632
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Design spaces and EEG frequency band power in constrained and open design

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 193-221 | Received 26 Feb 2021, Accepted 28 Feb 2022, Published online: 18 Apr 2022

ABSTRACT

Design space is a common abstraction used in the investigation of design cognition. Characteristic properties of design spaces and how they change are underexplored. Design spaces can vary with the design task and its constraints, which are assumed to result in differences in neurocognitive processes. We review general cognition, creative cognition and design neurocognition EEG studies. We analyzed the brain activity of 32 professional mechanical engineers and industrial designers while performing constrained and open design tasks. The neurophysiological activations during reading the task, earliest reaction, and open externalization stages of constrained and open design are compared based on EEG frequency band power. Significant differences between constrained and open design for the beta bands were found in the earliest reaction stage. Significant differences between constrained and open design for alpha 2 and the beta bands were found in the open externalization stage. We discuss the results and relate the higher brain activity and significant differences in open design to cognitive functions of interest to design cognition. We show that EEG brain activation is sensitive to the level of constraints in designing, in particular alpha 2 and beta bands can act as proxies of the change and expansion of design spaces.

1. Introduction

Constrained and open design tasks are often used in experiments on the basis that they evoke different design behaviors framing distinct design spaces. In this paper, we test this claim by using electroencephalography technique (EEG) measurements to explore frequency band power associated with brain activation of professional designers, while designing constrained and open design tasks, and use these measurements as a proxy for constrained and open design spaces.

The notion of design spaces has its origin in the formation of the problem space and has been a subject of investigation and debate for the last 60 years. In the problem space theory of problem-solving (Newell & Simon, Citation1972), new constraints, subgoals, and design alternatives evoked from long-term memory leading to shifts in external memory representations, such as models and drawings, would be considered as changes of the problem space. The problem solver retrieval system (General Problem Solver, Newell et al., Citation1959), whether a human or computer, would continually modify and characterize the problem space while searching for solutions. The use of methods and techniques available for tackling ill-structured problems (Simon, Citation1973) would vary within the extent of the designer’s limited capacities and according to the problem’s goals, constraints, and generated alternatives. An alternative approach to problem-solving later emerged as a reflective practice (Schön, Citation1983, Citation1987). The designer, by thinking and doing (knowing in action), would construct the design world and set the dimensions of the problem space and the moves by which he/she would attempt to find solutions (Argyris et al., Citation1985; Schön, Citation1992). The situated cognition research approach (Clancey, Citation1997) elaborated the idea that learning takes place when an individual is doing something. The term situated emphasized that perceptual mechanisms causally relate human cognition to the environment and action. Being situated involved a causal coupling in the moment within internal organizing and between internal and external organizing, while changing things in the world. As a research approach, situated cognition was suitable to investigate design cognition (Gero, Citation1990). Design, seen as a temporal activity that generates appropriate solutions to situated and open requests framing the designer’s mental space, would require constructs such as problem finding (Simon, Citation1995) before problem-solving takes place (Runco, Citation1994; Runco & Nemiro, Citation1994). In the last 40 years, alternative views to the problem-solving space emerged with the focus on the ultimate purpose for change, the solution space.

1.1. Design Spaces

The notion of design space, where designers explore an abstract space of possibilities, has been a useful abstraction in understanding designing (Amstel et al., Citation2016; MacLean et al., Citation1991). The problem-solving view of design claims that the designing process commences with an exploration within the problem space (Goel, Citation1994; Goel & Pirolli, Citation1992; Goldschmidt, Citation1997), while others claim that designing commences by generating the solution space (Dorst, Citation2019; Dorst & Cross, Citation2001; Gero, Citation1990; Gero & Kumar, Citation1993; Kruger & Cross, Citation2006; Visser, Citation2009; Yoshikawa, Citation1981). Both views have been used in design cognition studies based on methods such as protocol analysis (Goldschmidt, Citation2014; Kan & Gero, Citation2017). Perspectives on the dynamics of designing and how it unfolds such as co-evolution between the problem space and the solution space (Dorst, Citation2019; Dorst & Cross, Citation2001; Maher & Poon, Citation1996) have been another view consistent with the notion of design as situated cognition (Gero & Kannengiesser, Citation2004).

Another view is the notion that the design space can be constrained or open, depending on the design request’s level of constraint and openness to exploration and that is the focus of the research reported in this paper. While a constrained design space is usually confined by specific requirements, an open design space expands by the introduction of new design variables leading to solutions, which might not have been possible earlier. This can occur where constraints are in conflict and hence there are no feasible solutions, and a better design is desired or when the designer introduces new variables (Vieira, Citation2012). The design space expands, or a new solution space emerges (Gero & Kumar, Citation1993; Biskjaer & Halskov, Citation2013).

The problem-solving space view was shown to be incomplete with Schön’s work (Schön, Citation1983) and later when in creativity research problem finding was identified as an important component of creative performances, and distinct from problem-solving (Abdulla et al., Citation2020; Runco, Citation1994). Problem finding was considered related to skills such as problem identification, problem definition, and also problem expression, problem construction, problem generation, and eventually problem discovery (Runco & Nemiro, Citation1994). Similar characteristics were identified in protocol studies of design and non-design problem spaces, such as problem finding and problem forming (Simon, Citation1995) and problem structuring (Goel, Citation1994). In the last three decades, other constructs with a focus on the solution, concept, and ideation were proposed, but the modeling of design spaces has received less attention.

These views have helped thinking about one of the central design research questions, when and whether designing, as a cognitive process, is distinct from problem-solving (Goel & Pirolli, Citation1989, Citation1992; Vieira, Citation2021; Visser, Citation2009). Distinctive brain activity between design tasks, based on problem-solving and layout design (Alexiou et al., Citation2009; 2010; Vieira et al., Citation2020a) and problem-solving and open design (Vieira et al., 2019; Vieira et al., Citation2020b, Citation2020a) have provided preliminary answers to this core design research question.

As part of a larger experiment, this study integrates a research project with the goal to distinguish designing from problem-solving across design domains. Studies have so far provided results for domains of expertise (Vieira et al., Citation2019b, Citation2020b), degree of professional experience (Vieira et al., Citation2020c), and gender effect (Vieira et al., Citation2021, Vieira, Benedek, et al., Citation2022). Here, by comparing two prototypical tasks, we extend our motivation and investigate whether and how the neurophysiological activation of professional designers while designing in constrained and open design tasks (Vieira et al., Citation2020b) translate into frequency band power and may be used as measures of the change of design spaces. One of our aims and motivation of the study is to stimulate discussion about the creation and development of a novel index to characterize design spaces, the Design Spaces Index (DSI). EEG measures can constitute not only indices (Shan et al., 2019), in addition to other task-related and design-related measures of individual performance, but also teamwork (Perišić et al., Citation2021). Temporal analysis of networks based on frequency bands and networks of task-related and design-related measures can constitute such indices measuring the dynamics of design spaces. The DSI would not just give feedback on design spaces expansion but also on its pliability. By pliability, we mean not just the change, expansion, or contraction but also the flexibility of the design space. Such indices can be used as metrics for pattern recognition methods in real-time feedback mechanisms (e.g., indoor environment, Shan et al., 2019). The ultimate motivation for the creation of a DSI feedback tool is the possibility to inform learning and education models, to enhance design creativity and innovation processes, and to support design management and design research.

1.2. Neuroscience of Creative Cognition and Design Neurocognition

Research on brain activity has been developed in the fields of cognitive science and neuroscience. In the last 50 years, cognitive science (Boden, Citation2006; Collins, Citation1977) has contributed to the understanding of memory, emotional and cognitive processing, attentional demands, associative mechanisms, inhibition, and cognitive control of general cognition. Creative cognition (Finke et al., Citation1992; Smith et al., Citation1995) has been investigated in several fields of science (i.e., Psychology, Cognitive Science, Design, and Neuroscience) for the last 30 years with relevant developments in understanding the underlying role of executive functions, memory, attention, and cognitive control of creative performance (Benedek & Fink, Citation2019; Benedek et al., Citation2014a; Fink et al., Citation2007). Design cognition (Akin, Citation2001; Cross, Citation2001; Eastman, Citation1970; Eastman, Citation2001; Hay et al., Citation2020; Lloyd et al., Citation1996; Oxman, Citation2001) has been investigated based on macro perspectives by distinguishing phases or stages of designing from grounded theory approaches or theoretical models (Hatchuel & Weil, Citation2009; Kan & Gero, Citation2017; Kannengiesser & Gero, Citation2019) mostly based on protocol analysis, for the last 50 years.

