. It presents one general research question and three sub-questions:

  • Research Question 1: Of which populations are samples collected to test theories about celebrity advertising representative?

    • Sub-Question 1: What proportion of the samples is composed of US subjects?

    • Sub-Question 2: What proportion of the samples is composed of students?

    • Sub-Question 3: What proportion of the samples is composed of US students?

It is also important to examine how this knowledge has evolved over time. Is research based on US students dated? Is the work on non-US subjects very recent? Are there any trends in the development of the collective sample of celebrity advertising research that might affect the body of knowledge produced by it? This generates the second research question:

  • Research Question 2: How has the constitution of the samples used in celebrity advertising research changed over time?

Finally, we are assuming that this knowledge matters because it is read and noted by the academe and by marketing and advertising professionals. However, we cannot assume that each paper is equally impactful. It might be quite possible for the samples used to test a theory to be unrepresentative of a larger population, but for readers to gain a more representative picture by using the papers selectively. They might, for example, take much more interest in results drawn from non-student populations, and this may counter a tendency to over-sample students. This provides the last research question:

  • Research Question 3: How have data based on different sorts of samples been used by other researchers working on theories of celebrity advertising?

Methods

To survey the celebrity advertising literature I first turned to existing surveys. I included all the studies I could obtain from the Amos et al (Citation2008) review, and from Erfgen’s (Citation2011) and Erdogan’s (Citation1999) reviews of these literatures. Then I searched for other more recent papers (since 2004) in the journals in which these authors had found their studies. I also included theses I have come across in other literature and studies on the impact of celebrity on political campaigns. The journals I surveyed are presented in .

Table 2. Journals surveyed for this research.

This search produced 96 relevant papers and theses, of which 83 were published papers. For each document I recorded the number of people who took part in the experiment/survey, whether they were students or not (whether graduates or undergraduates), and which country they lived in. Recording the country of the study was not always straightforward because it was not always mentioned, particularly in some of the earlier papers. However, since the authors of these papers were based in US universities, were generally describing work undertaken with students, and would often refer to the students’ university according to whether it was ‘East Coast’ or ‘Mid-West’ and so forth, it was plain where the study was undertaken. I also recorded the samples’ mean age and their gender, but not enough studies provided this information for it to be used in the analysis.

I have presented in the following information about the samples from the published papers only, because it is these which are most accessible in the public domain. These documents are most able to exert influence on the thinking of advertising and marketing professionals, and students, who may encounter them. Therefore the make-up of their samples is important. The papers used for the following data are marked with an asterisk in the Reference list.

To gauge the nature of a paper’s influence I collected citation data from the Web of Knowledge for each published article. The flaw in this method is that it can result in unpopular papers scoring highly, even though they are only referred to because of their flaws. However, having read these papers it is clear that this happens rarely, if at all.

Results

The vast majority of the papers in this literature review are based on work undertaken by scholars working in the US (). Of the 83 papers published on celebrity advertising, 76% were based on work there. The samples for the other papers are scattered all over the world, with Europe providing the main secondary concentration of research (nine published papers).

Table 3. Countries in which the work in the published papers analysed in this article was conducted.

Unsurprisingly, the vast majority of the people constituting the samples of surveys, or taking part in experiments, were resident in the US (). Most of the studies have also worked with samples of students, although students are only a minority of the non-US subjects. The vast majority of these samples were from single-country studies. There are only a couple of incidences when authors have tried to make international comparisons and surveyed people outside their own countries.

Table 4. Number of students and non-students from the US and non-US countries making up the samples of the published papers.

The tendency to survey US populations, and US students, here is notable (). Seventy-three per cent of the people sampled in these studies were from the US, and 79% of those subjects were students. Altogether 58% of all the people making up the samples in all of the published papers were US students, and only 13% of all the samples represent people who are neither from the US nor college students.

Please note, however, that samples taken from outside the US have a much more even split of students to non-students (48% to 52%). This is still a higher proportion of the population than students actually constitute, but nonetheless is more representative than the US samples. This means also that US students dominate the student samples. Only 23% of students who were sampled for quantitative research on celebrity came from outside the US.

More striking still than the stark US-student bias of this research is the fact that US populations were exclusively sampled by the authors of this literature until relatively recently (). Non-US populations only began to be studied from 1998 onwards. The geographical spread of subjects in the samples outside the United States is now quite diverse, with no particular country dominating, although Europe is the best region represented (). But this is a recent phenomenon. The fact that such samples now constitute as much as 25% of the total sample population is due in part to just four Asian surveys, which account for half of the non-US samples assembled here. Without those four surveys, US populations would still constitute nearly 90% of the sample population.

