2,362
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The relationship between perseverative cognitions and mental health and physical health complaints among college students

ORCID Icon, , , , & ORCID Icon | (Reviewing Editor) show all
Article: 1475878 | Received 06 Mar 2017, Accepted 15 Apr 2018, Published online: 14 Jun 2018

Abstract

College students often experience health problems, but approaches are needed to determine those that are most vulnerable. This study examined whether perseverative cognitions (PCs) represent a common risk factor for poor health. Students (= 489) completed measures of PCs, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology, general mental health, and physical health complaints. Regressions indicated that PCs were associated with higher levels of PTSD symptomatology, poor mental health, and physical health complaints. Structural equation modeling analyses suggested that PTSD symptoms explain, in part, the association between PCs and physical health complaints. Results are discussed in terms of PCs as a risk stratifying factor.

Public Interest Statement

With resources to treat mental health being scarce, especially on college campuses, identifying who might be most vulnerable to poor health is critical for successful intervention. The present study measured the health and well-being of college students through a series of surveys. It found that those who were likely to engage in repetitive negative thinking like rumination and worry were also those most likely to report both mental and physical health complaints. These results suggest that detecting who engages in these forms of negative thinking may be a way to understand who is most at risk and might benefit most from intervention approaches.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Although many factors may contribute to mental and physical health issues, identifying common risk factors (particularly those that are malleable) is important as it suggests the potential to efficiently use interventions to promote health and resilience. This risk stratification approach may be especially relevant for college students for two reasons. First, with access to mental healthcare and other resources being limited for many college students (Gallagher, Citation2008; Hunt & Eisenberg, Citation2010), identifying contexts or situations in which an intervention could have broad-scale effects may be necessary to promote positive health. Second, college has been identified as a period of risk for increased mental and physical health issues, including depression (Furr, Westefeld, McConnell, & Jenkins, Citation2001), anxiety disorders (Kushner & Sher, Citation1993), and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Bernat, Ronfeldt, Calhoun, & Arias, 1998). For example, a population-based study found that more than half of college students reported depression since the beginning of college; of those, approximately 9% had considered committing suicide (Furr et al., Citation2001). High prevalence rates for poor physical health have similarly been reported among university and college students (Lund, Reider, Whiting, & Prichard, Citation2010); in turn, those with poor physical health are more likely to engage in risky health behaviors (Petry & Weinstock, Citation2007), implicating both immediate and downstream negative health consequences. This paper extends prior work to examine whether engagement in perseverative cognitions (PCs) is a common risk factor for poor mental health and physical health complaints in college students.

PCs are cognitive representations of stress that are repeatedly activated, typically negative, and unconstructive (Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer, Citation2006; Smyth, Zawadzki, & Gerin, Citation2013). In other words, PCs represent a pattern of thought that repetitively focuses on the same content such that it does not aid in meaning making or understanding and blocks mental capacity for those other thoughts (e.g., problem solving or reflection). They are conceptually similar to other constructs such as rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Friedrickson, Citation1993), worry (Borkovec, Robinson, Pruzinsky, & DePree, Citation1983), unconstructive repetitive thoughts (Watkins, Citation2008), as well as intrusive thoughts and cognitions (Rachman, Citation1981). Among college students, an example of PCs might include repeatedly thinking about one’s potential performance on an upcoming exam and the potential for failure without reflecting on ways to improve and prepare. These negative and repetitive thoughts can result in worse cognitive and emotional states, including more depressed mood and pessimism (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation1995), angrier mood (Rusting & Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation1998), more anxiety and difficulty concentrating (McLaughlin, Borkovec, & Sibrava, Citation2007), and less effective problem solving (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation1995). In turn, these dysphoric emotional and cognitive states may predispose individuals to various poor mental and physical health outcomes (Rawson, Bloomer, & Kendall, Citation1994).

The first aim of this study was to examine whether those who engage in more PCs also report a range of poor health outcomes. Prior work has begun to link PCs to a variety of poor health outcomes, including depressed mood and clinical depression (Hong, Citation2007; Thomsen, Mehlsen, Christensen, & Zachariae, Citation2003), poor sleep quality (Thomsen et al., Citation2003; Zawadzki, Graham, & Gerin, Citation2013), reduced quality of life (Dupont, Bower, Stanton, & Ganz, Citation2014), worse self-reported physical health (Petrie et al., Citation2001; Thomsen et al., Citation2003), and worse cardiovascular function (Hogan & Linden, Citation2004). These individual studies are consistent with recent meta-analyses demonstrating the negative effects of PCs across a range of somatic health complaints (e.g., higher heart rate, blood pressure, and cortisol levels; Ottaviani, Thayer, Verkuil, Longiro, Medea, Couyoumdijiam, & Brosschot, Citation2015).