While general cognition requires the ability to adjust modes of thought to match the demands of each situation (Gabora, Citation2002), creative cognition encompasses special combinations and patterns of the same cognitive processes seen in other non-creative endeavors (Finke et al., Citation1992). Research in each area has followed different paths. Although creativity and design are drivers of innovation and social and cultural progress, crucial for economic sustainability and well-being, they are not identical. Design, in its broad definition, is the generation of a plan for a change, and as such, it is high-level cognition involving multimodal behavior (Park & Alderman, Citation2018). Creativity is defined as the ability to generate novel and effective ideas (Runco & Jaeger, Citation2012), or artifacts that are new, surprising, and valuable (Boden, Citation2004). Creative thinking is traditionally associated with conceptual expansion implying a creative change in the approach to the request (Abraham, Citation2019; Abraham et al., Citation2012). The creative problem-solving process involves the integration of creativity and intelligence to tackle requests that ask for appropriate solutions (Benedek et al., Citation2018; Jaarsveld et al., Citation2015).

Design as a temporal pursuit involves complex activities of concurrent and entangled cognitive processes occurring while designing. Recent paradigm shifts in creativity assessment in neuroscience research highlight those neural responses of cognitive processes cannot be observed in isolation from other ongoing processes (Benedek et al., Citation2018). These ideas supported pairing neuroscience methods with behavioral paradigms during ecologically valid, real-world design tasks to improve the understanding of design cognition (Chrysikou & Gero, Citation2020; Goel, Citation2014; Vieira et al., Citation2020b) and design creativity (Goldschmidt, Citation2018; Gero, Citation2020). Design neural process studies emerged in the last two decades (Alexiou et al., Citation2009), originating from the field of design neurocognition (Gero, Citation2019; Vieira, Citation2018). Design neurocognition (Vieira et al., Citation2019a, Citation2019b), emerged as the field that provides the convergence of research methods from design cognition and neuroscience. Literature reviews (Borgianni & Maccioni, Citation2020; Gero & Milovanovic, Citation2020) have recently highlighted the research endeavor in this field. For the purpose of this paper, we focus on the literature using the electroencephalographic (EEG) technique for assessing brain activation in general cognition, creativity, and design research.

Neurocognitive creativity studies using EEG started more than 40 years ago, by investigating cortical activation during multiple creative tasks (Martindale & Hines, Citation1975), and differences as a function of creativity, stage of the creative process and originality (Martindale & Hasenfus, Citation1978). Other studies have compared the brain activation of experts and novices (Göker, Citation1997), a topic recently revisited from other perspectives (Liang, Chang & Liu, 2019; Vieira et al., Citation2020d). Investigations have focused either on single-domain studies (Liang et al., Citation2017; Liu et al., Citation2018, Citation2016; Nguyen & Zeng, Citation2010; Jia & Zeng, Citation2021; Vieira et al., Citation2019a, Citation2020e), domain comparisons between mechanical engineers and architects (Vieira et al., Citation2019b), and mechanical engineers and industrial designers (Vieira et al., Citation2020b), and gender (Vieira, Benedek, et al., Citation2022).

In the neuroscience of creative cognition, comprehensive literature reviews have focused on topics relevant to design research, such as visual creativity, visual mental imagery (Aziz-Zadeh et al., Citation2013; Pidgeon et al., Citation2016), gender, creative potential and cognitive strategy (Abraham, Citation2016; Baer & Kaufman, Citation2008), creativity assessment (Benedek et al., Citation2019), and brain networks (Beaty et al., Citation2016; Beaty & Kennett, Citation2020). Neural processes associated with general creativity have been widely investigated (Abraham, Citation2019; Abraham et al., Citation2012; Benedek & Fink, Citation2019; Benedek et al., Citation2018; Dietrich & Haider, Citation2017; Goel & Vartanian, Citation2005; Sawyer, Citation2011). We highlight results relevant to the understanding of constrained and open design spaces.

Studies using the EEG technique are usually based on the analysis of activation in specific frequency bands (Benedek & Fink, Citation2020; Stevens & Zabelina, Citation2019). About 20 years ago, the oscillatory neuroelectric activity of delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands was proposed to act as resonant communication networks through large populations of neurons, with functional relations to memory and integrative functions. Complex stimuli would elicit superimposed oscillations of different frequencies (Başar et al., Citation1999). Although neglected in the early 1990s, in the last few decades, most studies focused on the alpha frequency band. Higher alpha band activity is thought to be more sensitive to specific task-related requirements, while the lower alpha band activity is associated with attention processes such as vigilance and alertness (Klimesch, Citation1999). Increased alpha activity at prefrontal sites is considered to be an indication of the complex cognitive processes implicated in ill-defined problem spaces (Fink & Benedek, Citation2014; Fink et al., Citation2009), reflecting high internal attention demands (e.g., imagination) or the inhibition of task-irrelevant sensory processes (Benedek, Citation2018; Fink & Benedek, Citation2014). Increased alpha activity in temporal and occipital areas is associated with visualization processes (Jaarsveld et al., Citation2015). Relevant studies of the frequency bands of interest in the present study (theta, alpha, and beta waves) on general cognition, creative cognition, and design neurocognition from the last 35 years are summarized in .

Table 1. EEG findings in general cognition, creative cognition, and design neurocognition by frequency band.

In design research, frequency-specific brain activation has been used as a measurement tool to compare design activities (Liu, Zeng & Hamza, 2016), such as visual thinking spent during solution generation with solution evaluation (Liu et al., Citation2018). Higher alpha and beta frequency bands have been found to play a key role in open design tasks (Vieira et al., Citation2020a). Higher alpha power has been found to be associated with open-ended tasks and divergent thinking (Nguyen & Zeng, Citation2010) and visual association in expert designers (Liang et al., Citation2018), while higher theta and beta powers have been found to be associated with convergent thinking in decision-making and constraint tasks (Nguyen & Zeng, Citation2010). Higher beta power has been associated also with visual attention (Liang et al., Citation2018).

In this study, we look at the cognitive demands associated with constrained and open design tasks and how these translate into brain activation and specifically changes in frequency bands. These aspects are further described in the analysis of frequency bands, and we suggest comparing the results with selected cognitive functions associated with the respective Brodmann areas (BA) that can be inferred from the literature and have potential connections to design cognition in constrained and open design. Brodmann’s studies (1909) on the brain’s structure, function, and connectivity have been refined and correlated with various cortical functions and cognitive activities (Glasser et al., Citation2016). The electrode placement of the EEG device and their associated Brodmann area are shown in . The relation between cognitive functions and Brodmann areas is listed in .

Figure 1. (a) Electrodes placement related to each cortex of the brain and (b) corresponding Brodmann areas.

Figure 1. (a) Electrodes placement related to each cortex of the brain and (b) corresponding Brodmann areas.

Table 2. Brodmann areas (BA) and design relevant associated cognitive functions for each hemisphere.

Each Brodmann area is increasingly associated with more and more cognitive functions and most researchers are cautious about relating specific electrode positions with higher cognitive functions, although such associations are commonly used when discussing brain regions of main findings.

Through the comparison of frequency band power between the two prototypical tasks, we connected the results to the literature on associated cognitive functions and present an overview of hypothetical inferences and interpretation. These inferences are not intended to claim the presence of cognitive processes from observed brain activation (reverse, Poldrack, Citation2011; Poldrack, Citation2006). This would ask for other concurrent sources of data such as think-aloud design protocols and infer the cognitive processes from verbalizations. Instead, we highlight selected studies of cognitive functions associated with channels that reveal statistical differences in the present study and infer potential associations that relate to design cognition, in particular to these stages of design cognition that can inspire future design experiments.

1.3. Research Questions and Approach

We investigate constrained and open design spaces through the analysis of the neurophysiological activation differences of mechanical engineers and industrial designers, when addressing two prototypical tasks, a constrained problem-solving layout design task and an open sketching design task, in line with the studies in the right column of . We use frequency bands power from the constrained and open design tasks as proxies for constrained and open design spaces. The experiment design from which we report results here included four different tasks, as previously detailed in Vieira et al. (Citation2020b). Among the different tasks proposed to the subjects, for this study we selected those clearly presenting the traits of the intended comparison: a constrained layout design task and an open design task.

In the present paper, we divided the two tasks into three stages of categorical similarity: Stage 1, reading the task; Stage 2, earliest reaction; Stage 3, open externalization. Distinguishing the three stages is motivated by the assumptions that:

  1. designing starts by reading the task request, whether the request is constrained or open, and may evoke different levels of conceptual expansion, prompting designers to construct different design spaces.

  2. protocol analyses usually address only the third stage as they rely on the externalizations by the designer, and we use the potential of the EEG neurophysiological measurements to investigate what comes before the externalization of the idea and immediately after reading the task request.

  3. protocol studies have identified problem finding and problem forming (Simon, Citation1995) and problem structuring (Goel, Citation1994) as invariants of design problem spaces and how these differ from problem-solving in non-design problem spaces (Goel, Citation1994). We explore if such difference takes place between design spaces of constrained and open tasks, by examining brain activity.

The analysis focuses on the frequency bands power differences observed along the three different stages of the execution of the tasks. By temporally segmenting these activations for each participant, it is possible to distinguish brain activation within design sessions across the three stages. We use these activations from constrained and open design tasks as proxies for constrained and open design spaces. We investigate the following research questions:

  • What are the similarities and differences in frequency band power in constrained and open tasks associated with reading the task, earliest reaction, and open externalization?