Table 5. Sampling of different countries and populations over time.

Table 6. Sample size from countries other than the US.

I have presented this information in two ways in . The left-hand side of the table presents the raw data, and the right-hand side presents the cumulative proportions of the make up of the collective samples by US subjects and US students respectively. The cumulative proportions are important because they give an indication of what anyone reviewing the literature will encounter as they look back through it. Thus, someone reading the literature at the start of 2002 would encounter samples totalling over 9400 subjects, 97% of whom were American, and 78% of whom were US college students.

Furthermore, not only is there a historically dominant body of US-based, and US-student-based, knowledge about celebrity advertising, but papers based on such subjects are much more influential within the literature than papers based on work in other countries, and on other population groups. Papers based on US subjects account for 96% of all citations. Those based on US students account for 72% of citations, whereas US students only constitute 58% of the collective sample ().

Table 7. Citation count of different sample types.

Such dominance is to be expected given that papers earlier on in this sequence could only cite US-based work; no other work existed. However, since papers based on other countries have begun to be published they have tended to be cited less than papers published based on US subjects which were published in equivalent years. Papers based on non-US populations are cited on average 0.6 times for each year of their existence, and those on US populations 2.2 times per year.

For the purpose of this argument it matters not why these non-US papers are cited less. They may simply deserve little attention. What matters is that samples used to test theories of celebrity advertising are based largely on US subjects, and that this effect is exaggerated by the attention paid to these samples in the literature.

Discussion

Geographical and historical differences

There is one important caveat we must consider with respect to Amos et al.’s findings before we can consider further implications of these results. It is possible that the difference between US and non-US populations which Amos et al. (Citation2008) identify reflects a historical difference not a geographical difference. No research on celebrity advertising was conducted outside the US before 1998. The difference between the US and non-US populations could therefore reflect a difference between results produced before and after that year. It is theoretically possible for both US and non-US samples to be producing few significant results after 1998. This would be possible if the non-significant US effects produced after 1998 were masked by the scale of the significant results found in US samples before 1998. If this has happened then it would suggest that US populations are taking less notice of celebrity now than then, and have become more like non-US consumers.

Interest in celebrity can wax and wane over time. But the scenario I have just sketched is an unlikely possibility. I have not come across any indication that interest in celebrity has been declining in the US in the last 14 years. If anything, the popular perception is that the reverse is true. Certainly within academia there has been an increase in the power and reach of celebrity over this same time period (Beer and Penfold-Mounce Citation2010). Assuming therefore that the differences between the countries are real, we can proceed to discuss their implications

The implications of the US/student bias

There are obvious reasons for the biases demonstrated here. The US houses a large number of advertising and marketing professors, who command the ready attention of a large number of students in their classes. They can offer the students course credits for taking part in their experiments and surveys (a common practice) and thus can produce a large amount of fact about responses to celebrity advertising for free.

However, this method presents a problem for the accumulated celebrity advertising literature because its findings are derived from the most celebrity-orientated population. Theories about celebrity advertising have been based on the collective views of a population which is not representative of the rest of the US. Furthermore, the US populations are not representative of the rest of the world. The extent to which these theories will be useful for talking about the responses to celebrity advertising from different population groups is more limited than has thus far been realised.

Note that I am not criticising any individual author of any particular paper. Most papers I have read were quite careful to consider the limitations of their sample for the conclusions they could draw. Most recognised that students were not representative of the population as a whole – but were still an important market and deserved attention for that reason. These are sound arguments. Their cumulative effect, however, is to produce an unrepresentative picture of responses to celebrity advertising.

These effects will have been particularly marked up until relatively recently. Since 2000, a significant minority, but a growing proportion, of the subjects sampled has not been US subjects (). Since most people mostly read the more recent literature, this may diminish the some of the domination of the US focus of the exclusively American era of advertising research pre 2000. However, anyone reading back though the literature will quickly encounter work that was either only composed of US samples or was written informed by such work. In that respect the influence of a biased sample early on in the formation of a body of literature has an enduring effect. There is inertia towards such knowledge. In any case, while recent literature is more balanced than before, it is still unbalanced. US subjects still account for most of the sampling conducted for most of the papers published about celebrity advertising. Moreover, their dominance in the literature is accentuated by citation practices which pay more attention to research conducted on US populations.

Figure 1. Trends in sampling in celebrity marketing literature from 2000 onwards.

Figure 1. Trends in sampling in celebrity marketing literature from 2000 onwards.