Yet, much of this work has focused on a limited range of health outcomes in any particularly sample, such as concentrating on a single measure of mental health (see exceptions: Segerstrom, Roach, Evans, Schipper, & Darville, Citation2010; Thomsen et al., Citation2003). The current study extends this work by examining both mental and physical health indicators within the same sample to determine if there is a potential generalized effect of PCs (i.e., PCs are a common risk factor). Also, most prior studies did not examine health outcomes particularly relevant to college students. College is rated as a period of high stress (Abouserie, Citation1994), with unique developmental challenges as students encounter new experiences and develop their own identity (Cooper, Healy, & Simpson, Citation1994; Hu & Kuh, Citation2003). We focused on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms (Boals & Hathaway, Citation2010; Frazier et al., Citation2009), general self-reported mental health (Eisenberg, Hunt, Speer, & Zivin, Citation2011), and general reported physical health complaints (El Ansari, Oskrochi, & Stock, Citation2013), which are highly prevalent among college students and are each predictive of worse future health and mortality (Chida & Steptoe, Citation2008; Xu & Roberts, Citation2010). For example, rating one’s health as fair or poor predicts later life mortality (Idler & Benyamini, Citation1997), and almost 8% of college students rate their health in this way (Zullig, Reger-Nash, & Valois, Citation2012).

On a more exploratory basis, the second aim of the study was to test whether PTSD symptoms in part explain the associations between PCs and mental and physical health. Although past theory and research provides support for the model we test, we approach these analyses as exploratory given that we have cross-sectional data that cannot directly speak to causal processes. For example, in a prospective longitudinal study of road traffic accident survivors who were children, those who engaged in rumination more (among other factors) were those more likely to have and maintain PTSD symptoms three and six months after the accident (Ehlers, Mayou, & Bryant, Citation2003). A similar set of findings were found for adults (Ehring, Frank, & Ehlers, 2008), and has generalized to other populations (e.g., assault survivors: Kleim, Ehlers, & Glucksman, 2007; survivors of 9/11: DeRoma et al., Citation2003). In turn, PTSD is related to worse mental (Miller, Wolf, Martin, Kaloupek, & Keane, Citation2008; Zatzick et al., Citation2014) and physical health (Norris, Slone, Baker, & Murphy, Citation2006; Zoellner, Goodwin, & Foa, Citation2000). In other words, those who have a tendency to engage in PCs may have a cognitive vulnerability to the development of PTSD (Elwood, Hahn, Olatunji, & Williams, Citation2009), and then it is these PTSD symptoms that lead to worse mental and physical health. Yet, to our knowledge, the potential for PTSD to account for the effects of PCs on mental and physical health has not been explored in college students.

The present study examines the associations between PCs and mental health and physical health complaints, and explores whether PTSD accounts in part for the association between PCs and health in college students. Importantly, we measured PCs using multiple scales that more reliably assesses this construct. Most past work has often relied on single indicators of PCs that may be more limited in capturing the construct and result in idiosyncratic associations. For example, the Ruminative Response Scale is often used to test rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, Citation1991), yet this scale has considerable overlap with measures of depression (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation2003). Likewise, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire is used to assess worry (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, Citation1990), yet this scale may particularly capture anxiety (Brown, Antony, & Barlow, Citation1992; Davey, Citation1993). Thus, using multiple scales and more broadly capturing the construct of PC allows for a comprehensive and reliable assessment of PCs and testing of its relation to mental and physical health. As such, we predict that those who report greater PCs will also report worse levels of mental health and physical health complaints compared to those reporting lesser PCs (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore, we predict that PTSD will explain, in part, the association between PCs and mental and physical health (Hypothesis 2).

1. Method

1.1. Participants

In exchange for partial course credit, 491 students participated in the study from an undergraduate psychology subject pool. Two participants were removed for having significant amounts of missing data. The resulting sample was comprised of 371 women (75.9%) and 118 men (24.1%) aged 18–62 (= 20.94, SD = 4.54). Participants identified primarily as Caucasian (53.8%), followed by African American (15.3%), Hispanic (11.5%), Asian (9.6%), Native American (0.6%), and other (9.0%); one participant did not report race or ethnicity. This sample’s demographics were largely consistent with the body of students from which it was drawn.