  • What are the similarities and differences in frequency band power in constrained and open tasks associated with reading the task, earliest reaction, and open externalization between mechanical engineers and industrial designers?

2. Methods

The research questions are investigated by using the constrained design task as the reference task for the open design task. We analyze the frequency power (Pow) across distinct frequency bands. The tasks and experimental procedure were piloted prior to the full study, which produced changes resulting in the final experiment design (Vieira et al., Citation2020b).

2.1. Participants

Results are based on 32 right-handed participants, 15 mechanical engineers, aged 25–43 (M = 28.4, SD = 4.7), 10 men (age M = 29, SD = 5.5) and 5 women (age M = 27.2, SD = 2.7); and 17 industrial designers, aged 25–50 (M = 33.1, SD = 8.9), 9 men (age M = 35.7, SD = 8.6) and 8 women (age M = 30.4, SD = 8.8). The results of the unpaired t-test controlling for experience between cohorts revealed no statistically significant difference, t(30) = 1.8, p = .08. The participants are all professionals (years of experience M = 6.4, SD = 6.2). This study was approved by the local ethics committee of the University of Porto.

2.2. Experiment tasks

All participants completed two experimental tasks, a constrained and an open design task. The constrained task was adopted from the problem-solving layout design described in the Alexiou et al. (Citation2009) fMRI study. This task is considered a constrained problem-solving task as the problem itself is well-defined, and the set of solutions is unique (Alexiou et al., Citation2009). We designed a block experiment that consisted of a sequence of tasks previously reported (Vieira et al., Citation2020b). We added an open design task that included free-hand sketching. This task is an ill-defined and fully unconstrained task (see ).

Figure 2. Depiction and description of the constrained design task based on problem-solving and the open design sketching task.

Figure 2. Depiction and description of the constrained design task based on problem-solving and the open design sketching task.

In both tasks, the subject received the description of the assignment in the form of written text. Moreover, the interaction is based on the visualization of the layout or sketch and on the application of design modifications through manual actions on the furniture elements or hand-sketching. In turn, the most significant difference between the two tasks is the degree of openness. We explore the observable and measurable differences of the frequency bands power between the constrained and the open design task.

To understand brain activity in constrained and open design tasks, we took a macroperspective, by distinguishing three stages as follows:

  • Stage 1, Reading, is the stage in which the designers read the task request.

  • Stage 2, Earliest reaction, is the stage in which the designers take the earliest reaction after reading the request. In the constrained design task, it starts when the participant takes action to locate the three strict requests and ends when these are accomplished. This stage is labeled problem-solving. All the 32 participants immediately take this action after reading, by locating the three requests in one sequence. Two participants of each cohort ended the task here. In the open design task, a stage of reflecting is the earliest reaction after reading the request and ends when the participant initiates sketching or notation activities. All the 32 participants exhibited the stage of reflecting.

  • Stage 3, Open externalization, is the stage in which the designers have more freedom to externalize their design solutions. In the constrained design task, after the problem-solving stage, the participants freely complete the layout design. In the open design task, the open externalization stage starts with the beginning of sketching or notation activities and ends when the design is concluded. This stage is labeled sketching and was performed by all the participants.

By taking the problem-solving stage of the constrained task, we know that participants cognitive effort is focused on solving well-defined instructions. By narrowing the problem-solving stage of the task, we consider this a suitable segment for comparison with the other stages, in particular the stages of the open design task (Vieira, Benedek et al., Citation2022).

2.3. Setup and procedure

The setup and complete procedure have been previously described in Vieira et al. (Citation2020b). A brief outline is presented here. Electromagnetic interference of the room was checked including the 50 Hz power line contamination. One researcher was present in each experiment session to instruct the participants and to check for recording issues. A period of 10 minutes for setting up and a few minutes for a short introduction were necessary for informing each participant, reading and signing the consent agreement and setting the room temperature. The researcher followed a script to conduct the experiment so that each participant was presented with the same information and stimuli. The participants were asked to start by reading the task request, which took an average of 10 s. The participants were asked to stay silent during the task and use the breaks for clarifying questions. In the constrained design task, participants received a tangible interface based on magnetic material for easy handling. In the open design task, each participant was given two sheets of paper (A3 size) and three instruments, a pencil, graphite, and a pen (see ).

The average stage duration for the constrained design task was as follows: reading, M = 11.1 s (SD = 7.1); problem-solving, M = 30.5 s (SD = 17.8); and layout, M = 56.1 s (SD = 31.5) for the mechanical engineers and reading, M = 12.5 s (SD = 8.5); problem-solving, M = 30.2 s (SD = 13.1); and layout, M = 53.1 s (SD = 37.1) for the industrial designers.

The average stage duration for the open design task was as follows: reading, M = 8.2 s (SD = 3.6); reflecting, M = 51.6 s (SD = 67.8); and sketching, M = 522.7 s (SD = 290.0) for the mechanical engineers and reading, M = 7.6 s (SD = 2.9); reflecting, M = 54.7 s (SD = 53.1); and sketching, M = 519.1 s (SD = 266.6) for the industrial designers.

2.4. Equipment and data collection methods

EEG activity was recorded using a portable 14-channel system Emotiv Epoc+. Each of the Emotiv Epoc+ channels collects continuous signals of electrical activity at their location with a 256 Hz sampling rate. Fourteen electrodes were positioned according to the 10–10 I.S, .

Figure 3. Electrodes placement according to the 10–10 I.S in the brain cortex.

Figure 3. Electrodes placement according to the 10–10 I.S in the brain cortex.

The participants performed the tasks, with two video cameras capturing the participants’ face and activity. All the data captures were streamed using the Panopto software (https://www.panopto.com/). Sessions took place at the University of Porto, between March and July 2017 and June and September 2018 at Mouraria Creative Hub, Lisbon, during August 2018 between 9:00 and 15:00.

2.5. Data processing methods

The signal was band pass-filtered with a low cutoff of 3.5 Hz and a high cutoff of 28 Hz to maintain only oscillatory brain activity between the theta and beta frequency range. As both tasks involved motor activity, we applied methods to attenuate the muscle artifact contamination of the EEG recordings. We adopted the blind source separation (BSS) technique based on canonical correlation analysis (CCA) for the removal of muscle artifacts from EEG recordings (De Clercq et al., Citation2006; Vos et al., Citation2010) adapted to remove any short EMG bursts. Data processing included the removal of the Emotiv-specific DC offset with the Infinite Impulse Response (IIR) filter and BSS-CCA. The BSS-CCA procedure successfully filters most of the signal from artifacts. The data were visually checked for the remaining artifacts, and artifactual epochs caused by muscle tension, eye blinks or eye movements were excluded from further analysis. A z-score was conducted in parallel to this procedure and applied to each frequency band. The decomposition of the EEG signal followed the typical component frequency bands and their approximate spectral boundaries, theta (3.5–7 Hz), alpha 1 (7–10 Hz), alpha 2 (10–13 Hz), beta 1 (13–16 Hz), beta 2 (16–20 Hz), and beta 3 (20–28 Hz). By adopting lower and upper alpha boundaries, and beta sub-bands, we ensured that our findings can be related to the literature in other domains.

Data analysis included power values of frequency bands on individual and aggregate levels using MatLab and EEGLab open-source software. All the EEG segments of the recorded data were used for averaging throughout the segments corresponding to each of the stages in the analysis. We report on one measurement, the power (Pow) of each frequency band. The Pow was obtained by band-pass filtering the EEG signal at each electrode for specific frequency bands (see above) and computing the mean of the squared values of the resulting signal. This measure tells us about the amplitude of the frequency power per channel and per participant. After a z-score was conducted to determine outliers, the criteria for excluding participants were based on the evidence of 6 or more threshold z-score values above 1.96 or below −1.96 and individual measurements above 2.81 or below −2.81 for each stage of the two tasks and each frequency band. To avoid extreme outliers in the EEG data, only stages with activation periods of at least 2 s artifact-free EEG recording were used for statistical analyses. The valid EEG data corresponding to each stage of the constrained and open design tasks were averaged, respectively. The segmentation of each task in three stages followed a time-stamping procedure according to the criteria presented in the methods section and then computed in MatLAB. The divisions into Stage 1, reading the task, Stage 2, earliest reaction, and Stage 3, open externalization, were visually checked through the observation of the two videos captured per session. We present frequency bands Pow values on aggregates of the 32 participants’ individual results, for each stage of each task and cohort.

2.6. Statistical approach

We performed standard statistical analyses based on the design of the experiment: a mixed measure design (2x2x3x2x7) with domain (mechanical engineers, industrial designers) as the between-subjects factor and task (constrained design task and open design task), stage (reading, earliest reaction, open externalization), hemisphere (left, right), and electrode (O1/2, P7/8, T7/8, FC5/6, F7/8, F3/4, AF3/4) as within-subject factors. Analyses were performed for the dependent variable of Pow for each frequency band. The threshold for significance in all the analyses is p ≤ .05.

3. Analysis of results

The analysis of the transformed power (Pow) of each frequency band indicates that the constrained and open design tasks can be distinguished from each other. Results from running the 2x2x3x2x7 mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA are presented in .