It is not possible to determine from these data whether the limited social basis of tests about theories of celebrity advertising matters very much. We know that should business, advertising and marketing students, practitioners and researchers explore this literature, then they will be exposed to unrepresentative results. Their understanding of the power of celebrity advertising may be skewed by the domination of a celebrity-orientated population’s views about it in the literature which they read. On the basis of the evidence presented here, students and researchers reading about the effects of celebrity advertising will not have been able to glean a reasonable picture of what celebrity advertising does to most people. We cannot tell, however, how influential those theories actually are either in the minds of these students or in the practices of advertising and marketing professionals.

Even if these results have been influential, then would it matter? Do the opinions of non-US subjects matter with respect to celebrity advertising? Not if we are concerned only with US markets. Indeed, given that such consumers behave so differently, people interested in US markets and US consumers should actively ignore research based elsewhere. The presence of non-US research in recent years would simply be a distraction for such people. All that is required is to undertake more research into non-student populations, because the current constitution of the collective sample is not representative of US populations.

However, regardless of its actual influence, it is unsatisfactory to have a body of theory based on such limited samples and for those limitations to persist unrecognised. It could distort readers’ views of the world. Moreover, if non-US markets are interesting, then we will need to take more interest in research based in them. With globalisation these markets are becoming more important. Internationally famous figures are being used to endorse brands across different countries, and at international gatherings (the Olympics, the FIFA World Cup) which concentrates attention of audiences from numerous countries. As brands (of products and celebrities) expand their international reach, advertisers will need to build a stronger empirical base upon which to base the value of their associations.

Globalisation, however, does not mean that we can assume the world is becoming more uniform. It is more connected, but the different economies and societies being connected can vary considerably. There is good reason to think that they vary in response to celebrity. The few studies which have explicitly compared US with non-US samples have noted the value of doing so. For example, Silvera and Austad, comparing Norwegian and US students, suggested that the lack of influence of celebrity could reflect the lower prominence of celebrity culture, and celebrity credibility generally, in Norwegian (and Nordic) countries more generally (Silvera and Austad Citation2003). I have demonstrated elsewhere that, with respect to British audiences, public reception of celebrity advocacy for good causes is surprisingly muted, at the same time as it is still widely believed to be powerful (Brockington Citation2014, Brockington and Henson Citation2014).

Conversely there are also comparisons with Asian countries, where celebrity advertising is apparently much more extensive and, by implication, more powerful than in the US. Choi and colleagues compared the use of celebrity in advertisements in South Korea and the USA (Choi et al. Citation2005). Celebrity was used much more in the former country, with celebrity appearing in 57% of 841 South Korean adverts (the samples were collected in 2002 and 2003). They also note that the tone of the adverts differs, with celebrities more likely to play themselves in the US and to act roles in South Korea. Money et al. (Citation2006) report Kilburn’s findings that in the late 1990s in Japan 70% of all advertisements, and 90% of all that were ‘likable, popular or memorable’, featured a celebrity. In contrast, in Choi et al.’s survey, just 9.3% of 2354 US commercials on the three major TV channels featured celebrities.

Clearly therefore the categorisation of ‘US’ and ‘non-US’, encompassing as it does celebrity-sceptical countries and celebrity-receptive countries, is an unsatisfactory one. Moreover, even those continental sub-divisions are unsatisfactory. I make them because the knowledge base beyond the US is thin, not because they are inherently reasonable. Rather than using crude regional generalisations, theories of the power of celebrity advertising will need to take into account different national celebrity cultures and different advertising regimes, as the other papers in this volume demonstrate. Work such as that by Choi et al. (Citation2005), which is deliberately and systematically building up an understanding of advertising in South Korea, will be necessary in order to explore how responses to advertising vary within and between countries.

A clear research agenda therefore emerges from this analysis. We need to have nationally and regionally differentiated studies of the variable power of celebrity persuasions. We have an authoritative body of knowledge on the responses of US college students. Celebrity advertising research now needs to expand its horizons and examine how different audiences respond (or fail to respond) to celebrity endorsements in other countries, and particular demographics within those countries.

But my suggestion, in closing, is that in expanding their sample base, celebrity advertisement researchers also expand their methodological repertoire. Thus far researchers’ preference for using quantitative methods does not appear to be justified by the wealth of insights these methods afford. It is clear that the knowledge built up has not properly addressed the challenges of sampling upon which effective quantitative methods hinge. It would be possible for new research to address these problems and proceed as before, but simply in a broader variety of countries, and with fewer students in the US. With the methods currently employed, this work will ultimately produce a list of significant and insignificant results among different specified portions of different countries’ populations. This list could then be subject to further meta-analyses of the sort Amos and colleagues conducted, producing a refined and more significant set of results.