1.2. Materials

1.2.1. Perseverative cognition scales

Based on theory and preliminary work (Smyth & Sliwinski, Citation2010), two scales were used to capture PCs. First, participants completed the worry and punishment subscales of the Thought Control Questionnaire (TCQ; Wells & Davies, Citation1994). Each subscale has six items that assess the extent to which participants have intrusive thoughts (worry subscale; e.g., “I think more about the minor problems I have.”) or punish oneself for having the thought (punishment subscale; e.g., “I get angry at myself for having the thought.”). Due to a programming error, one item from the worry subscale was omitted (“I worry about more minor things instead.”). Participants responded using a 1 (never) to 4 (almost always) scale. All items were averaged together such that higher numbers indicated greater PCs (Cronbach’s α = .86).

Second, participants also completed the 15-item White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI; Wegner & Zanakos, Citation1994). The WBSI measures inclinations to suppress intrusive thoughts (e.g., “I always try to put problems out of my mind.”). Participants rated items using a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) scale. All items were averaged together such that higher numbers indicated greater PCs (Cronbach’s α = .93).

1.2.2. Mental and physical health scales

Participants responded to three scales measuring mental and physical health. We began with a measure of PTSD symptoms. Participants were asked to “…nominate a negative event from your life that is currently most distressing to you” and then completed the PTSD Checklist (PCL; Weathers, Huska, & Keane, Citation1991) in reference to this event. The PCL is a 17-item questionnaire designed to measure the existence of PTSD symptoms according to the diagnostic criteria for PTSD found in the DSM-IV. Participants responded to the specific version of the PCL, in which they indicated how much in the past seven days they have been bothered by particular symptoms associated with the event they nominated (e.g., “Having difficulty concentrating?”). Participants responded using a 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) scale. Items were summed together to create an overall score such that higher numbers indicated greater PTSD symptomatology (Cronbach’s α = .91). Scores of 50 or higher are considered indicative of PTSD (Terhakopian, Sinaii, Engel, Schnurr, & Hoge, Citation2008).

The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12; Goldberg & Blackwell, Citation1970) is a self-report measure designed to screen for psychiatric disorders such as depression. Using a 1 (not at all) to 4 (much more than usual) scale, participants indicated how much they experienced poor health (e.g., “Have you felt constantly under strain?”). Items were averaged together such that higher numbers indicated worse mental health (Cronbach’s α = .88).

The 54-item Pennebaker Inventory of Limbic Languidness (PILL; Pennebaker, Citation1982) was used to assess the experience of common health symptoms and bodily sensations. Participants indicated how often they have experienced symptoms like insomnia or dizziness using a 1 (have never or almost never experienced) to 5 (more than once every week) scale. Items were averaged together such that higher numbers indicated greater physical health complaints (Cronbach’s α = .95).

1.3. Procedure

All materials and procedures were approved by the relevant institutional review board. Participants completed all measures online, which served as a screener for a larger study. After completing an informed consent notice, participants completed all measures in the following fixed order: PCL, PILL, GHQ-12, TCQ, and WBSI.

1.4. Analytic plan

We first explored bivariate correlations between the PCs measures and PTSD symptomatology, mental health, and physical health complaints. We then explored whether there were gender and age differences in PCs, mental health, and physical health complaints. Next, to test Hypothesis 1, we conducted multivariate regression analyses in which the health outcomes (tested in separate models) were regressed on PCs, gender, and age. Due to potential conceptual overlap between the items that make up our measure of PCs and the items of our measure of PTSD symptoms (both measures contain items about intrusive thoughts and avoidance), we examined the correlations between PCs and each of the three subscales of the PCL (intrusive thoughts, avoidance, and arousal). If the correlation sizes are stronger for intrusive thoughts and avoidance than for arousal, this pattern of results would suggest any significant correlations may be due in part to conceptual overlap between our predictor and outcome variables. However, if the correlation sizes do not significantly differ, this pattern of results would suggest limited overlap. To test Hypothesis 2, we conducted a structural equation model in AMOS 24.0 in which a latent variable of PCs—indicated by the TCQ and WBSI—predicted PTSD symptomatology, and both PCs and PTSD symptomatology predicted mental health and physical health complaints. We then tested for indirect effects of PCs on mental health and physical health complaints via PTSD symptomatology by employing a boot-strapping procedure (Cheung & Lau, Citation2008). We specified 5000 resamples and 95% bias corrected confidence intervals, which has the resulting effect of producing a range of indirect effects from each resampled subset of the data so as to test the stability of the parameter estimate (MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, Citation2004). A confidence interval that does not include 0 is significant. To determine if the structural equation model was a good fit, we used standard recommendations (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999): a non-significant chi-square (χ2), a comparative fit index (CFI) and normed fit index (NFI) greater than .95, and a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) less than .05 with a confidence interval from .00 to .08.