Table 3. Significant main effects and interaction effects from the ANOVA (2x2x3x2x7) for each frequency band.

Significant main effects

From the analysis of the 32 participants, we found significant main effects and significant interaction effects between multiple factors (see ).

The main effect of task was found for four frequency bands. Industrial designers and mechanical engineers show significant differences for alpha 2 and the beta bands between the two tasks. Of particular interest for this study is the absence of a significant main effect of stage, and the interaction effect between task and stage for alpha and beta frequency bands. No differences were found between stages within the same task, but differences were found between tasks within the same stage.

No significant main effect was found for the between-subjects factor domain across the six frequency bands. Interaction effects with the between-subjects factor domain were found for alpha 1 and beta 3. Mechanical engineers show higher prefrontal alpha 1, and alpha 2 () in the constrained task, possibly due to dominant attentional processes (Klimesch, Citation1999; Klimesch et al., Citation1998). Industrial designers show higher prefrontal beta 3 () in the constrained task. These two significant interaction effects provide initial answers to the research question on differences and might be partly due to how these groups differ in expertise/experience and thus approach these tasks. While differences characterizing the abilities of mechanical engineers and industrial designers of relevance to perform these tasks can be discriminated, such as different uses of sketching and sketching abilities, the results reveal major similarities between these cohorts from the two domains. The almost complete absence of main effects and interaction effects may indicate prospective similarities or a lack of statistical power to detect those differences in the current study.

Stage and electrode are further investigated with detailed analyses. We perform electrode-based comparisons to check for differences by comparing the seven electrodes per task, stage, and hemisphere. Through the analysis of the electrode positions, we connect the results to the literature on selected cognitive functions identified in studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) (, of section 1.2), relevant to understanding design cognition and the change between the design spaces of the two tasks within and across domains.

3.1. Analysis of Stages between Constrained and Open Design across Domains

From the temporal analysis of the EEG full signal of the same tasks, significant differences were found for both domains between these two prototypical tasks (Vieira et al., Citation2020b). The present results augment the significant differences based on frequency band power and similar changes in brain activity for both cohorts. The statistical analyses of stages revealed significant differences between two of the three stages across domains. The pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences for: Stage 2, earliest reaction, between the two tasks corresponding to the problem-solving and reflecting stages, for the three beta bands; and Stage 3, open externalization, between the two tasks corresponding to the layout and sketching stages, for upper alpha and beta bands.

No significant differences were found between the reading stages of both tasks for the range of frequency bands. We further describe a detailed analysis of Stages 2 and 3 based on shared significant differences of frequency bands for both cohorts. This is followed by electrode-based comparisons and hypothetical associations to selected cognitive functions.

From the results on significant differences in the two stages between tasks for specific frequency bands, we infer that prioritizing some cognitive functions seems to play a role in mechanical engineers’ and industrial designers’ approaches to constrained and open design tasks. Hence, we connect the discussion of the results to the literature on selected cognitive functions associated with channels of statistical significance, relevant to understanding design cognition. These inferences, based on results from studies using fMRI and PET, should not be understood as absolute reverse inference (Poldrack, Citation2011, Citation2006), but exploratory and hypothetical inferences by selecting studies of cognitive functions related to these stages of design cognition. By doing so, we open possibilities for insights into hypotheses building, new studies, and experiments.

3.1.1. Stage 2 - Earliest Reaction: Problem-solving and Reflecting

This section focuses on the comparison of the constrained and open design tasks in the earliest reaction stage (i.e., problem-solving vs reflecting). Significant differences in beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands were observed between the problem-solving and reflecting stages for both domains, . The plots show the two hemispheres by distributing the electrodes (10–10 IS) symmetrically around a vertical axis. Pow scores per electrode (average of the entire stage) can be considered by comparing using the vertical scale. With the exception of channel AF4 for beta 2 of the mechanical engineers cohort, all channels of significant differences for beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 show higher activation in the reflecting stage of the open design task for both domains.

Beta 1

The analysis revealed common significant differences across the two domains for beta 1 in channels T8, mapped onto the right hemisphere, and channels P7 and F3, mapped onto the left hemisphere, . Potential cognitive functions associated with each channel, relevant for designing are presented. Channel T8 is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann Area (BA) 21, such as observation of motion (Rizzolatti et al., Citation1996), possibly related to designing as participants are about to sketch. Channel P7 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 37, such as semantic categorization (Gerlach et al., Citation2000) semantic relations (MacDermott et al., Citation2003), metaphor comprehension (Rapp et al., Citation2004), and deductive reasoning (Goel et al., Citation1998). Channel F3 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 08, such as inductive reasoning (Goel et al., Citation1997), planning (Crozier et al., Citation1999), and executive control (Kübler, Dixon & Garavan, Citation2006).

Figure 4. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of mechanical engineers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 4. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of mechanical engineers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 5. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of industrial designers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 5. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of industrial designers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Beta 2

The analysis also revealed common significant differences for beta 2, in channels AF4 and F4, mapped onto the right hemisphere, and channels O1, FC5, F7, and F3, mapped onto the left hemisphere, . Potential cognitive functions relevant for designing are associated with each channel. Channel AF4 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 09, namely planning (Fincham et al., Citation2002) and decision-making (Rogers et al., Citation1999). Channel F4 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 08, such as selective memory retrieval (Rugg et al., Citation1996), executive control (Kübler, Dixon & Garavan, Citation2006) and planning (Crozier et al., Citation1999). Channel O1 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 18, such as visual mental imagery (Platel et al., Citation1997) and visual word form (Vorobyev et al., Citation2004).

Figure 6. Channels of significant differences (p ≤ .05) between problem-solving and reflecting, in beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands that were observed for both domains.

Figure 6. Channels of significant differences (p ≤ .05) between problem-solving and reflecting, in beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands that were observed for both domains.

In the left hemisphere, channel FC5 is associated with the cognitive functions of Broca’s area BA 45, known for being involved in complex verbal functions and reasoning processes (Goel et al., Citation1997, Citation1998) and metaphor processing (Rapp et al., Citation2004). Channel F3, as previously described, and Channel F7 are associated with the cognitive functions of BA 47, in deductive reasoning and semantic processing (Goel et al., Citation1997).

Beta 3

The analysis also revealed significant differences for beta 3 in channels F4 and T8 mapped onto the right hemisphere and channels O1, P7, and F3 mapped onto the left hemisphere whose associated cognitive functions were previously mentioned, .

In Stage 2 of the open design task, the cognitive demand in the reflecting stage is translated in distinct brain activations from the problem-solving stage of the constrained design task. We hypothesize that the designers’ search relies to a certain extent on problem finding, distinct from problem-solving and an essential component of creative thinking (Simon, Citation1995; Abdulla et al., Citation2020; Runco & Nemiro, Citation1994). While reflecting in the open design space designers conceivably required other constructs, such as framing, defining, and generating the solution, concepts and ideation. In the last 30 years, useful cognitive constructs have been formalized in design research with focus not just on the problem, but also on the solution, likely to occur in reflecting stages, of high iterative recurrence and possibly extended to the next stage of open externalization.

3.1.2 Stage 3 - Open Externalization: Layout and Sketching

This section focuses on the comparison of the constrained and open design tasks in the open externalization stage (i.e., layout vs sketching). Significant differences in alpha 2 and beta frequency bands were observed between the layout and sketching stages for both domains, .

Figure 7. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of the mechanical engineers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 7. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of the mechanical engineers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 8. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of the industrial designers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

Figure 8. Transformed power (Pow) across channels of alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands of the industrial designers. The solid circles represent channels of significant differences between stage, task, and domain (p ≤ .05).

All channels with significant differences show higher activation in the open design task. The mechanical engineers reveal higher activation of alpha 2 in the prefrontal cortex in the layout stage and the industrial designers reveal higher activation of beta 3 in the right prefrontal cortex in the layout and sketching stages.

Alpha 2

The analysis revealed significant differences for alpha 2 in channels T8, P7, and F3, previously mentioned, channels FC6 and P8, mapped onto the right hemisphere, and channel FC5 mapped onto the left hemisphere, . Channel FC6 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 44 such as goal-intensive processing (Fincham et al., Citation2002) and search for originality (Nagornova, Citation2007).

Channel P8 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 37, such as monitoring shape (Le et al., Citation1998), visual fixation (Richter et al., Citation2004), and drawing (Harrington et al., Citation2007). Channel FC5, associated with the cognitive functions of Broca’s area BA 45, is known for being involved in complex verbal functions and reasoning processes (Goel et al., Citation1997, Citation1998) and metaphor processing (Rapp et al., Citation2004).

Figure 9. Channels of significant differences (p ≤ .05) between layout and sketching, in alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands that were observed for both domains.

Figure 9. Channels of significant differences (p ≤ .05) between layout and sketching, in alpha 2, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 frequency bands that were observed for both domains.

Beta Bands

The analysis revealed significant differences for beta 1 in channels F4, FC6, P8, P7, FC5, and F3 previously mentioned, mapped symmetrically in both hemispheres, .