However, it is plain from the research conducted thus far that these methods will produce little more than such a list. While this might be a reasonably interesting resource for advertisers, it will not really explain how particular effects become significant (or not) through consumers’ consumption practices. Qualitative methods are particularly useful when addressing the meaning of consumption practices, which is likely to be central to any understanding of how celebrity works (Marshall Citation1997, Turner et al. Citation2000, Rojek Citation2001, Turner Citation2004, Ferris Citation2007). These will be obviously useful when exploring how celebrity works in contexts foreign to researchers, but also provide a good means of understanding societies with which we think we are familiar. In current marketing practice, the utility of more qualitative, and specifically anthropological and ethnographic, methods is increasingly well recognised (Anderson Citation2009, Jackson Citation2009, Radjou Citation2009, Reinan Citation2009).

These methods could explore the ways in which celebrity endorsements work among different groups – what sort of identity work they involve and how meaning is constructed between celebrity and consumer brands in the minds of different publics. They could explore the different consequences of direct and indirect endorsements. This would entail in-depth interviews, focus groups, diaries, q methodology and participant observation. Through such methods it would be possible to learn more about the differentiated consumption practices that different populations demonstrate in their following, and not following, celebrity endorsements.

If qualitative methods can be countenanced by the community of researchers exploring the impacts of celebrity advertising, then quite an exciting research programme could unfold. A mixed-methods approach that, based on representative samples, combines the predictive power of quantitative methods with the quality of insight that qualitative methods afford would be a powerful thing. Driven by such approaches, it might be possible for a representative and rigorous theory of celebrity advertising to emerge.

Conclusion

Research into the effectiveness of celebrity advertising has largely been based on US students. The samples assembled are, collectively, not representative of the US population as a whole. Nor are they representative of other parts of the world. Theories and models of celebrity advertising which are built upon the collective wisdom of the research undertaken thus far can only safely be applied to a relatively small proportion of consumers.

A weighty body of knowledge has built up based on these samples, and apparently in ignorance of the problems in the sample base. There is evidence to suggest that these flaws have not been noticed before. Amos and colleagues discussed the different responses of these groups, but not their consequences for the samples collected. It is particularly notable that the total dominance of US research until the late 1990s excited no comment. Indeed, current citation practices still accentuate the bias towards US-based samples. It is quite difficult, even for diligent students, to learn about how celebrity advertising may work in other parts of the world or other populations. The bias of earlier work is likely to have an enduring effect unless it is explicitly recognised.

This means that theories of celebrity advertising have a much more limited scope than has previously been realised. It does not mean that they are wrong, or that their scope cannot get broader. The far greater prevalence of celebrity advertising in some Asian countries could mean that US students, who are more attentive to celebrity than their compatriots, may make a good empirical base from which to learn about other population groups. My point is simply that current theories have been tested in a limited sector of the market, and have to be extrapolated with care.

More insight into the power of celebrity advertising internationally therefore will require more research in other countries. Current research cannot be safely applied there. My plea, however, is that such work should undertake more qualitative research into the meaning of consumption practices which celebrity enables or stymies. Even when applied to the US student populations of which they are most representative, some of the current conclusions are not particularly surprising. I am not convinced that advertisers needed to be told that credible, attractive and expert celebrities are effective endorsers. Understanding which groups of the population are most swayed by such endorsers in other countries would have only limited value. Exploring the richer ways in which consumers interact with celebrity through qualitative research and mixed methods may provide more useful insights.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author.

Additional information

Funding

Funding for this research was generously provided by the Economic and Social Research Council [RES 070-27-0035].

Notes on contributors

Dan Brockington

Dan Brockington is a Professor of Conservation and Development at the University of Manchester. Trained as an anthropologist in London, he has worked previously in the geography departments of the Universities of Oxford and Cambridge. His research has covered the social impacts of conservation policy, the relationships between capitalism and conservation, the work of media and celebrity in development, and long-term livelihood change in East Africa. He has conducted most of his research in Tanzania but has also worked in South Africa, Australia, New Zealand and India, as well as conducting global overviews of the social impacts of protected areas, media and conservation and continental wide examinations of the work of conservation NGOs in sub-Saharan Africa. Dan is happiest conducting long-term field research in remote areas but also learns much from studying plush fundraising events. He has recently published Celebrity Advocacy and International Development (2014) and has also written Celebrity and the Environment (2009), Nature Unbound (2008) (with Rosaleen Duffy and Jim Igoe) and Fortress Conservation (2002), and co-edited (with Rosaleen Duffy) Capitalism and Conservation (2011).

