2,383
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

Using more native-like language acquisition processes in the foreign language classroom

| (Reviewing Editor)
Article: 1429134 | Received 14 Sep 2017, Accepted 08 Jan 2018, Published online: 28 Jan 2018

Abstract

This work presents the case for using native-like language and learning networks in the classroom-based teaching of foreign languages and suggests how this might be done for EFL lessons. Networks in both syntax and syntax learning are discussed. Language and learning networks are then identified using principles from Evolutionary Linguistics and Cognitive Linguistics. The work culminates in an examination of how the identified networks in language and language learning can be better employed by teachers in the classroom to improve retention and use. Three general lesson plan types are suggested, which correspond with the three language network types that have been identified, which go further to attach meaning to the structures learnt in class.

Public Interest Statement

The broader implications of this work are diverse and far reaching. On one hand, the work presented here explains why it has been difficult for past generations to use the foreign languages they learnt at school, and how we can change this for future generations. However, the work presented here also gives a good example of how teaching practices need to fit how the brain has evolved to learn in order to maximise the benefits conferred upon students in a formal learning environment.

1. Introduction

All humans use language in the same way (Calude & Pagel, Citation2011; Fitch, Citation2011; Kemp & Regier, Citation2012): language is what humans do. Furthermore, humans did not invent language; rather, it arose over time (Hauser et al., Citation2014; Szathmáry & Smith, Citation1995). However, despite humans having used language for a long time, we still have much to understand about how we develop competency in the use of a language, how language is stored in the brain, how it is processed and produced in communication, and why language change over time benefits these. Moreover, we are still unable to facilitate development of proficiencies in the foreign language classroom that are close to those native children are able to develop in a very short time, and although a great deal of progress has been made in the last couple of decades or so to meet unknowns, much of the work in the aforementioned fields has not found its way into the foreign language classroom in the form of teaching practise. While it is clear that competency with a language comes about through use (Ellis, O’Donnell, & Römer, Citation2015; Tomasello, Citation2008), problems with content-based, task-based and other student-centred methodologies abound (Baecher, Farnsworth, & Ediger, Citation2014; Bruton, Citation2011; Ellis, Citation2009; Foster, Citation2009), often because teachers do not understand what they ought to do, because linguistic objectives are hard to incorporate, or simply because lower proficiency levels do not have the language skills to participate.

Grammar is essential to language use: a gifted person could memorise the dictionary of a certain language, but would not be able to communicate using that language without knowledge of grammar. This paper is the result of research aiming to identify how native-like syntax-learning processes can be better replicated and incorporated into lessons. Initially these lessons are designed for university students on compulsory courses, with lower levels of proficiency, who have already been the recipients of many years of formal, classroom-based, English as a Foreign Language (EFL) tuition, but who have never developed any communicative competence although the method could equally well be used with children starting their foreign language education. The need for the work has arisen because it is clear that under current teaching practises some aspects of a foreign language are very hard to retain and use for students, such as past participles in English, but they are integral to using that specific language.

One good candidate reason for the general underperformance of classroom-based foreign language acquisition is not utilising the network structure of language as an integral part of the learning process when building form-meaning units. Native-speaking foreign language teachers, i.e. those teaching abroad, are all familiar with students who are able to complete paper-based exercises, e.g. gap-fill exercises, perfectly, but who have no skill at all in using the structures in the exercise to communicate. The work presented here suggests ways to incorporate network-based methods to better build form-meaning pairings in the classroom, and is useful for foreign language teachers as it suggests lessons plans for the purpose.

The work begins by examining networks in words and structures and in language learning, goes on to identify those language networks that ought to be taught in EFL lessons and culminates with corresponding lesson plans.

2. Networks that contribute to language acquisition

Networks, like fishing nets laid out on a beach, can be two-dimensional, or if each point of connection, or node, connects to many other points of connection, can be in more dimensions. More complex systems can have links—or edges—of different strengths, exist only between specific nodes, have multiple levels or be active only at certain times (Davies, Kounios, & Power, Citation2015; Kivelä et al., Citation2014; Sayama, Citation2015). Networked systems pervade nature, including brain structure and function, and language.

2.1. Networks in the brain: Domain-general behaviour, learning and “chunks”

The brain is a network of activity, with neurones being linked to other neurones and electrical activity travelling across them (Medaglia, Lynall, & Bassett, Citation2015; Mišić & Sporns, Citation2015; van Schijndel, Exley, & Schuler, Citation2013; Yaveroğlu et al., Citation2014). Furthermore, domain-general behaviours, being controlled by the brain, also exhibit network properties, as do learning behaviours (Hinton, Citation2007; Solway et al., Citation2014). Examples of networked learning-behaviours providing added benefits for learning include reduced processing times with increased intelligence (Hearne, Mattingley, & Cocchi, Citation2016; Tang et al., Citation2010), predictive ability (Clark, Citation2013; Weber, Lau, Stillerman, & Kuperberg, Citation2016), finding patterns (Egner, Citation2014; Tenenbaum, Kemp, Griffiths, & Goodman, Citation2011), learning by trial and error (Botvinick, Citation2012; O’Doherty, Lee, & McNamee, Citation2015), and adaptive behaviour (Cushman & Morris, Citation2015; Schiffer, Waszak, & Yeung, Citation2015).