The analysis revealed significant differences for beta 2 in channels FC6 and P8, mapped in the right hemispheres and channels P7, FC5, and F3 mapped in the left hemisphere, .

The analysis also revealed significant differences for beta 3 in channels, F4, FC6, P8, P7, and F3 previously mentioned and channel O2, . Channel O2 is associated with the cognitive functions of BA 18, such as visuospatial information processing (Waberski et al., Citation2008).

In Stage 3 of the open design task, the search for a creative and original solution in the sketching stage translates into distinct brain activations from the layout stage of the constrained task. We hypothesize that the distinct brain activations reflects designers’ search within the co-evolution of problem and solution in the sketching stage of the open design space (Dorst, Citation2019; Dorst & Cross, Citation2001; Maher & Poon, Citation1996).

5. Discussion

This controlled experiment provides evidence for the sensitivity of EEG brain activation for differences between constrained and open design tasks, carried out by two cohorts from the domains of mechanical engineering and industrial design. From the results of the analysis between the two tasks, we can infer the following similarities and differences in brain activation associated with the stages of reading, earliest reaction, and open externalization in constrained and open design tasks:

  1. No significant differences were found between the reading stages of both tasks for a range of frequency bands.

  2. Significant differences were found for Stage 2, earliest reaction, in the three beta bands between the two tasks. We infer that reflecting about the open design request evokes different levels of semantic associations, metaphor comprehension, deductive and inductive reasoning, observation of motion, planning, and executive control than in the problem-solving stage, prompting designers to change their design space, which results in increased activation in the open design task.

  3. Significant differences were found for Stage 3, open externalization, in alpha 2 and beta bands between the two tasks. We infer that design sketching evokes a more complex network of cognitive functions potentially related to visual mental imagery; search for originality; monitoring shape; goal-intensive processing; deductive, inductive, and complex reasoning; planning; and executive control than the layout stage in the constrained design task. We hypothesize that designers’ higher activations while freely producing creative and original solutions expand their design spaces.

Together, these findings highlight not only the sensitivity of EEG to the brain activity and potential cognitive functions associated with design-related tasks and stages but also its specificity as differences were only observed in more active designing stages (stages 2 and 3) but not during initial reading (stage 1). The results are partially consistent with the literature in design neurocognition studies. Beta 2 is found to play a role in searching for originality and goal-intensive processing in open design (Vieira et al., Citation2020a) and has been associated with design activities (Liu, Zeng & Hamza, 2016). Alpha 2 has been found to be associated with open tasks (Vieira et al., Citation2020a) and divergent thinking (Nguyen & Zeng, Citation2010) and visual association (Liang et al., Citation2018). While beta bands are found to play a role in reflecting and externalization in open design tasks, this is not consistent nor immediately plausible with results from the literature on beta power associated with convergent thinking in decision-making and constraints tasks (Nguyen & Zeng, Citation2010). Beta bands are related to active thinking, attention, or solving specific problems (Sanei & Chambers, Citation2007). Beta 1 associated functions in general and creative cognition are underexplored. Further studies are needed to disentangle the inconsistency of these results.

  • The absence of significant differences in upper alpha, in Stage 2, between problem-solving and reflecting stages for both domains is consistent with findings on decreased alpha in visual mental imagery in high- and low-creative groups (Volf et al., Citation2010). While other results show higher alpha in creative ideation (Fink & Benedek, Citation2014; Stevens & Zabelina, Citation2019), this seems to indicate that the different characteristics of the ideation process and stimuli in creative and design tasks translate into different brain activations of the upper alpha band.

  • The significant differences in upper alpha, in Stage 3, between the layout and sketching stages for both domains are consistent with findings of increased alpha in mental elaboration of drawings (Rominger et al., Citation2018). Higher alpha band activity is thought to be more sensitive to specific task-related requirements (Klimesch, Citation1999). The results on increased alpha in temporal and occipital areas and over prefrontal sites are associated with visualization processes and complex information processing, respectively (Jaarsveld et al., Citation2015). Increases in prefrontal and posterior alpha have been interpreted as reflecting high internal attention demands (e.g., imagination) or the inhibition of task-irrelevant sensory processes enfolding within ill-defined problem spaces (Benedek, Citation2018; Fink & Benedek, Citation2014).

  • From the significant differences in beta bands in Stage 2, between the problem-solving and reflecting stages for both domains, we infer that the unrestrictedly open design task requires other constructs, distinct from problem-solving, such as problem finding (Simon, Citation1995; Abdulla et al., Citation2020; Runco & Nemiro, Citation1994), problem framing, problem generation, problem definition (Runco & Nemiro, Citation1994), problem structuring (Goel, Citation1994) and solution focused constructs, as well (Dorst, Citation2019; Dorst & Cross, Citation2001; Gero, Citation1990; Gero & Kumar, Citation1993; Kruger & Cross, Citation2006; Visser, Citation2009; Yoshikawa, Citation1981) while reflecting. From the present study, we infer and therefore hypothesize that such constructs distinguish not just design from non-design problem spaces (Goel, Citation1994) but also constraints from open design tasks.

  • While these same constructs are expected to develop in Stage 3, the significant differences between the layout and sketching stages for both domains are indicated by higher brain activity for alpha 2 and beta bands in the open design task. This leads us to infer that the nature of the constraints, whether by specific requirements in the constrained design task or the absence of specific requirements in the open design task, prompted participants to engage additional and more complex networks of cognitive functions that translated into higher brain activity while sketching.

While in the past, protocol studies on design and non-design problem spaces revealed the distinction between problem structuring as a specific feature of design spaces (Goel, Citation1994) and distinct from problem-solving (Goel, Citation1994; Simon, Citation1973), the present results raise the possibility that a constrained design space shares characteristics with a non-design space (Goel, Citation1994). Problem structuring was defined as the process of drawing upon knowledge to compensate for missing information and using this knowledge to construct the problem space (Simon, Citation1973). Whether the request is constrained or open prompts the designer to adopt a formal problem-solving mode or a design thinking approach, through the use of other constructs involved in the co-evolution of problem and solution. The first reaction to a task request produced by a formal problem-solving approach is a characteristic of non-design spaces (Goel, Citation1994) and also of constrained design tasks. Instead, the design thinking approach to the open request results in higher brain activity for upper alpha and beta bands. This we take to imply that the design space of the open design task further expands.

6. Conclusion

Taking the approach that design creativity is associated with opening the space of possible designs, among other changes, this experiment has shown that EEG frequency band power may reflect the extent to which opening the design space of constrained and open design tasks may differ. The present results reveal significant differences based on frequency band power, and similar changes in brain activity for both cohorts that provide the basis for the development of indices constituted by EEG measures. Such EEG measures have the potential to constitute indices for pattern-recognition methods in real-time feedback tools, such as the Design Spaces Index. We asked participants to design for a highly constrained task which, we infer, results in a constrained design space and then to design for an open task which, we infer, results in a further expanded design space. Both tasks differently prompt ideational skills, self-expression, and creative potential. The results contribute to the knowledge of oscillatory brain responses across frequency bands for the brain/body–mind functional dynamics (Başar & Karakas, Citation2006) and to the knowledge about neurocognitive measurements in design.

7. Limitations of the research

The knowledge level of the participants and the variability of their EEG acquired signals are variables that we cannot fully control. Among the general limitations, we might also mention the potential influence of other uncontrolled differences between the two proposed tasks (e.g., between the motor actions for moving magnets vs. motor actions for sketching). Although mechanical engineers might not be so familiar with free-hand sketching, this does not seem to have an impact in the results across both cohorts.

The statistical approach we described, and the signal processing treatment reduced the potential effects on the results of the limitations of the EEG device. Due to the low spatial resolution of the EEG device used, the results cannot support strong claims related to location, as fields extend across the brain. By connecting the results to the literature on associated cognitive functions, we present an overview of exploratory and hypothetical inferences not intended to claim the presence of cognitive processes from brain activation (Poldrack, Citation2011; Poldrack, Citation2006), but to inspire hypothesis generation by selecting cognitive functions that relate to design cognition, in particular to each of the three stages considered in this study. These exploratory inferences can play a role in the early development of future and advanced work. To better identify unique brain regions associated with neural activity, a larger number of EEG channels are needed.

The sketching activities have been related to a specific area of the right occipitotemporal cortex (Harrington et al., Citation2007); however, its potential influence on brain activity needs further research (Raimo, Santangelo, et al., Citation2021). Higher activation of the magnetic field around this area revealed in subjects with planned sketching ability but also struggling with its representation asks for future disentanglement.

Since the comparison in this study involved six frequency bands, the significance level of the comparison should be reduced to control the family-wise false-positive rate. For instance, according to the Bonferroni method, significant results would be considered only if critical p (p ≤ .05) is divided by 6 (frequency bands) = > p < .0083. In such a perspective, only highly significant results should be considered as consolidated, while the results on alpha 2 and some of the results for beta 1 and beta 3 need more caution and should be explored in larger data sets.