References

  • Agrawal, J. and Kamakura, W.A., 1995. The economic worth of celebrity endorsers: an event study analysis. The journal of marketing, 59 (3), 56–62.
  • Amos, C., Holmes, G., and Strutton, D., 2008. Exploring the relationship between celebrity endorser effects and advertising effectiveness. International journal of advertising research, 27 (2), 209–234.
  • Anderson, K., 2009. Ethnographic research: a key to strategy. Available from: http://hbr.harvardbusiness.org/2009/03/ethnographic-research-a-key-to-strategy/ar/1 [ Accessed 12 november 2012].
  • *Atkin, C. and Block, M., 1983. Effectiveness of celebrity endorsers. Journal of advertising research, 23 (1), 57–61.
  • *Austin, E.W., et al., 2008. Celebrity endorsements and their potential to motivate young voters. Mass communication and society, 11 (4), 420–436.
  • *Basil, M.D., 1996. Identification as a mediator of celebrity effects. Journal of broadcasting & electronic media, 40, 478–495.
  • *Basil, M.D. and Brown, W., 1997. Marketing AIDS prevention: the differential impact hypothesis versus identification effects. Journal of consumer psychology, 6 (4), 389–411.
  • *Batra, R. and Homer, P.M., 2004. The situational impact of brand image beliefs. Journal of consumer psychology, 14 (3), 318–330.
  • *Becker, A.B., 2012. Engaging celebrity? Measuring the impact of issue-advocacy messages on situational involvement, complacency and apathy. Celebrity studies, 3 (2), 213–231.
  • Beer, D. and Penfold-Mounce, R., 2010. Researching glossy topics: the case of the academic study of celebrity. Celebrity studies, 1 (3), 360–365.
  • *Biswas, D., Biswas, A., and Das, N., 2006. The differential effects of celebrity and expert endorsements on consumer risk perceptions. The role of consumer knowledge, perceived congruency, and product technology orientation. Journal of advertising, 35 (2), 17–31.
  • Brockington, D., 2014. Celebrity advocacy and international development. Abingdon: Routledge.
  • Brockington, D. and Henson, S., 2014. Signifying the public: celebrity advocacy and post-democratic politics. International journal of cultural studies, online version.
  • *Bush, A.J., Martin, C.A., and Bush, V.D., 2004. Sports celebrity influence on the behavioral intentions of Generation Y. Journal of advertising research, 44 (1), 108–118.
  • *Chan, K. and Prendergast, G.P., 2008. Social comparison, imitation of celebrity models and materialism among Chinese youth. International journal of advertising, 27 (5), 799–826.
  • *Chao, P., Wührer, G., and Werani, T., 2005. Celebrity and foreign brand name as moderators of country-of-origin effects. International journal of advertising, 24 (2), 173–192.
  • Choi, S.M., Lee, W.-N., and Kim, H.-J., 2005. Lessons from the rich and famous: a cross-cultural comparison of celebrity endorsement in advertising. Journal of advertising, 34 (2), 85–98.
  • *Choi, S.M. and Rifon, N.J., 2007. Who is the celebrity in advertising? Understanding dimensions of celebrity images? The journal of popular culture, 40 (2), 304–324.
  • *Choi, S.M. and Rifon, N.J., 2012. It is a match: the impact of congruence between celebrity image and consumer ideal self on endorsement effectiveness. Psychology & marketing, 29 (9), 639–650.
  • *Cronley, M.L., et al., 1999. Endorsing products for the money: the role of the correspondence bias in celebrity advertising. Advances in consumer research, 26 (1), 627–631.
  • *DeSarbo, W.S. and Harshman, R.,A., 1985. Celebrity and brand congruence analysis. Current issues and research in advertising, 8, 17–52.
  • *Edwards, S.M. and La Ferle, C., 2009. Does gender impact the perception of negative information related to celebrity endorsers? Journal of promotion management, 15 (1–2), 22–35.
  • *Eisend, M. and Langner, T., 2010. Immediate and delayed advertising effects of celebrity endorsers’ attractiveness and expertise. International journal of advertising, 29 (4), 527–546.
  • Erdogan, B.Z., 1999. Celebrity endorsement: a literature review. Journal of marketing management, 15 (4), 291–314.
  • *Erdogan, B.Z., Baker, M.J., and Tagg, S., 2001. Selecting celebrity endorsers: the practitioners’ perspective. Journal of advertising research, 41 (3), 39–49.
  • Erfgen, C., 2011. Impact of celebrity endorsement on brand image: a communication process perspective on 30 years of empirical research, University of Hamburg. Research Papers on Marketing and Retailing.
  • Ferris, K.O., 2007. The sociology of celebrity. Sociology compass, 1 (1), 371–384.