It is important to note that these domain-general networks of activity and learning are also present in the brains of other animals (e.g. Bardella, Bifone, Gabrielli, Gozzi, & Squartini, Citation2016; Dehghani et al., Citation2016). Furthermore, learning actually changes brain networks (Bassett et al., Citation2011; Bola & Borchardt, Citation2016), meaning the brain can be seen as a network that is in constant flux. Indeed, if the physical structure of the brain is in flux, then so too must any stored information. Taking small combinations of stored information from cognitive networks and using them sequentially, whether in learning, processing or production, is called chunking, the small combinations being chunks. Chunking is a defining characteristic of how the brain learns and manages information and is thought to be instrumental in allowing integration of the short-term memory and long-term information retention and learning (Chekaf, Cowan, & Mathy, Citation2016; Fonollosa, Neftci, & Rabinovich, Citation2015; Lee, Seo, & Jung, Citation2012; Mathy, Fartoukh, Gauvrit, & Guida, Citation2016), and networks in language production and processing most probably rely on chunking. Having now looked at domain-general learning networks, the network-like structure of language will be examined.

2.2. Networks in language

Language structure is determined by brain function (Bickel, Witzlack-Makarevich, Choudhary, Schlesewsky, & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Citation2015; Garagnani & Pulvermüller, Citation2016; Skeide, Brauer, & Friederici, Citation2015; Zaccarella & Friederici, Citation2016), and is composed from combinatorial signals—chunks—from networks of stored inventories (Carr, Smith, Cornish, & Kirby, Citation2016; Christiansen & Chater, Citation2016; Hoffmann & Trousdale, Citation2011; Martinčić-Ipšić, Margan, & Meštrović, Citation2016; Solé, Corominas-Murtra, Valverdie, & Steels, Citation2011). Furthermore, the signals from which language is composed are ambiguous in their meaning unless placed in context (Piantadosi, Tily, & Gibson, Citation2012; Wedel, Citation2012), and that context, and the meaning created, are derived from learning (van Dijk, Citation2006; Krishnan, Watkins, & Bishop, Citation2016; MacDonald, Citation2015; Tamariz, Ellison, Barr, & Fay, Citation2014; Ullman, Citation2016). For native speakers learning in their natural environment, learning depends on the frequency of exposure to a certain linguistic structure, or form, and its situational concomitant meaning. This meaning is initially generalised and ever more refined and segmented as the frequency of contextually experiencing the form-meaning pairing increases (Frost & Monaghan, Citation2016; Silvey, Kirby, & Smith, Citation2015).

Therefore, bringing meaning to ambiguity through context results in a finite set of signals becoming a rich and flexible system of language that is able to convey infinite meaning. Nodes of ambiguous words in the environment are linked to nodes of ambiguous cognitive concepts and given meaning after being linked to other nodes by edges of context. Moreover, the process of composing signals from these networked components manifests the syntactic structures found in language.

For example, in English, embedded structures (Christiansen & MacDonald, Citation2009; Miyagawa, Berwick, & Okanoya, Citation2013), dependency of time, gender and number/countability—or parsing (Beuls & Steels, Citation2013; de Vries, Magnus Petersson, Geukes, Zwitserlood, & Christiansen, Citation2012; van den Bos, Christiansen, & Misyak, Citation2012) and collocations and linguistic formulas (Brezina, McEnery, & Wattam, Citation2015; McCauley & Christiansen, Citation2014) are all examples of networks that are ambiguous nodes of words and meanings until linked together by edges of context to create word-meaning associations. In this work, a specific type of collocation or formula will be concentrated on, namely a root word that appears with other words on separate occasions that change the context and therefore the meaning, e.g. past participles being found with different auxiliary verbs. Specific examples are presented below in Table .

Table 1. Examples of syntactic structures in English that employ network-like structure

Finally, networks in communication signals are not unique to humans (Collier, Bickel, van Schaik, Manser, & Townsend, Citation2014, Rey, Perruchet & Fagot, Citation2012), and as described above with domain-general learning being networked, gaining the ability to use a language—language acquisition—is the interaction of language networks, and learning and memory networks, which will be examined below.

2.3. Networks in the acquisition of syntactic structures and their concomitant meanings

Memory and learning are networked phenomena with different areas of the brain being linked together and playing different roles. Working memory, colloquially called short-term memory, is very limited and can store correctly three to five “chunks” of information (Baddeley, Citation2012; Cowan, Citation2010), and it is necessary to minimise any dependency on working memory when learning to use languages (O’Grady, Citation2015). Long-term memory, which is associated with learning, is understood as being of the following two non-exclusive types: procedural memory—implicitly learning through practice, such as learning a sport, playing an instrument or using tools—and declarative memory—explicitly memorising things like dates and names for an exam (Andringa & Rebuschat, Citation2015; Ellis, Citation2015; Morgan-Short, Faretta-Stutenberg, Brill-Schuetz, Carpenter, & Wong, Citation2014; Ullman, Citation2016).