2. Results

2.1. Preliminary analyses

All variables were submitted to bivariate correlations. As can be seen in Table , greater engagement in PCs (the TCQ and WBSI were examined separately) was associated with greater PTSD symptomatology and poorer mental health and physical health complaints. Likewise, PTSD symptomatology was positively correlated with mental health and physical symptomatology, as were mental health and physical health complaints with each other.

Table 1. Bivariate correlations of perseverative cognitions, PTSD symptomatology, mental health, and physical health complaints

We then tested if there were gender and age differences in PCs, mental health, and physical health complaints. For gender differences, a set of independent samples t-tests were conducted comparing men and women on all variables. Women, compared to men, reported greater PCs (TCQ: = 1.76, SD = 0.45 vs. = 1.65, SD = 0.48; = .022; WBSI: = 3.68, SD = 0.75 vs. = 3.29, SD = 0.92; < .001), PTSD symptomatology (= 2.44, SD = 0.82 vs. = 2.23, SD = 0.77; = .014), poorer mental health (= 2.25, SD = 0.52 vs. = 2.15, SD = 0.52; = .051), and greater physical health complaints (= 2.24, SD = 0.55 vs. = 1.95, SD = 0.56; < .001). For age differences, we correlated age with PCs, mental health, and physical health complaints. Those who were older reported fewer PCs than those younger (TCQ: = −.13, = .005; WBSI: = −.10, = .033), but no statistically significant differences emerged for mental health nor physical health complaints (rs < .09, ps > .05).

H1: PCs and Mental and Physical Health

To test Hypothesis 1, multivariate regressions were conducted in which PTSD symptomatology, mental health, and physical health complaints (tested in separate models) were regressed on PCs, gender, and age. Because of the medium to high correlation between the two measures of PCs (= .51), the TCQ and WBSI scale means were averaged together to create a single PCs index. We next examined the correlations between PCs and each of the three subscales of the PCL (intrusive thoughts, avoidance, and arousal) to assess for potential overlap in items between our predictor and outcome variables. The resulting correlations were rs = .44, .39, and .44, respectively. None of these correlation sizes significantly differed from each other (all Zs < 1). As can be seen in Table , those who reported greater PCs were those who reported greater PTSD symptomatology, poorer mental health, and greater physical health complaints (ps < .001) even after controlling for gender and age.

H2: Indirect Effects of PCs on Health via PTSD Symptomatology

Table 2. Multivariate regressions testing PCs, gender, and age as predictors of mental and physical health

We then examined whether part of the effect of PCs on mental health and physical health complaints could be explained by PTSD symptomatology. We conducted a structural equation model in which a latent variable of PCs indicated by the TCQ and WBSI predicted PTSD symptomatology, and both PCs and PTSD symptomatology predicted mental health and physical health complaints. The model fit the data well: χ2 (2) = 0.58, = .748, CFI = 1.000, NFI = .999, RMSEA = .00, RMSEA 90% CI = .00 to .06. As can be seen in Figure , and replicating the bivariate correlations, PCs predicted PTSD symptomatology, mental health, and physical health complaints (ps < .001). In turn, PTSD symptomatology predicted physical health complaints (< .001) but not mental health (= .315). The model accounted for 34.1% of the variance in mental health and 34.9% of the variance in physical health complaints. Finally, the indirect effect for PCs on mental health via PTSD symptomatology did not include 0, thus indicating a significant effect (95% CI [.15, .40]); however the indirect effect for physical health complaints did (95% CI [−.08, .18]). In follow-up testing, results were nearly identical when controlling for participant age and sex (results available upon request).

Figure 1. Structural equation model testing for indirect effects of perseverative cognitions on mental health and physical health complaints via PTSD symptomatology. The percentages by mental and physical health indicate the amount of variance of these variables that is explained by the model.