  • *Fireworker, R.B. and Friedman, H.H., 1977. The effects of endorsements on product evaluation. Decision sciences, 8, 576–583.
  • *Forehand, M.R. and Perkins, A., 2005. Implicit assimilation and explicit contrast: a set/reset model of response to celebrity voice‐overs. Journal of consumer research, 32 (3), 435–441.
  • *Freiden, J.B., 1982. An evaluation of spokesperson and vehicle source effects in advertising. Current issues & research in advertising, 5 (1), 77–86.
  • *Friedman, H.H. and Friedman, L., 1979. Endorser effectiveness by product type. Journal of advertising research, 19, 63–71.
  • *Friedman, H.H., Santeramo, M.J., and Traina, A., 1978. Correlates of trustworthiness for celebrities. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 6 (4), 291–299.
  • *Friedman, H.H., Termini, S., and Washington, R., 1976. The effectiveness of advertisements utilizing four types of endorsers. Journal of advertising, 5 (3), 22–24.
  • *Gail, T., et al., 1992. The use of created versus celebrity spokesperson in advertisements. The journal of consumer marketing, 9 (4), 45–51.
  • *Goldsmith, R.E., Lafferty, B.A., and Newell, S.J., 2000. The impact of corporate credibility and celebrity credibility on consumer reaction to advertisements and brands. Journal of advertising, 29 (3), 43–54.
  • Harrison, R.L. and Reilly, T.M., 2011. Mixed methods designs in marketing research. Qualitative market research: an international journal, 14 (1), 7–26.
  • *Heath, T.B., McCarthy, M.S., and Mothersbaugh, D.L., 1994. Spokesperson fame and vividness effects in the context of issue-relevant thinking: the moderating role of competitive setting. Journal of consumer research, 20 (4), 520–534.
  • *Hovland, C.I. and Weiss, W., 1951. The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public opinion quarterly, 15 (4), 635–650.
  • *Hung, K., Chan, K.W., and Tse, C.H., 2011. Assessing celebrity endorsement effects in China: a consumer-celebrity relational approach. Journal of advertising research, 51 (4), 608–623.
  • *Jackson, D.J., 2008. Selling politics. Journal of political marketing, 6 (4), 67–83.
  • *Jackson, D.J. and Darrow, T.I.A., 2005. The influence of celebrity endorsements on young adults’ political opinions. The Harvard journal of press/politics, 10 (3), 80–98.
  • Jackson, S., 2009. Breakthrough market discoveries often hide out in latent form. Available from: http://www.examiner.com/x-17952-DC-Business-Commentary-Examiner~y2009m8d6-Breakthrough-market-discoveries-often-hide-out-in-latent-form [ Accessed 12 November 2012].
  • *Kahle, L.R. and Homer, P.M., 1985. Physical attractiveness of the celebrity endorser: a social adaptation perspective. Journal of consumer research, 11 (4), 954–961.
  • *Kamins, M.A., 1989. Celebrity and non-celebrity advertising in a two sided context. Journal of advertising research, 29 (3), 34–42.
  • *Kamins, M.A., 1990. An investigation into the “match-up” hypothesis in celebrity advertising: when beauty may be only skin deep. Journal of advertising, 19 (1), 4–13.
  • Kamins, M.A., et al., 1989. Two-sided versus one-sided celebrity endorsements: the impact on advertising effectiveness and credibility. Journal of advertising, 18 (2), 4–10.
  • *Kamins, M.A. and Gupta, K., 1994. Congruence between spokesperson and product type: a matchup hypothesis perspective. Psychology and marketing, 11 (6), 569–586.
  • *Klaus, N. and Bailey, A.A., 2008. Celebrity endorsements: an examination of gender and consumers’ attitudes. American journal of business, 23 (2), 53–62.
  • *Klebba, J.M. and Unger, L.S., 1982. The impact of negative and positive information on source credibility in a field setting. Advances in consumer research, 10, 11–16.
  • *Koernig, S.K. and Boyd, T.C., 2009. To catch a tiger or let him go: the match-up effect and athlete endorsers for sport and non-sport brands. Sport marketing quarterly, 18, 25–37.
  • *La Ferle, C. and Choi, S.M., 2005. The importance of perceived endorser credibility in South Korean advertising. Journal of current issues & research in advertising, 27 (2), 67–81.
  • *Lafferty, B.A. and Goldsmith, R.E., 1999. Corporate credibility’s role in consumers’ attitudes and purchase intentions when a high versus a low credibility endorser is used in the Ad. Journal of business research, 44, 109–116.
  • *Lafferty, B.A., Goldsmith, R.E., and Newell, S.J., 2002. The dual credibility model: the influence of corporate and endorser credibility on attitudes and purchase intentions. Journal of marketing theory and practice, 10 (3), 1–12.