Specific implicit learning phenomena that relate to the frequency of occurrence of certain structures, or formulas (e.g. Ellis, Citation2012; Wray, Citation2012, 2013), resulting in those formulas becoming entrenched and proceduralised (Krishnan et al., Citation2016; Ullman, Citation2016), and gramaticalised (Chang, Citation2008) include a bias towards using recently heard structures—a process called structural or syntactic priming (Kaschak, Kutta, & Coyle, Citation2012; Mahowald, James, Futrell, & Gibson, Citation2016; Pickering & Ferreira, Citation2008; Rowland, Chang, Ambridge, Pine, & Lieven, Citation2012), entrenching forms after repetitive exposure—a process called statistical learning (Fine & Jaeger, Citation2013; Frank, Tenenbaum, & Gibson, Citation2013; Vuong, Meyer, & Christiansen, Citation2016), demarcation of word boundaries (Erickson & Thiessen, Citation2016; Finn & Hudson Kam, Citation2015) and allocation of syntactic category (Reeder, Newport, & Aslin, Citation2012; Robenalt & Goldberg, Citation2015). Other specific examples of implicit learning include mastering embedded hierarchies (Lai & Poletiek, Citation2011, 2013) and embedded relative clauses (Fitz, Chang, & Christiansen, Citation2011), and eliminating ambiguity by learning to parse (Haskell, Thornton, & MacDonald, Citation2010; Phillips & Ehrenhofer, Citation2015; Pozzan & Trueswell, Citation2015). Specific examples of syntactic structures are presented in Table .

Table 2. Networked syntactic structures that are learnt with networked-learning

Additionally, specific learning phenomena of which the language user has some awareness—explicit learning—include building meaning through comparison and attaching it to words (Lany & Saffran, Citation2011; Wojcik & Saffran, Citation2013) and structures (Syrett, Arunachalam, & Waxman, Citation2014; van Dam & Desai, Citation2016), and using generalised concepts of meaning in different contexts (Finn & Hudson Kam, Citation2015; Fisher, Gertner, Scott, & Yuan, Citation2010; Foraker, Regier, Khetarpal, Perfors, & Tenenbaum, Citation2009). Specific examples of syntactic structures are presented in Table below.

Notice that the second and third rows and the last row in Table are very similar to the three rows in Table .

To enable foreign language teachers to use language networks effectively in the classroom, it is necessary to know which form-meaning parings to teach foreign language students and how they are created by native speakers.

3. Identifying which form-meaning pairings to teach in the foreign language classroom and how they are created by natives

Having defined the roles of networks in language structure and acquisition, it is now necessary to know which structures are the most important to teach in a foreign language classroom, and how those forms are given meaning by users through experience. In this case, the “most important networks” being those that facilitate any rudimentary-level of communication. In order to do this, two different branches of linguistics will be employed: Evolutionary Linguistics (EL) and Cognitive Linguistics (CL). Both will be explained in greater detail below, but a comparison is presented below in Table . Essentially, EL is a way of understanding words and structures and CL is a way of understanding how meaning is attached to words.

Learning the meaning of language through use is a common phenomenon observed in action by all parents and all persons who go to live in a country where a different language is spoken. However, while CL can explain how meaning is attached to language, it cannot tell us to why it becomes attached to the language units it does, which is where EL is needed. To identify and use networks in language in the foreign language classroom, we need to employ ideas from EL and CL, and a comparison of both is presented in Table .

Table 3. Comparing evolutionary linguistics and cognitive linguistics

Evolutionary Linguistics is a framework of theories that model some reasons as to why words and structures change in terms of becoming easier to learn and use after being inherited from parental generations in the process of iterative learning, or learning by copying (de Boer, Citation2015; Kirby, Griffiths, & Smith, Citation2014). As they have social impacts, the processes that drive language change in EL are referred to as cultural selection (Steels, Citation2011; Tamariz et al., Citation2014). The implications for words and structures are broad, and EL has been used to explain semantic change (Landsbergen, Lachlan, Ten Cate, & Verhagen, Citation2010), syntactic change (Kirby, Tamariz, Cornish, & Smith, Citation2015) and vocabulary evolution (Smith, Citation2004).

Cognitive Linguistics (CL) is a branch of linguistic research that uses principles that are general to cognition as a whole to model some ways the brain learns and uses language from the environment. First, that an innate linguistic faculty does not exist, i.e. all brain-based competencies used for the production and processing of language are not language-specific; second, that the meaning of language exists in the brain as concepts; and finally, that meaning comes through use (Croft & Cruse, Citation2004, Pleyer & Winters, Citation2015). Core ideas in CL are the Cognitive Commitment and the Generalisation Commitment. Essentially, the Cognitive Commitment states that any ideas about how the brain handles language should not be different to how the brain handles any other task; whereas the Generalisation Commitment states that any principle in CL should apply to all human languages (Evans, Citation2012). Meaning coming through use, is referred to as the “usage-based” approach (Ellis et al., Citation2015; Janda, Citation2015; Tyler & Ortega, Citation2016), and two subthemes from CL, Connectionism and Constructivism, approach the topic in different ways (Lain, Citation2016). Connectionism sees knowledge as being represented as patterns of numerical activity across simple processing units; where processing occurs across large sets of connections in networks, and; in which learning occurs through non-language specific, general mechanisms combined with experience (Joanisse & McClelland, Citation2015). Constructivism sees individuals creating meaning over time through experience and active participation, rather than “acquiring it”, and that this knowledge occurs within the context that was learnt (Ertmer & Newby, Citation2013). Both these ideas rely on the network structures of concepts and situational use (Baronchelli, Ferrer-i-Cancho, Pastor-Satorras, Chater, & Christiansen, Citation2013).

3.1. Combining EL and CL

Language exits as words and structures that human brains can attach meaning to and use to communicate effectively. Networks of cognitive form-meaning parings allow efficient communication—storage, retrieval, processing and production—in real time (Chater, McCauley, & Christiansen, Citation2016; Garagnani & Pulvermüller, Citation2016), making language usable and learnable.