8. Future work

The present results allowed the exploration of the neurophysiological activation across frequency bands as proxies for assessing design space. We infer that the designers’ neurophysiological activation reflects the expansion or contraction of the design space from the analysis of two prototypical tasks. We hypothesize that alpha and beta frequency bands can be an effective approach for measuring changes in design spaces. As the ultimate outcome from this study, we propose a shift in the analysis of design spaces, from external memory representations, such as models and drawings, to the inclusion of brain activation as predictors of design space pliability. Further experiments are necessary to test how far neurophysiological activation can work as an anchor and be correlated with other possible measures of design spaces, as items toward the development of a Design Spaces Index (DSI), a feedback system of the pliability of the design space created by the designers while designing. The ongoing analysis of think-aloud protocols of related experiments (Vieira, Kannengiesser et al., Citation2022), collected while measuring EEG responses can add support to this hypothesis. The development of the Design Spaces Index can be relevant to support neurocognitive, ideational, and creative feedback and inspire methodological changes in design thinking, design management, design education, and design research. More data need to be collected to understand the extent of variation in EEG data of design studies necessary for the development of DSI data sets, of potential use in Artificial Biological Intelligence. This paper provides initial results and a foundation to support the development of the DSI.

Acknowledgments

Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. The 1st author plans to publish the data open access after completing the analyses of the experiment, and is available to provide data and details upon request.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

The research was funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (Grant No. SFRH/BPD/104281/2014). The third author acknowledges the support of the US National Science Foundation (Grant Nos CMMI-1762415 and EEC-1929896).