***< .001.

Figure 1. Structural equation model testing for indirect effects of perseverative cognitions on mental health and physical health complaints via PTSD symptomatology. The percentages by mental and physical health indicate the amount of variance of these variables that is explained by the model.***p < .001.

3. Discussion

Past research has demonstrated that the college years represent a period of risk for poor health among students (Bernat et al., 1998; Furr et al., Citation2001; Kushner & Sher, Citation1993; Lund et al., Citation2010; Petry & Weinstock). In line with this work, students who reported more PCs—that is, unwanted repetitive, unconstructive, and negative thoughts (Brosschot et al., Citation2006; Smyth et al., Citation2013)—were those who also reported worse mental health and physical health complaints. Importantly these results suggest a general effect across outcome “types,” including one’s psychological well-being and reports of daily physiological/health functioning. Although more work is needed to test this effect more generally (e.g., using objective biological indicators of poor health), and to examine these relationships longitudinally to test whether PCs predict the onset of poor health (e.g., see Zawadzki et al., Citation2013), these results suggest that PCs may be a general mechanism leading to poor mental and physical health among college and university students.

In addition, we found that PTSD symptomatology explained part of the relationship between PCs and physical health complaints but not mental health. This analysis was tested with cross-sectional data that cannot directly speak to causality; however, these results are consistent with theory and research suggesting such a relationship. For example, a review suggests that the tendency to engage in rumination and have a sensitivity to experiencing anxiety—what could be considered types of PCs—makes one cognitively vulnerable to the development of PTSD (Elwood et al., Citation2009). Indeed, some empirical work bears this out finding that engaging in more ruminative thinking after a traumatic event prospectively predicted the onset and maintenance of PTSD months later (Ehlers et al., Citation2003;  Ehring et al., Citation2008; Kleim et al., Citation2007). Moreover, PTSD is associated with worse mental and physical health (Miller et al., Citation2008; Norris et al., Citation2006; Zatzick et al., Citation2014; Zoellner et al., Citation2000). That said, it is not clear why the expected pattern of results was found for physical health complaints but not mental health. We note that the bivariate correlations demonstrated a significant positive relationship between PTSD symptomatology and mental health. Thus, it appears that PCs account for this relationship when included in the analysis. One possibility is that general mental health issues are highly prevalent among college students (e.g., the prevalence of depressed mood ranges from 10–25%: Mackenzie et al., Citation2011), and thus might appear in more students than only those who have PTSD symptomatology. Future research may benefit from continuing to explore potential differential relationships of PTSD symptomatology with mental and physical health.

Although the need to intervene is apparent, there is less consistent knowledge concerning how to intervene. It would be easy to say that all students experiencing symptoms consistent with PTSD or other psychological health problems should engage in psychotherapy, yet this is likely unrealistic. In light of research suggesting that mental health resources are often limited on campuses ranging from small colleges to large universities (Gallagher, Citation2008; Hunt & Eisenberg, Citation2010), a challenge will be in the development and implementation of readily accessible programs that are able to be administered in groups, and/or that have potential broad-scale effects. Given the findings of the present study, that those who engage in PCs may be vulnerable to a range of worse health problems, targeting those students prone to ruminating, worrying, and otherwise experiencing intrusive cognitions may be a promising start. Ideally one would want to prevent individuals from engaging in PCs, yet this might not be possible and perhaps critical. Instead, it may be most important to take the often common and natural PCs that individuals have and help to make those unconstructive thoughts more constructive. A systematic review concluded that although unconstructive repetitive thoughts were associated with depression, anxiety, and difficulties in physical health, when those thoughts were constructive there was faster recovery from traumatic events and better engagement in adaptive planning and health-promoting behaviors (Watkins, Citation2008). In fact, there is an emerging range of interventions showing potential to influence PCs, including expressive writing (Sloan, Marx, Epstein, & Dobbs, Citation2008) and writing about life goals (Teismann, Het, Grillenberger, Willutzki, & Wolf, Citation2014), meditation (Jain et al., Citation2007), social support seeking (Nolen-Hoeksema & Davis, Citation1999), and cognitive behavioral therapy (Watkins et al., Citation2007). A common reason that all may be effective is their capacity to transform more negative unconstructive thoughts to more constructive forms.