  • *Langmeyer, L. and Walker, M., 1991. A first step to identigy the meaning in celebrity endorsers. Advances in consumer research, 18, 364–371.
  • *Lee, J.-G. and Thorson, E., 2008. The impact of celebrity-product incongruence on the effectiveness of product endorsement. Journal of advertising research, 48 (3), 433–449.
  • *Louie, T.A. and Obermiller, C., 2002. Consumer response to a firm’s endorser (dis)association decisions. Journal of advertising, 31 (4), 41–52.
  • *Lynch, J. and Schuler, D., 1994. The matchup effect of spokesperon and product congruence: a schema theory interpretation. Psychology and marketing, 11 (5), 417–445.
  • Marshall, D.P., 1997. Celebrity and power. Fame in contemporary culture. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  • *Marshall, R., et al., 2008. Endorsement theory: how consumers relate to celebrity models. Journal of advertising research, 48 (4), 564–572.
  • *Martin, B.A.S., Wentzel, D., and Tomczak, T., 2008. Effects of susceptibility to normative influence and type of testimonial on attitudes toward print advertising. Journal of advertising, 37 (1), 29–43.
  • Mathur, L.K., Mathur, I., and Rangan, N., 1997. The wealth effects associated with a celebrity endorser: the Michael Jordan phenomenon. Journal of advertising research, 37 (3), 67–73.
  • McCracken, G., 1989. Who is the celebrity endorser? Cultural foundations of the endorsement process. Journal of consumer research, 16, 310–321.
  • *Miller, F.M. and Allen, C.T., 2012. How does celebrity meaning transfer? Investigating the process of meaning transfer with celebrity affiliates and mature brands. Journal of consumer psychology, 22, 443–452.
  • *Misra, S. and Beatty, S.E., 1990. Celebrity spokesperson and brand congruence. Journal of business research, 21, 159–173.
  • *Money, R.B., Shimp, T.A., and Sakano, T., 2006. Celebrity endorsements in Japan and the United States: is negative information all that harmful? Journal of advertising research, 46 (1), 113–123.
  • *Moore, D.J., Mowen, J.C., and Reardon, R., 1994. Multiple sources in advertising appeals: when product endorsers are paid by the advertising sponsor. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 22 (3), 234–243.
  • *Mowen, J.C. and Brown, S., 1981. On explaining and predicting the effectiveness of celebrity endorsers. Advances in consumer research, 8, 437–441.
  • *Nataraajan, R. and Chawla, S.K., 1997. “Fitness” marketing: celebrity or non-celebrity endorsement? Journal of professional services marketing, 15 (2), 119–129.
  • *Newman, G.E., Diesendruck, G., and Bloom, P., 2011. Celebrity contagion and the value of objects. Journal of consumer research, 38 (2), 215–228.
  • *Nownes, A.J., 2011. An experiemental investigation of the effects of celebrity support for political parties in the United States. American politics research, 40 (3), 476–500.
  • *O’Mahony, S. and Meenaghan, T., 1997/8. The impact of celebrity endorsements on consumers. Irish marketing review, 10 (2), 15–24.
  • *Ohanian, R., 1990. Construction and validation of a scale to measure celebrity endorsers’ perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. Journal of advertising, 19 (3), 39–52.
  • *Ohanian, R., 1991. The impact of celebrity spokespersons’ perceived image on consumers’ intention to purchase. Journal of advertising research, FebruaryMarch, 46–54.
  • *Pornpitakpan, C., 2003. The effect of celebrity endorsers’ perceived credibility on product purchase intention: the case of Singaporeans. Journal of international consumer marketing, 16 (2), 55–73.
  • *Priester, J. and Petty, R.E., 2003. The influence of spokesperson trustworthiness on message elaboration, attitude strength, and advertising effectiveness. Journal of consumer psychology, 13 (4), 408–421.
  • Pringle, H., 2004. Celebrity sells. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
  • *Pughazhendi, A., Thirunavukkarasu, R., and Susendiran, S., 2011. A study on celebrity based advertisements on the purchase attitude of consumers towards durable products in Coimbatore city, Tamil Nadu, India. Far East journal of marketing and management, 1 (1), 16–27.
  • Radjou, N., 2009. R&D 2.0: Fewer engineers, more Anthropologists. Available from: http://blogs.hbr.org/radjou/2009/06/rd-20-fewer-engineers-more-ant.html [ Accessed 12 November 2012].