Language is usable because the different network types present in language—reusing the same word in different combinations/chunks/transposing concepts (Arnon & Christiansen, Citation2014, Christiansen & Chater, Citation2016), dependencies (Dyson, Citation2009; Hoffmann & Trousdale, Citation2011; Kuperberg & Jaeger, Citation2015; O’Grady, Citation2015; Omaki & Lidz, Citation2015; Traugott, Citation2014) and embedded structures (Lai & Poletiek, Citation2011; Piantadosi et al., Citation2012; Trueswell & Gleitman, Citation2007)—have evolved to be dealt with by a brain that operates using different memory types that have finite capacities for storage and processing (Krishnan et al., Citation2016; Vagharchakian, Dehaene-Lambertz, Pallier, & Dehaene, Citation2012) and uses proceduralisation and prediction to speed up processing (Huettig, Citation2015; van Schijndel et al., Citation2013).

Language is learnable because the combined characteristics of language and cognition mean an individual is able to learn his or her native language by interacting with the environment (Lain, Citation2016; Tamariz et al., Citation2014; Winters, Kirby, & Smith, Citation2015), and that other users of the same language distributed in time, e.g. generations, space, e.g. continents, can still understand each other (Silvey et al., Citation2015, Wedel, Citation2012) despite their different learning experiences (Foraker et al., Citation2009; Kirby et al., Citation2015).

3.2. Identifying specific examples of networked structures

Identification of the most important networked words and structures that are learnt in a natural, native environment that need to be taught in the foreign language classroom can be effected by employing ideas from EL. If language is changing to be more usable and learnable, it is necessary to teach the language that is changing as a part of the networks in which it is changing. To facilitate this analysis, the level of change has been categorised as follows: lost, has found multiple uses, currently changing and has not changed despite expectations. These categories and examples, are then cross-referenced with their proposed drivers of evolutionary change and contextual situations in which a native might become familiar with them are presented in Table . The forth column in Table , which depicts structures that have not changed despite an expectation that they might is not trivial. Networks of overlapping usages, some of which are vital to the functional integrity of the language, might conserve certain domains of linguistic structure through a language’s evolutionary history, as with specific areas of restricted genome change in biological evolution (Blair Hedges & Kumar, Citation2003; Siepel et al., Citation2005). Furthermore, the third row, which presents examples of iterative learning situations, helps users to build a meaning and attach it to a structure: a process that leads on to the next section, which seeks to identify how specific concepts of meaning are formed.

Table 4. Language change examples, their evolutionary drivers and situations in which natives might become familiar with them

Additionally, identification of the most fundamental networked concepts of meaning that natives attach to certain words and structures that need to be taught in the foreign language classroom can be effected using ideas from CL. The words and structures presented in Table are processed in situations providing context, and meaning is formed over repeated episodes of use. Examples of how these concepts are linked with the concomitant language in the natural environment, which might be replicated in the classroom, are presented in Table along with the three previously identified network types: embedded structures, agreement and transposed concepts of meaning from Tables and .

Table 5. Examples of networked syntactic structures and social situations of use in which they could gain meaning

Having now identified the most fundamental form-meaning pairings in English that ought to be taught in the EFL classroom, it is possible to construct lesson plans that replicate these learning processes in lessons.

4. Replicating how natives use cognitive and linguistic networks to create and proceduralise form-meaning pairings in the EFL classroom

Having identified how networks are involved in language and language learning, presented below in Table are three lesson plan types, based on the three learning networks identified, namely embedding, dependencies and transposing concepts of meaning.

Table 6. Lesson plan types that incorporate linguistic and cognitive networks to build meaning through use and attaching it to structures and words

The same language and learning networks have been followed in Tables , and . However, implicitly learning from repeated expose to certain structures and explicitly attaching concepts of meaning to words and structures appear only in Table . That is because these learning processes need to be a part of every lesson. Furthermore, although these lesson plans aim to facilitate implicit learning as much as possible, it has been shown that in the early stages of learning a foreign language, implicit learning alone is not effective (Andringa & Rebuschat, Citation2015; Boers, Lindstromberg & Eyckmans, Citation2014), and some explicit instruction and declarative-learning is beneficial (Morgan-Short et al., Citation2014). These lessons should therefore contain a minimum of explicit instruction, but the lessons must be repeated continually to facilitate implicit learning. Additionally, each lesson must contain an element that demonstrates that student has linked a concept of meaning to the language being used.

These methods could be used to teach most of the structures used in everyday English, as depicted in Table .

Table 7. Examples of syntactic forms that might be taught using these different teaching approaches

5. Conclusion

Lesson plans have been suggested that purport to facilitate learning in a more native-like way in classroom-based EFL lessons, and thereby aid retention, recall and communication skill. The proposed lesson plans use the results of linguistic, teaching and neuroscience research to provide a bridge across the gap between compulsory foreign language students and children learning their native language in order to improve foreign language teaching.

Core to the argument presented is the use of the same three networks used in learning and the use that are present in English, namely embedded structures, agreement of dependencies and transposing concepts or meaning. It has been shown that the brain stores, retrieves, processes and produces information as networks of ambiguous concepts that gain meaning through context, and the lesson plans presented here use the same networks as those found in the networks in words and structures, and learning.

The teaching approach suggested might be termed guided-implicit learning, and it aims to intensify and speed up natural implicit learning processes such that the benefit provided by the necessarily limited amount of classroom instruction time is maximised. Furthermore, three general lesson plan types have been defined, which are based on the networks shown to be present in English and accessible by networked, domain-general learning processes in human cognition. .