3.1. Limitations

Prior research and theory guided our analyses, yet the data were cross-sectional and cannot directly speak to issues of causality. As such, alternative models may be plausible and potential third-variables could be contributing to the observed pattern of results. For example, trait neuroticism and negative affect have been found to associate with many of the variables in the tested model (e.g., Watson & Pennebaker, Citation1989). Thus, some of the association between PCs and mental health, for example, may have been due to trait neuroticism. Yet, work testing the associations between rumination and depressive symptoms and poor sleep quality among college students continued to find significant relationships after controlling for trait neuroticism (Zawadzki et al., Citation2013). Future work should explore these relationships with longitudinal data to enhance capacity for causal inferencing. In addition, the majority of participants were women, which is common for participants from an undergraduate psychology subject pool. Yet, such a sample may limit generalizability. For example, women tend to report more rumination than men (Broderick, Citation1998; Nolen-Hoeksema & Jackson, Citation2001). Controlling for participant gender in the present study, however, did not influence the results. This suggests that although levels may be different across women and men, which was also true in the present study, the general impact of PCs on health is consistent across gender (Nolen-Hoeksema & Harrell, Citation2002), as well as age and race (Zawadzki, Sliwinski, & Smyth, Citation2018). A strength of the study was the use of multiple scales to robustly measure PCs to avoid potential idiosyncratic relationships between a particular scale and outcome. It should be noted that a similar pattern of results emerged when using each individual scale (results available upon request), which is to be expected given the correlation between the two scales and the strength of loadings for the latent PCs variable. Yet, we were still reliant on self-report to measure a complex cognitive process, as well as the assessments of mental and physical health. Future work should supplement self-report with observational, clinical, and/or objective measures.

4. Conclusion

Many campuses’ health services are faced with the challenge of identifying on whom to specifically intervene, and how, especially in light of limited resources and high level of mental health problems among college students. In addition, it is increasingly recognized that college represents a unique period of development that shapes future outcomes. Notably, the health problems that develop in college are likely to persist later in life or predict more deleterious health consequences (Chida & Steptoe, Citation2008; Xu & Roberts, Citation2010). Although much work is yet to be done on how to intervene, results of the present study suggest that those who engage in PCs are also those who have worse mental and physical health. In other words, PCs may be a risk stratifier for identifying those most in need.

Additional information

Funding

The authors received no direct funding for this research.

Notes on contributors

Matthew J. Zawadzki

Dr. Matthew Zawadzki’s (http://faculty.ucmerced.edu/mzawadzki) research examines how stress contributes to the development and progression of chronic disease, with a focus on cardiovascular disease and its psychosocial predictors. Among potential mechanisms linking stress to health, I examine the interactive effects of thoughts and emotions focusing largely on the role that perseverative cognitions, such as rumination and worry, have in creating, exacerbating, and extending physiological, psychological, and emotional responses to stressful experiences. The results from the present study continue to suggest the need to study how people mentally represent stress in their minds as these negative thought patterns relate to poor mental and physical health.

