775
Views
23
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REGULAR ARTICLE

Processing negation without context – why and when we represent the positive argument

, &
Pages 683-698 | Received 04 Apr 2015, Accepted 16 Dec 2015, Published online: 25 Apr 2016
 

ABSTRACT

When processing negative sentences without context, participants often represent states of the positive arguments. Why and when does this occur? Using visual world eye-tracking, participants listened to positive and negative sentences in simple or cleft forms (e.g., [It is] Matt [who] hasn’t shut his dad’s window), while looking at scenes containing a target and a competitor (matches or mismatches the implied shape of the final noun). Results show that in the simple but not the cleft condition, there is a difference between negatives and positives: shortly after the verb, there is more looks to the competitor in the simple negatives than the positives. This suggests that the representation of the positive is not a mandatory first step of negation processing (as per rejection accounts). Rather results support the Question Under Discussion (QUD) accommodation account wherein both sentence content and contextual source of relevance are targets of incremental sentence processing.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes

1. “Positive” and “affirmative” are used interchangeably in this paper. We use “argument of negation” to refer to the semantic, conceptual content of the affirmative counterpart of the sentence containing the negative element, “not”.

2. See Geurts (Citation2010) for a discussion of implicatures derived from presuppositions.

3. But note that other visual world studies report a delay in access to implicatures (see Tomlinson et al., Citation2013).

4. We did not use adjectives gap fillers such as “Matt has/hasn't shut the bright new window”, because adjectives (bright new) often imply visual qualities, which can be used to identify the target. In addition, adjective–noun phrases are often used contrastively, implying a salient alternative with different qualities. This will make the sentences less felicitous against our visual scenes.

5. We did not instruct the speaker to insert a post-verb silence. It occurred from natural reading.

6. See Appendix 2 for plots showing the “raw” probability values, separately for target and competitor.

7. The log correction is applied since fixation proportions are bounded by 0 and 1.

8. It is worth noting that the rejection account proposed by Clark and Chase (Citation1972) and Carpenter and Just (Citation1975) was devised in the period before ‘real-time’ incremental processing was accepted in psycholinguistic research.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Centre for the Study of Mind in Nature (Oslo), “Linguistic Agency” project.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 444.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.