14,030
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Delayed visual maturation: a visual inattention problem

&
Pages 421-430 | Published online: 09 Jan 2014

Abstract

Delayed visual maturation (DVM) is a term used to describe infants who do not exhibit the ability to fix or follow objects in the environment, but subsequently improves by the age of 6 months without treatment. Infants with these features were first described in the 1920s by Beauvieux, but today there remains little consensus as to the etiology of this phenomenon. This article reviews the existing literature on DVM and summarizes what is known about the classification, clinical characteristics and prognosis of this condition. In this article diagnosing DVM is discussed as well as a summary of other conditions to consider on the differential. Current thought on the etiology of this condition is explored. Ocular examination, electrophysiology tests, imaging results and central eye movements appear normal. These findings imply anterior and posterior sensory pathways as well as cortical motor pathways are intact in these patients. Given that these pathways are intact, a possible explanation for these symptoms includes temporary injury to the attention centers in the developing infant’s brain. Further research is needed to better understand the causes of this condition as well as the long-term sequela.

Medscape: Continuing Medical Education Online

This activity has been planned and implemented in accordance with the Essential Areas and policies of the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education through the joint sponsorship of Medscape, LLC and Expert Reviews Ltd. Medscape, LLC is accredited by the ACCME to provide continuing medical education for physicians.

Medscape, LLC designates this Journal-based CME activity for a maximum of 1 AMA PRA Category 1 Credit(s). Physicians should claim only the credit commensurate with the extent of their participation in the activity.

All other clinicians completing this activity will be issued a certificate of participation. To participate in this journal CME activity: (1) review the learning objectives and author disclosures; (2) study the education content; (3) take the post-test with a 70% minimum passing score and complete the evaluation at www.medscape.org/journals/expertophth; (4) view/print certificate.

Release date: 19 August 2011; Expiration date: 19 August 2012

Learning objectives

Upon completion of this activity, participants will be able to:

  • • Distinguish the clinical presentation of type 1 DVM

  • • Analyze diagnostic testing for DVM

  • • Describe different classifications of DVM

  • • Evaluate potential causes of DVM

Financial & competing interests disclosure

EDITOR

Elisa Manzotti

Editorial Director, Future Science Group, London, UK

Disclosure: Elisa Manzotti has disclosed no relevant financial relationships.

CME AUTHOR

Charles P Vega, MD

Associate Professor, Residency Director, Department of Family Medicine, University of California, Irvine, USA.

Disclosure: Charles P Vega has disclosed no relevant financial relationships.

AUTHORS AND CREDENTIALS

Patrick A Coady, MD, MBA

Smith–Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA, USA.

Disclosure: Patrick A Coady has disclosed no relevant financial relationships.

William V Good, MD

Smith–Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, CA, USA.

Disclosure: William V Good has disclosed no relevant financial relationships.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of acuity development in delayed visual maturation.

In delayed visual maturation (DVM) type I, visual acuity improves rapidly and returns to normal (line 1 connecting to line 4). In DVM type III, visual acuity also improves rapidly but returns to near-normal (line 1 connecting to line 3). In DVM types II and IV visual acuity is late to improve and remains impaired (line 2).

Reproduced with permission from Citation[42].

Figure 1. Schematic representation of acuity development in delayed visual maturation.In delayed visual maturation (DVM) type I, visual acuity improves rapidly and returns to normal (line 1 connecting to line 4). In DVM type III, visual acuity also improves rapidly but returns to near-normal (line 1 connecting to line 3). In DVM types II and IV visual acuity is late to improve and remains impaired (line 2).Reproduced with permission from Citation[42].

An infant who presents with isolated delayed visual maturation (DVM) is cause for great anxiety for parents. The child appears blind, unable to focus attention, fixate or follow any part of the visual world. Ocular and neurologic examination of the child is completely normal and visual behavior resolves rapidly (sometimes overnight) by 4–6 months of age. Symptoms resolve without treatment and often, no cause for the initial presentation is found.

The pathophysiology of isolated DVM is unknown and represents a spectrum disease with several possible entities. Cases of DVM were first described in other terms by Beauvieux and referred to as ‘temporary visual inattention’ Citation[1,2]. The author described an anomalous, gray appearance of the optic nerve in these cases and postulated that this was due to a problem with myelination. He called this phenomenon ‘pseudo-atrophie optique’ and ‘dysgenesie myelinique’ Citation[2]. Doggart et al. also described DVM infants and implicated a delay in myelination of the optic nerve Citation[3].