References

  • Abouserie, R. (1994). Sources and levels of stress in relation to locus of control and self esteem in university students. Educational Psychology, 14, 323–330. doi:10.1080/0144341940140306
  • Boals, A., & Hathaway, L. M. (2010). The importance of the DSM-IV E and F criteria in self-report assessments of PTSD. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 161–166. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.004
  • Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & DePree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary exploration of worry: Some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 9–16. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3
  • Broderick, P. C. (1998). Early adolescent gender differences in the use of ruminative and distracting coping strategies. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 173–191. doi:10.1177/0272431698018002003
  • Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–124. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
  • Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Psychometric properties of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 33–37. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(92)90093-V
  • Cheung, G. W., & Lau, R. S. (2008). Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent variables bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 296–325. doi:10.1177/1094428107300343
  • Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 741–756. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba
  • Cooper, D. L., Healy, M. A., & Simpson, J. (1994). Student development through involvement: Specific changes over time. Journal of College Student Development, 35, 98–102.
  • Davey, G. C. (1993). A comparison of three worry questionnaires. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 51–56. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90042-S
  • DeRoma, V., Saylor, C., Swickert, R., Sinisi, C., Marable, T. B., & Vickery, P. (2003). College students’ PTSD symptoms, coping, and perceived benefits following media exposure to 9/11. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 18, 49–64. doi:10.1300/J035v18n01_05
  • Dupont, A., Bower, J. E., Stanton, A. L., & Ganz, P. A. (2014). Cancer-related intrusive thoughts predict behavioral symptoms following breast cancer treatment. Health Psychology, 33, 155–163. doi:10.1037/a0031131
  • Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (2003). Cognitive predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder in children: Results of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1–10. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00126-7
  • Ehring, T., Frank, S., & Ehlers, A. (2008). The role of rumination and reduced concreteness in the maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 488–506. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9089-7
  • Eisenberg, D., Hunt, J., Speer, N., & Zivin, K. (2011). Mental health service utilization among college students in the United States. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199, 301–308. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182175123
  • El Ansari, W., Oskrochi, R., & Stock, C. (2013). Symptoms and health complaints and their association with perceived stress: Students from seven universities in England, Wales and Northern Ireland. Journal of Public Health, 21, 413–425. doi:10.1007/s10389-013-0571-x
  • Elwood, L. S., Hahn, K. S., Olatunji, B. O., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Cognitive vulnerabilities to the development of PTSD: A review of four vulnerabilities and the proposal of an integrative vulnerability model. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 87–100. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.002
  • Frazier, P., Anders, S., Perera, S., Tomich, P., Tennen, H., Park, C., & Tashiro, T. (2009). Traumatic events among undergraduate students: Prevalence and associated symptoms. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 450–460. doi:10.1037/a0016412
  • Furr, S. R., Westefeld, J. S., McConnell, G. N., & Jenkins, J. M. (2001). Suicide and depression among college students: A decade later. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32, 97–100. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.32.1.97
  • Gallagher, R. P. (2008). National survey of counseling center directors [monograph online]. Alexandria, VA: The International Association of Counseling Services, Inc. Retrieved from http://www.collegecounseling.org/pdf/2008_survey.pdf
  • Goldberg, D. P., & Blackwell, B. (1970). Psychiatric illness in general practice: A detailed study using a new method of case identification. British Medical Journal, 2, 439–443. doi:10.1136/bmj.2.5707.439
  • Hogan, B. E., & Linden, W. (2004). Anger response styles and blood pressure: At least don’t ruminate about It! Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 38–49. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2701_6
  • Hong, R. Y. (2007). Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 277–290. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.006
  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Hu, S., & Kuh, G. D. (2003). Diversity experiences and college student learning and personal development. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 320–334. doi:10.1353/csd.2003.0026
  • Hunt, J., & Eisenberg, D. (2010). Mental health problems and help-seeking behavior among college students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 3–10. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.08.008
  • Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 21–37. doi:10.2307/2955359
  • Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, P. J., Bell, I., & Schwartz, G. E. (2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: Effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 11–21. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm3301_2
  • Kleim, B., Ehlers, A., & Glucksman, E. (2007). Early predictors of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in assault survivors. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1457–1467. doi:10.1017/S0033291707001006
  • Kushner, M. G., & Sher, K. J. (1993). Comorbidity of alcohol and anxiety disorders among college students: Effects of gender and family history of alcoholism. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 543–552. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(93)90070-P
  • Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46, 124–132. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.016
  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176
  • Mackenzie, S., Wiegel, J. R., Mundt, M., Brown, D., Saewyc, E., Heiligenstein, E., … Fleming, M. (2011). Depression and suicide ideation among students accessing campus health care. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 81, 101–107. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01077.x
  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99–128. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4