6. Future research

Despite the advances made in teaching a foreign language that are made by incorporating the network principles discussed above, further resolution is necessary of the syntactic structures and the roles of language and learning networks determining how native speakers learn and use them. Examples might include how native speakers implicitly know to use a gerund (“-ing” form) or an infinitive (“to” form) of a verb when it is used as the subject of another verb, for example, “I want to do” or “I suggest doing”. Another might be implicitly knowing the required order of adjectives, for example, “the fat, tired, old, grey, EFL teacher” and not any other combination. Certainly, data driven learning (Boulton & Tyne, Citation2013; Callies & Paquot, Citation2015; Gablasova, Brezina, & McEnery, Citation2017; Granger, Citation2008)—using software to analyse collections of language use grouped by type, for example, native-speaking children of different ages, which is available for example, in the CHILDES database in the Talkbank system—will be indispensable in identifying learning and using networks in any future work.

Funding

The authors received no direct funding for this research.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank one anonymous referee for the very helpful review.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Kieran Green

My research activities take place at the intersection of human evolution, cognition and memory, and language learning and seek to bridge the gap in proficiencies in learning one’s native language and foreign languages, focusing on compulsory foreign language lessons in secondary and tertiary education.

References

  • Andringa, S., & Rebuschat, P. (2015). New directions in the study of explicit and implicit-learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 185–196. doi:10.1017/S027226311500008X10.1017/S027226311500008X
  • Arnon, I., & Christiansen, M. (2014). Chunk-based language acquisition. In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language development (pp. 88–91). SAGE publications.
  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Baecher, L., Farnsworth, T., & Ediger, A. (2014). The challenges of planning language objectives in content-based ESL instruction. Language Teaching Research, 18, 118–136. doi:10.1177/1362168813505381
  • Bardella, G., Bifone, A., Gabrielli, A., Gozzi, A., & Squartini, T. (2016). Hierarchical organization of functional connectivity in the mouse brain: A complex network approach. Scientific Reports, 6, 519. doi:10.1038/srep32060
  • Baronchelli, A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Pastor-Satorras, R., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Networks in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 348–360. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.010
  • Boers, F., Lindstromberg, S., & Eyckmans, J. (2014). Some explanations for the slow acquisition of L2 collocations. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11, 41–62.
  • Bassett, D. S., Wymbs, N. F., Porter, M. A., Mucha, P. J., Carlson, J. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2011). Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 108, 7641–7646. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018985108
  • Beuls, K., & Steels, L. (2013). Agent-based models of strategies for the emergence and evolution of grammatical agreement. PLoS One, 8, e0058960. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058960
  • Bickel, B., Witzlack-Makarevich, A., Choudhary, K. K., Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. (2015). The neurophysiology of language processing shapes the evolution of grammar: Evidence from case marking. PLoS One, 10, e0132819. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0132819
  • Blair Hedges, S., & Kumar, S. (2003). Genomic clocks and evolutionary timescales. Trends in Genetics, 19, 200–206. doi:10.1016/S0168-9525(03)00053-2
  • de Boer, B. (2015). Evolution of speech-specific cognitive adaptations. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01505
  • Bola, M., & Borchardt, V. (2016). Cognitive processing involves dynamic reorganisation of the whole-brain network’s functional community structure. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 3633–3635. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0106-16.2016
  • Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Hierarchical reinforcement learning and decision making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22, 956–962. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.008
  • Boulton, A., & Tyne, H. (2013). Corpus linguistics and data-driven learning: A critical overview. Bulletin Suisse de Linguistique Appliquée, 97, 97–118.
  • Brezina, V., McEnery, T., & Wattam, S. (2015). Collocations in Context. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 20, 139–173. doi:10.1075/ijcl.20.2.01bre
  • Bruton, A. (2011). Is CLIL so beneficial, or just selective? Re-evaluating some of the research. System, 39, 523–532. doi:10.1016/j.system.2011.08.002
  • Callies, M., & Paquot, M. (2015). Learner corpus research: An interdisciplinary field on the move. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 1, 1–6. doi:10.1075/ijlcr.1.1.00edi
  • Calude, A. S., & Pagel, M. (2011). How do we use language? Shared patterns in the frequency of word use across 17 world languages. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1101–1107. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0315
  • Carr, J. W., Smith, K., Cornish, H., & Kirby, S. (2016). The cultural evolution of structured languages in an open-ended, continuous world. Cognitive Science, 41, 892–923. doi:10.1111/cogs.12371
  • Chang, F. (2008). Implicit learning and language change. Theoretical Linguistics, 34, 115–122. doi:10.1515/THLI.2008.009
  • Chater, N., McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Language as skill: Intertwining comprehension and production. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 244–254. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2015.11.004
  • Chekaf, M., Cowan, N., & Mathy, F. (2016). Chunk formation in immediate memory and how it relates to data compression. Cognition, 155, 96–107. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.024
  • Collier, K., Bickel, B., van Schaik, C. P., Manser, M. B., & Townsend, S. W. (2014). Language Evolution: Syntax Before Phonology? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20140263. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0263
  • Croft, W. & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 279. doi:10.1017/S0140525X1500031X . Epub 2015 Apr 14.
  • Christiansen, M. H., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). A usage-based approach to recursion in sentence processing. Language Learning, 59, 126–161.10.1111/lang.2009.59.issue-s1
  • Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181–204. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000477
  • Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 51–57. doi:10.1177/0963721409359277
  • Cushman, F., & Morris, A. (2015). Habitual control of goal selection in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 13817–13822. doi:10.1073/pnas.1506367112
  • Davies, A., Kounios, L., & Power, D. (2015). Evolutionary connectionism: Algorithmic principles underlying the evolution of biological organisation in Evo-Devo, Evo-Eco and Evolutionary Transitions. Evolutionary Biology, 1–29. doi:10.1007/s11692-015-9358-z
  • Dehghani, N., Peyrache, A., Telenczuk, B., Le Van Quyen, M., Halgren, E., Cash, S. S., … Destexhe A. (2016). Dynamic balance of excitation and inhibition in human and monkey neocortex. Scientific Reports, 6, 569. doi:10.1038/srep23176
  • de Vries, M. H., Magnus Petersson, K., Geukes, S., Zwitserlood, P., & Christiansen, M. H. (2012). Processing multiple non-adjacent dependencies: Evidence from sequence learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 367, 2065–2076. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0414
  • Dyson, B. (2009). Processability theory and the role of morphology in English as a second language development: A longitudinal study. Second Language Research, 25, 335–376. doi:10.1177/0267658309104578
  • Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and Control): A mulit-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01247
  • Ellis, R. (2009). Task-based language teaching: Sorting out the misunderstandings. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 19, 221–246. doi:10.1111/j.1473-4192.2009.00231.x
  • Ellis, N. C. (2012). Formulaic language and second language acquisition: Zipf and the phrasal teddy bear. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 17–44.10.1017/S0267190512000025
  • Ellis, N. C. (2015). Implicit and explicit language learning: Their dynamic interface and complexity. In P. Rebuschat (ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/sibil
  • Ellis, N. C., O’Donnell, M. B., & Römer, U. (2015). Usage-based language learning. In Brian MacWhinney and William O’Grady (eds.), The handbook of language emergence (pp. 163–180). NJ, John Wiley & Sons,.
  • Evans, V. (2012). Cognitive linguistics. WIRES Cognitive Science, 3, 129–141. doi:10.1002/wcs.1163
  • Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D. (2016). Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predications of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Development Review, 37, 66–108. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.05.006
  • Ertmer, P. A., & Newby, T. J. (2013). Behaviorism, cognitivism, constructivism: Comparing critical features from an instructional design perspective. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 26, 43–71. doi:10.1002/piq.21143
  • Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F. (2013). Evidence for the learning of syntactic comprehension. Cognitive Science, 37, 578–591. doi:10.1111/cogs.12022
  • Finn, A. S., & Hudson Kam, C. L. (2015). Why segmentation matters: Experience-driven segmentation errors impair “morpheme” learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 41, 1560–1569. doi:10.1037/xlm0000114
  • Fisher, C., Gertner, Y., Scott, R. M., & Yuan, S. (2010). Syntactic bootstrapping. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 143–149. doi:10.1002/wcs.17
  • Fitch, W. T. (2011). Unity and diversity in human language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 376–388. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0223
  • Fitz, H., Chang, F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). A connectionist account of the acquisition and processing of relative clauses. In E. Kidd (Ed.), The acquisition of relative clauses (pp. 39–60). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  • Fonollosa, J., Neftci, E., & Rabinovich, M. (2015). Learning of chunking sequences in cognition and behavior. PLoS Computational Biology, 11, e1004592. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004592
  • Foraker, S., Regier, T., Khetarpal, N., Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. (2009). Indirect evidence and the poverty of the stimulus: The case of anaphoric one. Cognitive Science, 33, 287–300. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01014.x
  • Foster, P. (2009). Task-based language learning research: Expecting too much or too little? International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 19, 247–263. doi:10.1111/j.1473-4192.2009.00242.x
  • Frank, M. C., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Gibson, E. (2013). Learning and long-term retention of large-scale artificial languages. PLoS One, 8(1), e52500.10.1371/journal.pone.0052500
  • Frost, R. L. A., & Monaghan, P. (2016). Simultaneous segmentation and generalisation of non-adjacent dependencies from continuous speech. Cognition, 147, 70–74. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.11.010
  • Gablasova, D., Brezina, V., & McEnery, T. (2017). Exploring learner language through corpora: Comparing and interpreting corpus frequency information. Language Learning, 67, 130–154. doi:10.1111/lang.12226
  • Garagnani, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2016). Conceptual grounding of language in action and perception: A Neurocomputational model of the emergence of category specificity and semantic hubs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 43, 724–737. doi:10.1111/ejn.13145
  • Granger, S. (2008). Learner corpora in foreign language education. In N. Van Deusen-Scholl & N. H. Hornberger (eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education. Volume 4. Second and foreign language education (pp. 337–351). Springer.
  • Haskell, T. R., Thornton, R., & MacDonald, M. C. (2010). Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production. Cognition, 114, 151–164. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.017
  • Hauser, M. D., Yang, C., Berwick, R. C., Tattersall, I., Ryan, M. J., Watumull, J., … Lewontin, R. (2014). The mystery of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00401
  • Hearne, L. J., Mattingley, J. B., & Cocchi, L. (2016). Functional brain networks related to individual differences in human intelligence at rest. Scientific Reports, 6, 77. doi:10.1038/srep32328
  • Hinton, G. E. (2007). Learning multiple layers of representation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 428–434. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.004
  • Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (2011). Variation, change and constructions in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 22, 1–23. doi:10.1515/COGL.2011.001
  • Huettig, F. (2015). Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118–135. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.014