References

  • Abdulla, A. M., Paek, S. H., Cramond, B., & Runco, M. A. (2020). Problem finding and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000194
  • Abraham, A. (2016). Gender and creativity: An overview of psychological and neuroscientific literature. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2(2), 609–618. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9410-8
  • Abraham, A. (2019). The Neuroscience of Creativity. Cambridge University Press.
  • Abraham, A., Pieritz, K., Thybusch, K., Rutter, B., Krger, S., Schweckendiek, S., Stark, R., Windmann, S., & Hermann, C. (2012). Creativity and the brain: Uncovering the neural signature of conceptual expansion. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 1906–1917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.015
  • Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., & Corazza, G. (2020). Predicting response originality through brain activity: An analysis of changes in EEG alpha power during the generation of alternative ideas. NeuroImage, 207, 116385. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116385.
  • Akin, O. (2001). Variants in Design Cognition. In C. Eastman, M. MacCracken, & W. Newstetter (Eds.), Design Knowing and Learning (pp. 105–124). Elsevier.
  • Alexiou, K., Zamenopoulos, T., Johnson, J. H., & Gilbert, S. J. (2009). Exploring the neurological basis of design cognition using brain imaging: Some preliminary results. Design Studies, 30(6), 623–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2009.05.002
  • Amstel, F. V., Hartmann, T., Voort, M., & Dewulf, G. (2016). The social production of design space. Design Studies, 46, 199–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2016.06.002
  • Argyris, C., Putnam, R., & Smith, D. (1985). Action science: concepts, methods and skills for research and intervention. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
  • Aziz-Zadeh, L., Liew, S.-L., & Dandekar, F. (2013). Exploring the neural correlates of visual creativity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), 475–480. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss021
  • Baer, J., & Kaufman, J. (2008). Gender differences in creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42(2), 75–105. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01289.x
  • Başar, E. (1998a). Brain function and oscillations. i. principles and approaches. Berlin: Springer.
  • Başar, E. (1998b). Brain function and oscillations. ii. integrative brain function. in neurophysiology and cognitive processes. Berlin: Springer.
  • Başar-Eroglu, C., Başar, E., Demiralp, T., & Schurmann, M. (1992). P300-response: Possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 13(2), 161–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(92)90055-G
  • Başar, E., Bazar-Eroglu, C., Karakas, S., & Schurmann, M. (1999). Are cognitive processes manifested in event related gamma, alpha, theta and delta oscillations in the EEG? Neuroscience Letters, 259(3), 165–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00934-3
  • Başar, E., & Karakas, S. (2006). Neuroscience is awaiting for a breakthrough: an essay bridging the concepts of descartes, Einstein, Heisenberg, Hebb and Hayek with the explanatory formulations in this special issue. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(2), 194–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.010
  • Beaty, R., Benedek, M., Silvia, P., & Schacter, D. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(2), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004
  • Beaty, R., & Kennett, Y. (2020). Mapping the creative mind. American Scientist, 108(4), 218–224. https://doi.org/10.1511/2020.108.4.218
  • Benedek, M. (2018). Internally directed attention in creative cognition. In R. E. Jung & O. Vartanian (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 180–194). Cambridge University Press.
  • Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Konen, T., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2011). EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down processing in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia, 49(12), 3505–3511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.004
  • Benedek, M., Christensen, A. P., Fink, A., & Beaty, R. E. (2019). Creativity assessment in neuroscience research. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(2), 218–226. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000215
  • Benedek, M., & Fink, A. (2019). Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: The role of memory, attention, and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 116–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.002
  • Benedek, M., & Fink, A. (2020). Neuroscience: EEG. In: M., Runco, M. & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd edition, vol. 2. Elsevier, Academic Press. 216–220.
  • Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. (2014a). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.021
  • Benedek, M., Jung, R., & Vartanian, O. (2018). The neural bases of creativity and intelligence: Common ground and differences. Neuropsychologia, 118, 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.006
  • Benedek, M., Schickel, R. J., Jauk, E., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014b). Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention. Neuropsychologia, 56(100), 393–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.010
  • Biskjaer, M., & Halskov, K. (2013). Decisive constraints as a creative resource in interaction design. Digital Creativity, 25(1), 27–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/14626268.2013.855239
  • Boden, M. A. (2004). The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms (pp. 1–10). Routledge.
  • Boden, M. A. (2006). Mind as Machine: A History of Cognitive Science (Vol. 1, pp. 2). Oxford University Press.
  • Borgianni, Y., & Maccioni, L. (2020). Review of the use of neurophysiological and biometric measures in experimental design research. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 38(2), 248–285. doi:10.1017/S0890060420000062.
  • Chrysikou, E., & Gero, S. (2020). Using neuroscience techniques to understand and improve design cognition. Aims Neuroscience, 7(3), 319–326. https://doi.org/10.3934/Neuroscience.2020018
  • Clancey, W. (1997). Situated cognition: on human knowledge and computer representations. Cambridge University Press.
  • Cohen, M. X. (2017). Where does EEG come from and what does it mean? Trends Neuroscience, 40(4), 208–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.004
  • Collins, A. (1977). Why cognitive sciencecogn. sci. Cogn. Sci, 1(1), 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0101_1
  • Cross, N. (2001). Design cognition: results from protocol and other empirical studies of design activity. In C. Eastman, M. MacCracken, & W. Newstetter (Eds.), Design knowing and learning (pp. 79–103). Elsevier.
  • Crozier, S., Sirigu, A., Lehéricy, S., van de Moortele, P., Pillon, B., Grafman, J., Agid, Y., Dubois, B., & LeBihan, D. (1999). Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 37(13), 1469–1476. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00054-8
  • De Clercq, W., Vergult, A., Vanrumste, B., Van Paesschen, W., & Van Huffel, S. (2006). Canonical correlation analysis applied to remove muscle artifacts from the electroencephalogram. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 53(12), 2583–2587. https://doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2006.879459
  • Dietrich, A., & Haider, H. (2017). A neurocognitive framework for human creative thought. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2078. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02078
  • Dorst, K. (2019). Co-evolution and emergence in design. Design Studies, 65(C), 65–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2019.10.005
  • Dorst, K., & Cross, N. (2001). Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem-solution. Design Studies, 22(5), 425–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(01)00009-6
  • Eastman, C. (1970). On the analysis of intuitive design processes. In G. Moore (Ed.), Emerging methods of environmental design and planning (pp. 20–37). MIT Press.
  • Eastman, C. (2001). New directions in design cognition:studies of representation and recall. In C. Eastman, M. MacCracken, & W. Newstetter (Eds.), Design knowing and learning (pp. 147–198). Elsevier.
  • Erickson, B., Truelove-Hill, M., Oh, Y., Anderson, J., Zhang, F., & Kounios, J. (2018). Resting-state brain oscillations predict trait-like cognitive styles. Neuropsychologia, 120, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.014
  • Fincham, J., Carter, C., van Veen, V., Stenger, V., & Anderson, J. (2002). Neural mechanisms of planning: A computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 99(5), 3346–3351. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.052703399
  • Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44(100), 111–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.002
  • Fink, A., Benedek, M., Grabner, R. H., Staudt, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Creativity meets neuroscience: Experimental tasks for the neuroscientific study of creative thinking. Methods, 42(1), 68–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.001
  • Fink, A., Grabner, R., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., Neubauer, A. C., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). The creative brain: Investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 734–748. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20538
  • Finke, R., Ward, T., & Smith, S. (1992). Creative cognition. Theory, research and applications. MIT Press.
  • Gabora, L. (2002). Cognitive mechanisms underlying the creative process. In T. Hewett & T. Kavanagh (Eds.), Proceedings of the fourth international conference on creativity and cognition ,(126–13) October 13–16. UK: Loughborough University.
  • Gerlach, C., Law, I., Gade, A., & Paulson, O. B. (2000). Categorization and category effects in normal object recognition: A PET study. Neuropsychologia, 38(13), 1693–1703. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00082-8
  • Gero, J. (1990). Design Prototypes: A knowledge representation schema for design. Artificial Intelligence Magazine, 11(4), 26–36. https://doi.org/10.1609/aimag.v11i4.854
  • Gero, J. S. (2019). From design cognition to design neurocognition. talk at cogsci’19 montreal, ( available from) https://cogsci.mindmodeling.org/2019/papers/0025/0025.pdf
  • Gero, J. S. (2020). Nascent directions for design creativity research. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation, 8(3), 144–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/21650349.2020.1767885
  • Gero, J. S., & Kannengiesser, U. (2004). The situated function-behavior-structure framework. Design Studies, 25(4), 373–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2003.10.010
  • Gero, J., & Kumar, B. (1993). Expanding design spaces through new design variables. Design Studies, 14(2), 210–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/0142-694X(93)80048-H
  • Gero, J., & Milovanovic, J. (2020). A framework for studying design thinking through measuring designers’ minds, bodies and brains. Design Science, 6(e19), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.15
  • Glasser, M., Coalson, T., Robinson, E., Hacker, C., Harwell, J., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K., Andersson, J., Beckmann, C., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., & Van Essen, D. (2016). A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature, 536(7615), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18933
  • Goel, V. (1994). A comparison of design and nondesign problem spaces. Artificial Intelligence in Engineering, 9(1), 53–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/0954-1810(94)90006-X
  • Goel, V. (2014). Creative brains: Designing in the real world. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00241
  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport, 8(5), 1305–1310. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199703240-00049
  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 293–302. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562744
  • Goel, V., & Pirolli, P. (1989). Motivating the notion of generic design within information processing theory: The design problem space. AI Magazine, 10(1), 18–36. https://doi.org/10.1609/aimag.v10i1.726
  • Goel, V., & Pirolli, P. (1992). The structure of design problem spaces. Cognitive Science, 16(3), 395–429. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1603_3
  • Goel, V., & Vartanian, O. (2005). Dissociating the roles of right ventral lateral and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in generation and maintenance of hypotheses in set-shift problems. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1170–1177. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh217
  • Göker, M. (1997). The effects of experience during design problem solving. Design Studies, 18(4), 405–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(97)00009-4
  • Goldschmidt, G. (1997). Capturing indeterminism: Representation in the design problem space. Design Studies, 18(4), 441–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(97)00011-2
  • Goldschmidt, G. (2014). Linkography: unfolding the design process. MIT Press.
  • Goldschmidt, G. (2018). Design creativity research: Recent developments and future challenges. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation, 7(4), 194–195. https://doi.org/10.1080/21650349.2019.1646387
  • Harrington, G., Farias, D., Davis, C., & Buonocore, M. (2007). Comparison of the neural basis for imagined writing and drawing. Hum Brain Mapping, 28(5), 450–459. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20286
  • Hatchuel, A., & Weil, B. (2009). CK design theory: An advanced formulation. Research in Engineering Design, 19(4), 181–192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00163-008-0043-4
  • Hay, L., Cash, P., & McKilligan, S. (2020). The future of design cognition analysis. Design Science, 6, e20, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.20
  • Jaarsveld, S., Fink, A., Rinner, M., Schwab, S., Benedek, M., & Lachmann, T. (2015). Intelligence in creative processes: An EEG study. Intelligence, 49, 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.012
  • Jausovec, N. (1997). Differences in EEG Activity During the Solution of Closed and Open Problems. Creativity Research Journal, 10(4), 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1004_3
  • Jia W and Zeng Y. (2021). EEG signals respond differently to idea generation, idea evolution and evaluation in a loosely controlled creativity experiment. Sci Rep, 11(1), 10.1038/s41598-021-81655-0
  • Kan, J., & Gero, J. (2017). Quantitative Methods for Studying Design Protocols. Springer.
  • Kannengiesser, U., & Gero, J. (2019). Design thinking, fast and slow: A framework for Kahneman’s dual-system theory in design. Design Science, 5(e10), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2019.9
  • Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG syn- chronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(1–2), 61–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00057-8
  • Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2–3), 169–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3
  • Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 606–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007
  • Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Russegger, H., Pachinger, T., & Schwaiger, J. (1998). Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention. Neuroscience Letters, 244(2), 73–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00122-0
  • Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 63–88.
  • Kruger, C., & Cross, N. (2006). Solution driven versus problem driven design: Strategies and outcomes. Design Studies, 27(5), 527–548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2006.01.001
  • Kübler, A., Dixon, V., & Garavan, H. (2006). Automaticity and reestablishment of executive control-an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), 1331–1342. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1331