  • *Ranjbarian, B., Shekarchizade, Z., and Momeni, Z., 2010. Celebrity endorser influence on attitude toward advertisements and brands. European journal of social sciences, 13 (3), 399–407.
  • Reinan, J., 2009. Olson’s anthropologists bring meaning to marketing. Available from: http://www.minnpost.com/johnreinan/2009/04/13/8015/olsons_anthropologists_bring_meaning_to_marketing [ Accessed 12 November 2012]
  • *Rice, D.H., Kelting, K., and Lutz, R.J., 2012. Multiple endorsers and multiple endorsements: the influence of message repetition, source congruence and involvement on brand attitudes. Journal of consumer psychology, 22, 249–259.
  • Rojek, C., 2001. Celebrity. London: Reaktion Books.
  • *Rossiter, J.R. and Smidts, A., 2012. Print advertising: celebrity presenters. Journal of business research, 65, 874–879.
  • *Saleem, F., 2007. Effect of single celebrity and multiple celebrity endorsement on low involvement and high involvement product advertisements. European journal of social sciences, 5 (3), 125–132.
  • *Samman, E., Auliffe, E.M., and MacLachlan, M., 2009. The role of celebrity in endorsing poverty reduction through international aid. International journal of nonprofit and voluntary sector marketing, 14, 137–148.
  • *Siemens, J.C., et al., 2008. Product expertise versus professional expertise: congruency between an endorser’s chosen profession and the endorsed product. Journal of targeting, measurement and analysis for marketing, 16 (3), 159–168.
  • *Silvera, D.H. and Austad, B., 2003. Factors predicting the effectiveness of celebrity endorsement advertisements. European journal of marketing, 38 (11/12), 1509–1526.
  • *Stafford, M.R., Stafford, T.F., and Day, E., 2002. A contingency approach: the effects of spokesperson type and service type on service advertising perceptions. Journal of advertising, 31 (2), 17–35.
  • *Sternthal, B., Dholaika, R., and Leavitt, C., 1978. The persuasive effect of source credibility: test of cognitive response. Journal of consumer research, 4 (4), 252–260.
  • Tantiseneepong, N., Gorton, M., and White, J., 2012. Evaluating responses to celebrity endorsements using projective techniques. Qualitative market research: an international journal, 15 (1), 57–69.
  • *Till, B. and Shimp, T.A., 1998. Endorsers in advertising: the case of negative celebrity information. Journal of advertising, 27 (1), 67–82.
  • *Till, B.D. and Busler, M., 1998. Matching products with endorsers: attractiveness versus expertise. Journal of consumer marketing, 15 (6), 576–586.
  • *Till, B.D. and Busler, M., 2000. The match-up hypothesis: physical attractiveness, expertise, and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs. Journal of advertising, 29 (3), 1–13.
  • *Till, B.D., Stanley, S.M., and Priluck, R., 2008. Classical conditioning and celebrity endorsers: an examination of belongingness and resistance to extinction. Psychology and marketing, 25 (2), 179–196.
  • *Tripp, C., Jensen, T.,.D., and Carlson, L., 1994. The effects of multiple product endorsements by celebrities on consumers’ attitudes and intentions. Journal of consumer research, 20 (4), 535–547.
  • Turner, G., 2004. Unsderstanding celebrity. London: Sage.
  • Turner, G., Bonner, F., and Marshall, D.P., 2000. Fame games. The production of celebrity in Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • *Van der Waldt, D.L.R., Schleritzko, N.E.A., and Van Zyl, K., 2007. Paid versus unpaid celebrity endorsement in advertising: an exploration. African journal of business management, 1 (7), 185–191.
  • *Veer, E., Becirovic, I., and Martin, B.A.S., 2010. If Kate voted conservative, would you? The role of celebrity endorsements in political party advertising. European journal of marketing, 44 (3/4), 436–450.
  • Wheeler, R.T., 2009. Nonprofit advertising: impact of celebrity connection, involvement and gender on source credibility and intention to volunteer time or donate money. Journal of nonprofit & public sector marketing, 21, 80–107.
  • *White, D.W., Goddard, L., and Wilbur, N., 2009. The effects of negative information transference in the celebrity endorsement relationship. International journal of retail and distribution management, 37 (4), 322–335.
  • *Wood, N.T. and Herbst, K.C., 2007. Political star power and political parties: does celebrity endorsement win first-time votes? Journal of political marketing, 6 (2–3), 141–158.
  • *Zahaf, M. and Anderson, J., 2008. Causality effects between celebrity endorsement and the intentions to buy. Innovative marketing, 4 (4), 57–65.