  • Janda, L. A. (2015). Cognitive linguistics in the year 2015. Cognitive Semantics, 1, 131–154. doi:10.1163/23526416-00101005
  • Joanisse, M. F., & McClelland, J. L. (2015). Connectionist perspectives on language learning. Representation and processing. WIREs Cognitive Science. doi:10.1002/wes.1340
  • Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Coyle, J. M. (2012). Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 728–743. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.641387
  • Kemp, C., & Regier, T. (2012). Kinship categories across languages reflect general communicative principles. Science, 336, 1049–1054.10.1126/science.1218811
  • Kirby, S., Griffiths, T., & Smith, K. (2014). Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 28, 108–114.10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.014
  • Kivelä, M., Arenas, A., Barthelemy, M., Gleeson, J. P., Moreno, Y., & Porter, M. A. (2014). Multilayer Networks. Journal of Complex Networks, 2, 203–271. doi:10.1093/comnet/cnu016
  • Kirby, S., Tamariz, M., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2015). Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure. Cognition, 141, 87–102.10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.016
  • Krishnan, S., Watkins, K. E., & Bishop, D. V. (2016). Neurobiological basis of language learning difficulties. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20, 701–714. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.012
  • Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F. (2015). What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience,. doi:10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299
  • Lai, J., & Poletiek, F. H. (2011). The impact of adjacent-dependencies and staged-input on the learnability of centre-embedded hierarchical structures. Cognition, 118, 265–273. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.011
  • Lai, J., & Poletiek, F. H. (2013). How “small” is “starting small” for learning hierarchical centre-embedded structures? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 423–435. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.779247
  • Lain, S. (2016). Content-based instruction understood in terms of connectionism and constructivism. L2 Journal, 1, 18-31.
  • Landsbergen, F., Lachlan, R., Ten Cate, C., & Verhagen, A. (2010). A cultural evolutionary model of patterns in semantic change. Linguistics, 48, 363–390. doi:10.1515/ling.2010.012
  • Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2011). Interactions between statistical and semantic information in infant language development. Developmental Science, 14, 1207–1219. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01073.x
  • Lee, D., Seo, H., & Jung, M. W. (2012). Neural basis of reinforcement learning and decision making. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 287–308. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150512
  • MacDonald, M. (2015). The emergence of language comprehension. In B. MacWhinney & W. O’Grady (Eds.), The handbook of language emergence (pp. 81–92). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E. (2016). A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.009
  • Martinčić-Ipšić, S., Margan, D., & Meštrović, A. (2016). Multilayer network of language: A unified framework for structural analysis of linguistic subsystems. Physica A, 457, 117–128. doi:10.1016/j.physa.2016.03.082
  • Mathy, F., Fartoukh, M., Gauvrit, N., & Guida, A. (2016). Developmental abilities to form chunks in immediate memory and its non-relationship to span development. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 201. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00201
  • McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2014). Acquiring formulaic language, a computational model. The Mental Lexicon, 9, 419–436. doi:10.1075/ml.9.3.03mcc
  • Medaglia, J. D., Lynall, M. E., & Bassett, D. S. (2015). Cognitive network neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1471–1491. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00810
  • Mišić, B., & Sporns, O. (2015). From regions to connections and networks: New bridges between brain and behaviour. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2016.05.003
  • Miyagawa, S., Berwick, R. C., & Okanoya, K. (2013). The emergence of hierarchical structure in human language. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/psyg.2013.00071
  • Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. M. C. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 17, 56–72.10.1017/S1366728912000715
  • O’Doherty, J. P. W., Lee, S., & McNamee, D. (2015). The structure of reinforcement-learning mechanisms in the human brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 94–100. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.10.004
  • O’Grady, W. (2015). Processing determinism. Language Learning, 65, 6–32. doi:10.1111/lang.12091
  • Omaki, A., & Lidz, J. (2015). Linking Parser Development to Acquisition of Syntactic Knowledge. Language Acquisition, 22, 158–192. doi:10.1080/10489223.2014.943903
  • Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L. (2015). The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5, 409–453. doi:10.1075/lab.5.4.01phi
  • Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2012). The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition, 122, 280–291. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.004
  • Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427–459. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427
  • Pleyer, M., & Winters, J. (2015). Integrating cognitive linguistics and language evolution research. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 11, 19–43. doi:10.12775/ths-2014-002
  • Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J. C. (2015). Syntactic processing and acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5, 516–521. doi:10.1075/lab.5.4.13poz
  • Reeder, P. A., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2012). From shared contexts to syntactic categories: The role of distributional information in learning linguistic form-classes. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 30–54. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.09.001
  • Rey, A., Perruchet, P., & Fagot, J. (2012). Centre-embedded structures are a by-product of associative learning and working memory constraints: Evidence from baboons (Papio Papio). Cognition, 123, 180–184. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.005
  • Robenalt, C., & Goldberg, A. E. (2015). Judgment evidence for statistical preemption: It is relatively better to vanish than to disappear a rabbit, but a life guard can equally well backstroke or swim children to shore. Cognitive Linguistics, 26, 467–503. doi:10.1515/cog-2015-0004
  • Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125, 49–63. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.008