  • Le, T., Pardo, P., & Hu, X. (1998). 4 T-fMRI study of nonspatial shifting of selective attention: cerebellar and parietal contributions. The American Physiological Society, 79(3), 1535–1548. doi:10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1535.
  • Liang, C., Chang, C., & Liu, Y. (2018). Comparison of the cerebral activities exhibited by expert and novice visual communication designers during idea incubation. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation, 7(4), 213–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/21650349.2018.1562995
  • Liang, C., Lin, C., Yao, C., Chang, W., Liu, Y., & Chen, S. (2017). Visual attention and association: An electroencephalography study in expert designers. Design Studies, 48, 76–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2016.11.002
  • Liu, L., Li, Y., Xiong, Y., Cao, J., & Yuan, P. (2018). An EEG study of the relationship between design problem statements and cognitive behaviors during conceptual design. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing, 32(3), 351–362. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0890060417000683
  • Liu, L., Zeng, Y., & Ben Hamza, A.(2016). Identification of relationships between electroencephalography (EEG) bands and design activities. Volume .7: 28th international conference on design theory and methodology. Charlotte, North Carolina, USA, August 21–24, 2016.
  • Lloyd, P., Lawson, B., & Scott, P. (1996). Can concurrent verbalisation reveal design cognition? Design Studies, 16(2), 237–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/0142-694X(94)00011-2
  • Lundqvist, M., Rose, J., Herman, P., Brincat, S., Buschman, T., & Miller, E. (2016). Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron, 90(1), 152–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028
  • MacDermott, K., Petersen, S., Watson, J., & Ojemann, J. (2003). A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 293–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00162-8
  • MacLean, A., Young, R., Bellotti, V., & Moran, T. (1991). Questions, options, and criteria: Elements of design space analysis. Human–Computer Interaction 6(3–4), 201–250. doi:10.1080/07370024.1991.9667168.
  • Maher, M. L., & Poon, J. (1996). Modeling design exploration as co-evolution. Computer-Aided Civil and Infrastructure Engineering, 11(3), 195–209. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8667.1996.tb00323.x
  • Marsh, R., Zhu, H., Schultz, R., Quackenbush, G., Royal, J., Skudlarski, P., & Peterson, B. (2006). A developmental fMRI study of self-regulatory control. Hum Brain Mapping, 27(11), 848–863. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20225
  • Martindale, C., & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG differences as a function of creativity, stage of the creative process, and effort to be original. Biological Psychology, 6(3), 157–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(78)90018-2
  • Martindale, C., & Hines, D. (1975). Creativity and cortical activation during creative, intellectual and EEG feedback tasks. Biological Psychology, 3(2), 91–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(75)90011-3
  • Nagornova, V. (2007). Changes in the EEG power during tests for nonverbal (figurative) creativity. Human Physiology, 33(3), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119707030036
  • Newell, A., Shaw, J., & Simon, H. (1959). Report on a general problem solving program. In IFIP congress. 1-29. Carnegie Institute of Technology.
  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Prentice Hall.
  • Nguyen, T. A., & Zeng, Y. (2010). Analysis of design activities using EEG signals. 22nd International Conference on Design Theory and Methodology 5, 277–286. Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
  • Oxman, R. (2001). The mind in design: a conceptual framework for cognition in design education. In C. Eastman, M. MacCracken, & W. Newstetter (Eds.), Design knowing and learning (pp. 269–295). Elsevier.
  • Park, C., & Alderman, J. (2018). Designing across senses: A multimodal approach to product design. O’Reilly Media.
  • Pekkola, J., Ojanen, V., Autti, T., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Möttönen, R., Tarkiainen, A., & Sams, M. (2005). Primary auditory cortex activation by visual speech: An fMRI study at 3 T. Neuroreport, 16(2), 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200502080-00010
  • Perišić, M., Storga, M., & Gero, J. (2021). Computational study on design space expansion during teamwork. in Proceedings of the international conference on engineering design (ICED 21), Gothenburg, Sweden (Proceedings of the Design Society), August 16-20, doi:10.1017/pds.2021.69.
  • Pidgeon, L., Grealy, M., Duffy, A., Hay, L., McTeague, C., Vuletic, T., Coyle, D., & Gilbert, S. (2016). Functional neuroimaging of visual creativity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain and Behavior, 6(10), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.540
  • Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R., Lechevalier, B., & Eustache, F. (1997). The structural components of music perception. A functional anatomical study. Brain, 120(2), 229–243. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.2.229
  • Poldrack, R. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004
  • Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72(5), 692–697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001
  • Raimo. S., Santangelo, G., & Trojano, L. (2021). The Neural Bases of Drawing. A Meta-analysis and a Systematic Literature Review of Neurofunctional Studies in Healthy Individuals. Neuropsychol Rev, 31(4), 689–702.
  • Rapp, A., Leube, D., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Kircher, T. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Cogn Brain Research, 20(3), 395–402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.03.017
  • Ray, W., & Cole, H. (1985). EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science, 228(4700), 750–752. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3992243
  • Razumnikova, O., Volf, N., & Tarasova, I. (2010). Creativity associated beta 2 oscillations in men and women. In Proceedings of the International conference on medical physiology (physiology’10) .
  • Richter, H., Costello, P., Sponheim, S., Lee, J., & Pardo, J.P. (2004). Functional neuroanatomy of the human near/far response to blur cues: Eye-lens accommodation/vergence to point targets varying in depth. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20(10), 2722–2732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03743.x
  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., & Fazio, F. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. observation versus execution. Experimental Brain Research, 111(2), 246–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227301
  • Rogers, R., Owen, A., Middleton, H., Williams, E., Pickard, J., Sahakian, B., & Robbins, T. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(20), 9029–9038. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-20-09029.1999
  • Rominger, C., Papousek, I., Perchtold, C., Weber, B., Weiss, E., & Fink, A. (2018). The creative brain in the figural domain: Distinct patterns of EEG alpha power during idea generation and idea elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 18 (Pt A) , 13–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.013
  • Rugg, M., Fletcher, P., Frith, C., Frackowiak, R., & Dolan, R. (1996). Differential activation of the prefrontal cortex in successful and unsuccessful memory retrieval. Brain, 119(6), 2073–2083. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.6.2073
  • Runco, M. (1994). Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity. Ablex.
  • Runco, M., & Jaeger, G. (2012). The standard definition of Creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.650092
  • Runco, M., & Nemiro, J. (1994). Problem finding, creativity and giftedeness. Roeper Review, 16(4), 235–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/02783199409553588
  • Sanei, S., & Chambers, J. (2007). EEG signal processing. John Wiley & Sons.
  • Sawyer, K. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: A critical review. Creativity Research Journal, 23(2), 137–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.571191
  • Schön, D. A. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. Basic Books.
  • Schön, D. A. (1987). Educating the reflective practicioner. Jossey-Bass.
  • Schön, D. A. (1992). Designing as reflective conversation with the materials of a design situation. Knowledge-Based Systems, 5(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/0950-7051(92)90020-G
  • Schwab, D., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). The time- course of EEG alpha power changes in creative ideation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00310
  • Shibata, M., Abe, J., Terao, A., & Miyamoto, T. (2007). Neural mechanisms involved in the comprehension of metaphoric and literal sentences: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 1166, 92–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.06.040
  • Simon, H. A. (1973). The structure of ill structured problems. Artificial Intelligence, 4(3–4), 181–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(73)90011-8
  • Simon, H. A. (1995). Problem forming, problem finding, and problem solving in design. In A. Collen & W. W. Gasparski (Eds.), Design and systems: General applications of methodology (Vol. 3, pp. 245–257). Transaction Publishers.
  • Slotnick, S., & Moo, L. (2006). Prefrontal cortex hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate visual spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 44(9), 1560–1568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.018
  • Smith, S., Ward, T., & Finke, R. (1995). The Creative cognition approach. MIT Press.
  • Stevens, C., & Zabelina, D. (2019). Creativity comes in waves: An EEG-focused exploration of the creative brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 154–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.02.003
  • Sviderskaya, N. (2011). Influence of informational oversaturation on the quality of creative activity and spatial organization of EEG. Human Physiology, 37(6), 667–672. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119711040190
  • Sviderskaya, N., Taratynova, G., & Kozhedub, R. (2006). The effects of the experience of forming visual images on the spatial organization of the EEG. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 36(9), 941–949. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-006-0130-9
  • Vieira, S. (2012). Ambiguity, risk and change in designing: a micro-level description for a property-based approach. In Proceedings of the 2nd Inter. Workshop on Modelling and Management Engineering Processes, pp. 15–28. Cambridge Engineering Design Center.
  • Vieira, S. (2018). EEG Studies of Design Neurocognition: Experiences and Implications. Talk at Ph.D. Lectures series, Politecnico di Milano, Italy. Available from: https://www.mecc.polimi.it/fileadmin/user_upload/File/Dottorato/PhD_Lectures/2018/20181031_MeccPhD_Lecture_Vieira.pdf
  • Vieira, S. (2021). Transdisciplinary design: the environment for bridging research across and beyond design as a discipline. In L. Blessing, A. Qureshi, & K. Gericke (Eds.), The future of transdisciplinary Design (pp. 167–178). Springer Verlag.
  • Vieira, S., Benedek, M., Gero, J., Cascini, G., & Li, S. (2021). Brain activity of industrial designers in constrained and open design: the effect of gender on frequency bands. Proceedings of the design society 23rd international conference on engineering design, 1, 571–580. Gothenburg, Sweden.
  • Vieira, S., Benedek, M., Gero, J., Li, S., & Cascini, G. (2022). Brain activity in constrained and open design: the effect of gender on frequency bands. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing, 36, e6. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0890060421000202
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C., & Fernandes, A. (2019a). Understanding the design neurocognition of mechanical engineers when designing and problem-solving. ASME IDETC, paper IDETC2019-97838.
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C., & Fernandes, A. (2019b). Comparing the design neurocognition of mechanical engineers and architects: a study of the effect of designer’s domain, in 22nd international conference on engineering design 1 (Proceedings of the design society)( 1), 1853–1862.
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C., Parente, M., & Fernandes, A. (2020b). The neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers while designing and problem-solving. Design Science, 6(e26), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.26
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C., Parente, M., & Fernandes, A. (2020e). Industrial designers problem-solving and designing: An EEG study. In book: research & education in design (pp. 211–220). People & Processes & Products & Philosophy. Taylor and Francis.
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Li, S., Cascini, G., & Fernandes, A. (2020d). The Neurophysiological activations of novice and experienced professionals when designing and problem-solving. Proceedings of the Design Society: DESIGN Conference (1), 1569–1578. Cavtat, Croatia.
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J., Gattol, V., Delmoral, J., Li, S., Cascini, G., & Fernandes, A. (2020a). Designing-related neural processes: Higher alpha, theta and beta bands’ key roles in distinguishing designing from problem-solving. Design Computing and Cognition, DCC’20, 535–553. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-90625-2_32.
  • Vieira, S., Gero, J. S., Li, S., Gattol, V., & Fernandes, C. (2020c). Brain activity in constrained and open design spaces: An EEG study. In J.-F. Boujet etal. (eds), The Sixth International Conference on Design Creativity-ICDC2020, Design Society, 68–75. Oulu, Finland.
  • Vieira, S., Kannengiesser, U., & Benedek, M. (2022). Investigating triple process theory in design protocols. Proceedings of the design society: DESIGN Conference. Cavtat, Croatia.
  • Visser, W. (2009). Design: One, but in different forms. Design Studies, 30(3), 187–223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2008.11.004
  • Volf, N., & Tarasova, I. (2010). The relationships between EEG θ and β oscillations and the level of creativity. Human Physiology, 36(2), 132–138. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119710020027
  • Volf, N., Tarasova, I., & Razumnikova, O. (2010). Gender-Related Differences in Changes in the Coherence of Cortical Biopotentials during Image-Based Creative Thought: Relationship with Action Efficacy. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 40(7), 793–799. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-010-9328-y
  • Vorobyev, V., Alho, K., Medvedev, S., Pakhomov, S., Roudas, M., Rutkovskaya, J., Tervaniemi, M., Van Zuijen, T., & Näätänen, R. (2004). Linguistic processing in visual and modality-nonspecific brain areas: PET recordings during selective attention. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(2), 309–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.03.011
  • Vos, D., Riès, S., Vanderperren, K., Vanrumste, B., Alario, F., Huffel, V., & Burle, B. (2010). Removal of muscle artifacts from EEG recordings of spoken language production. Neuroinform, 8(2), 135–150. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12021-010-9071-0
  • Waberski, T., Gobbele, R., Lamberty, K., Buchner, H., Marshall, J., & Fink, G. (2008). Timing of visuo-spatial information processing: Electrical source imaging related to line bisection judgements. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1201–1210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.024
  • Yoshikawa, H. (1981). General design theory and a CAD system. In T. Sata & E. Warman, editors, Man-machine communication in cad/cam, proceedings of the ifip wg 5.2-5.3 working conference 1980 (Tokyo), 35–57, North-Holland, Amsterdam.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.