  • McLaughlin, K. A., Borkovec, T. D., & Sibrava, N. J. (2007). The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy, 38, 23–38. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003
  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6
  • Miller, M. W., Wolf, E. J., Martin, E., Kaloupek, D. G., & Keane, T. M. (2008). Structural equation modeling of associations among combat exposure, PTSD symptom factors, and global assessment of functioning. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 45, 359–369. doi:10.1682/JRRD.2007.06.0085
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Davis, C. G. (1999). Thanks for sharing that”: Ruminators and their social support networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 801–814. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.801
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Harrell, Z. A. (2002). Rumination, depression, and alcohol use: Tests of gender differences. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 391–403. doi:10.1891/jcop.16.4.391.52526
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Jackson, B. (2001). Mediators of the gender difference in rumination. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 37–47. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00005
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.115
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.20
  • Norris, F. H., Slone, L. B., Baker, C. K., & Murphy, A. D. (2006). Early physical health consequences of disaster exposure and acute disaster-related PTSD. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 19, 95–110. doi:10.1080/10615800600652209
  • Ottaviani, C., Thayer, J. F., Verkuil, B., Lonigro, A., Medea, B., Couyoumdjian, A., & Brosschot, J. F. (2015). Physiological concomitants of perseverative cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 231–259. doi:10.1037/bul0000036
  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  • Petrie, K. J., Sivertsen, B., Hysing, M., Broadbent, E., Moss-Morris, R., Eriksen, H. R., & Ursin, H. (2001). Thoroughly modern worries: The relationship of worries about modernity to reported symptoms, health and medical care utilization. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 51, 395–401. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00219-7
  • Petry, N. M., & Weinstock, J. (2007). Internet gambling is common in college students and associated with poor mental health. American Journal on Addictions, 16, 325–330. doi:10.1080/10550490701525673
  • Rachman, S. (1981). Part I. Unwanted intrusive cognitions. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 3, 89–99. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(81)90007-2
  • Rawson, H. E., Bloomer, K., & Kendall, A. (1994). Stress, anxiety, depression, and physical illness in college students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155, 321–330. doi:10.1080/00221325.1994.9914782
  • Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790–803. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.790
  • Segerstrom, S. C., Roach, A. R., Evans, D. R., Schipper, L. J., & Darville, A. K. (2010). The structure and health correlates of trait repetitive thought in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 25, 505–515. doi:10.1037/a0019456
  • Sloan, D. M., Marx, B. P., Epstein, E. M., & Dobbs, J. L. (2008). Expressive writing buffers against maladaptive rumination. Emotion, 8, 302–306. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.302
  • Smyth, J., Zawadzki, M., & Gerin, W. (2013). Stress and disease: A structural and functional analysis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 217–227. doi:10.1111/spco.2013.7.issue-4
  • Smyth, J. M., & Sliwinski, M. (2010). Stinking thinking: Dynamic perseverative cognition as a common pathway linking stress and health. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychosomatic Society, Portland, OR.
  • Teismann, T., Het, S., Grillenberger, M., Willutzki, U., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Writing about life goals: Effects on rumination, mood and the cortisol awakening response. Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 1410–1419. doi:10.1177/1359105313490774
  • Terhakopian, A., Sinaii, N., Engel, C. C., Schnurr, P. P., & Hoge, C. W. (2008). Estimating population prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder: An example using the PTSD checklist. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 290–300. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1573-6598
  • Thomsen, D. K., Mehlsen, M. Y., Christensen, S., & Zachariae, R. (2003). Rumination—Relationship with negative mood and sleep quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1293–1301. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00120-4
  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259. doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561
  • Watkins, E., Scott, J., Wingrove, J., Rimes, K., Bathurst, N., Steiner, H., … Malliaris, Y. (2007). Rumination-focused cognitive behaviour therapy for residual depression: A case series. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2144–2154. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.018
  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163
  • Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234–254. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.234
  • Weather, F. W., Huska, J. A., & Keane, T. M. (1991). PCL-S for DSM-IV. Boston, MA: National Center for PTSD, Behavioral Science Division.
  • Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640. doi:10.1111/jopy.1994.62.issue-4
  • Wells, A., & Davies, M. I. (1994). The thought control questionnaire: A measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 871–878. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90168-6
  • Xu, J., & Roberts, R. E. (2010). The power of positive emotions: It’sa matter of life or death—Subjective well-being and longevity over 28 years in a general population. Health Psychology, 29, 9–19. doi:10.1037/a0016767
  • Zatzick, D. F., Marmar, C. R., Weiss, D. S., Browner, W. S., Metzler, T. J., Golding, J. M., … Wells, K. B. (2014). Posttraumatic stress disorder and functioning and quality of life outcomes in a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1690–1695. doi:10.1176/ajp.154.12.1690
  • Zawadzki, M. J., Graham, J. E., & Gerin, W. (2013). Rumination and anxiety mediate the effect of loneliness on depressed mood and sleep quality in college students. Health Psychology, 32, 212–222. doi:10.1037/a0029007
  • Zawadzki, M. J., Sliwinski, M. J., & Smyth, J. M. (2018). Perseverative cognitions and stress exposure: Comparing relationships with psychological health across a diverse adult sample. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. doi:10.1093/abm/kay009
  • Zoellner, L. A., Goodwin, M. L., & Foa, E. B. (2000). PTSD severity and health perceptions in female victims of sexual assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 635–649. doi:10.1023/A:1007810200460
  • Zullig, K. J., Reger-Nash, B., & Valois, R. F. (2012). Health educator believability and college student self-rated health. Journal of American College Health, 60, 296–302. doi:10.1080/07448481.2011.604368