  • Sayama, H. (2015). Introduction to the modelling and analysis of complex systems. New York, NY: Open SUNY Textbooks.
  • Schiffer, A. M., Waszak, F., & Yeung, N. (2015). The role of prediction and outcomes in adaptive cognitive control. Journal of Physiology Paris, 109, 38–52. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2015.02.001
  • van Schijndel, M., Exley, A., & Schuler, W. (2013). A model of language processing as hierarchic sequential prediction. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5, 522–540. doi:10.1111/tops.12034
  • Siepel, A., Bejerano, G., Pedersen, J. S., Hinrichs, A. S., Hou, M., Rosenbloom, K., … Haussler, D. (2005). Evolutionarily conserved elements in vertebrate, insect, worm, and yeast genomes. Genome Research, 15, 1034–1050. doi:10.1101/gr.3715005
  • Silvey, C., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2015). Word meanings evolve to selectively preserve distinctions on salient dimensions. Cognitive Science, 39, 212–226. doi:10.1111/cogs.12150
  • Skeide, M. A., Brauer, J., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Brain functional and structural predictors of language performance. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv042
  • Smith, K. (2004). The evolution of vocabulary. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 228, 127–142. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2003.12.016
  • Solé, R. V., Corominas-Murtra, B., Valverdie, S., & Steels, L. (2011). Language networks: Their structure, function, and evolution. Complexity, 15, 20–26. doi:10.1002/cplx.2030
  • Solway, A., Diuk, C., Córdova, N., Yee, D., Barto, A. G., Niv, Y., & Botvinick, M. M. (2014). Optimal behavioral hierarchy. PLoS Computational Biology, 10, e1003779. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003779
  • Steels, L. (2011). Modelling the cultural evolution of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 8, 339–356. doi:10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.014
  • Syrett, K., Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R. (2014). Slowly but surely: Adverbs support verb learning in 2-year-olds. Language Learning and Development, 10, 263–278. doi:10.1080/15475441.2013.840493
  • Szathmáry, E., & Smith, J. (1995). The major evolutionary transitions. Nature, 374, 227–232.10.1038/374227a0
  • Tamariz, M., Ellison, T. M., Barr, D. J., & Fay, N. (2014). Cultural selection drives the evolution of human communication systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20140488. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0488
  • Tang, C. Y., Eaves, E. L., Ng, J. C., Carpenter, D. M., Maia, X., Schroeder, D. H., … Haier, R. J. (2010). Brain networks for working memory and factors of intelligence assessed in males and females with fMRI and DTI. Intelligence, 38, 293–303. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.03.003
  • Tenenbaum, J. B., Kemp, C., Griffiths, T. L., & Goodman, N. D. (2011). How to grow a mind: Statistics, structure, and abstraction. Science, 331, 1279–1285. doi:10.1126/science.119278, 10.1016/j.intell.2010.03.003
  • Tomasello, M. (2008). The usage based theory of language acquisition. In E. I. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 69–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Traugott, E. C. (2014). Toward a constructional framework for research on language change. Cognitive Linguistic Studies, 1, 3–21. doi:10.1075/cogls.1.1.01tra
  • Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2007). Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In G. Gaskell (ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Tyler, A., & Ortega, L. (2016). Usage-based approaches to language and language learning: An introduction to the special issue. Language and Cognition, 8, 335–345. doi: 10.1017/langcog.2016.15
  • Ullman, M. T. (2016). The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiological model of language leaning, knowledge and use. In G. Hickok & S. A. Small (Eds.), The neurobiology of language (pp. 1139–1159). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Vagharchakian, L., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Pallier, C., & Dehaene, S. (2012). A temporal bottleneck in the language comprehension network. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 9089–9102. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5685-11.2012
  • van Dam, W. O., & Desai, R. H. (2016). The semantics of syntax: The grounding of transitive and intransitive constructions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 693–709. doi:10.1162/jocon_a_00926
  • van den Bos, E., Christiansen, M. H., & Misyak, J. B. (2012). Statistical learning of probabilistic nonadjacent dependencies by multiple-cue integration. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 507–520. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.008
  • van Dijk, T. A. (2006). Discourse, context and cognition. Discourse Studies, 8, 159–177. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.08.02710.1177/461445606059565
  • Vuong, L. C., Meyer, A. S., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Concurrent statistical learning of adjacent and nonadjacent dependencies. Language Learning, 66, 8–30. doi:10.1111/lang.12137
  • Weber, K., Lau, E. F., Stillerman, B., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2016). The Yin and the Yang of prediction: An fMRI study of semantic predictive processing. PLoS One, 11, e0148637. doi:10.1371/journal
  • Wedel, A. (2012). Lexical contrast maintenance and the organization of sublexical contrast systems. Language and Cognition, 4, 319–355. doi:10.1515/langcog-2012-0018
  • Winters, J., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2015). Languages adapt to their contextual niche. Language and Cognition, 7, 415–449. doi:10.1017/langcog.2014.35
  • Wojcik, E. H., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). The ontogeny of lexical networks: Toddlers encode the relationship among referents when learning novel words. Psychological Science, 24, 1898–1905. doi:10.1177/0956797613478198
  • Wray, A. (2012). What do we (think we) know about formulaic language? An evaluation of the current state of play. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 231–254. doi:10.1017/S026719051200013X
  • Wray, A. (2013). Formulaic language. Language Teaching, 46, 316–334. doi:10.1017/S0261444813000013
  • Yaveroğlu, O. N., Malod-Dognin, N., Davis, D., Levnajic, Z., Janjic, V., Karapandza, R., … Pržulj, N. (2014). Revealing the hidden language of complex networks. Scientific Reports, 4, 4547. doi:10.1038/srep04547
  • Zaccarella, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2016). The neurobiological nature of syntactic hierarchies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, in press. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.038