Illingworth first introduced the term ‘delayed visual maturation’ to describe the phenomenon Citation[4]. He described two infants who initially took no notice of their surroundings, but gained signs of vision by 5–6 months of age. Both infants were not developmentally delayed, with the exception of one infant with delayed walking at the age of 2 years. Also of note, this infant had spasmus nutans (transient nystagmus) with slight residual signs at the end of follow-up.

True isolated DVM is a relatively rare phenomenon, but the prevalence is unknown. The cause is not well understood and there is no clear evidence that any primary visual system is delayed. Some authors believe that this entity represents a primary visual inattention disorder Citation[5].

Classification

The term DVM is usually used to refer to isolated (type I) DVM in the clinical setting. For the purposes of this article, we will describe the clinical characteristics and classification of all types of DVM and then return to a discussion of isolated DVM.

Beauvieux first classified the entity now known as DVM into two groups. The first group had DVM as the only anomaly with recovery rapid and complete. The second group included ocular problems such as strabismus, high refractive error as well as mental retardation, resulting in slower and often incomplete recovery Citation[2]. Next, DVM was classified into three groups: group I: simple visual developmental delay; group II: visual developmental delay associated with mental retardation and group III: visual developmental delay associated with ocular abnormalities Citation[6].

Group I was expanded by Fielder et al. to include two subgroups: group A: DVM as the sole abnormality and group B: DVM with ‘concurrent systemic illness or history of perinatal problems’ Citation[7]. The most common perinatal abnormalities presented in group IB included birth asphyxia and ‘jitters/fits’. In addition, group IA was further divided into IAi: infants with poor vision on presentation to the doctor and IAii: infants that improved by the time they presented to the doctor and were diagnosed by history Citation[7].

The classification system was again modified to its current form to separate group III into type III, which includes congenital nystagmus and albinism and type IV, which includes other severe ocular disorders Citation[8]. See Box 1 for summary of classification.

Clinical characteristics: type I DVM

Type I DVM is characterized by infants that do not have the ability to fix or follow objects in the environment. They are usually brought in for medical care when their symptoms do not resolve by 2–4 months, although no consensus exists on when diagnosis should be made. The diagnosis is confirmed after symptoms resolve and other visual disorders are excluded. DVM is characterized by a rapid resolution of symptoms by age 6 months.

Infants with DVM have no detectable ocular or neurologic abnormalities to explain their symptoms. Strabismus is often present during the phase of visual impairment, most commonly divergent and less frequently convergent Citation[9]. Pupils are normal and ocular examination is usually normal. However, as described by several authors, the optic disc has been noted to be gray compared with usual Citation[2,10].

Electrophysiology results

Several studies have shown normal electroretinogram (ERG) results in patients with DVM, including two studies that had eight and nine patients respectively Citation[11,12]. By contrast, there is little consensus regarding visual evoked potential (VEP) results for this patient population. An early study of four children with normal ERGs showed initially impaired visual evoked responses, but normal responses by the age of 4 months Citation[13]. Harel et al. described three infants with DVM whose VEPs were of prolonged latency, but repeated VEPs showed improvement in latency and waveforms after the age of 6 months Citation[14].

Other authors such as Lambert et al. demonstrated no significant difference in the latency or amplitude in VEPs when compared with age-matched controls Citation[12]. Another study showed abnormal flashed-evoked potentials in DVM infants that improved with resolution of symptoms Citation[15]. More recently, a case series of two DVM infants described normal thresholds for vernier and grating acuity Citation[16]. It is unclear whether DVM infants have abnormal VEPs, normal VEPs compared with age-matched controls or perhaps the entity may present with a spectrum of VEP results. Whatever the case, the appearance of an abnormal VEP before age 4 months is unlikely to dictate the infant’s prognosis Citation[13].

In another study, grating acuity was estimated using Teller Acuity Cards and showed normal results in infants with DVM Citation[17]. However, a previous study showed 26 infants with type I–IV DVM with abnormal grating acuity Citation[9].

Imaging results

Neuroimaging studies of patients with DVM have thus far indicated no significant difference between these patients and controls. An MRI study investigating 14 infants with type I DVM, evaluated by T1-weighted evaluation of optic nerves and chiasm showed no difference between these infants and controls Citation[18]. However, a delayed myelination pattern was found in three cases. Mercuri et al. showed one infant with DVM type Ib with diffuse white matter changes and bilateral lentiform nucleus lesions Citation[19].

Central eye movement characteristics

The defining characteristic of DVM is the inability to fix and follow. However, other central eye movement systems appear to be intact. Vestibular-ocular response (VOR) is observed in infants with DVM. Two out of eight infants in one study showed a completely normal VOR, while the rest only showed a lack of fast phase Citation[11]. This seems to be consistent the normal pattern in preterm infants as displayed in other studies Citation[20]. Only one out of nine infants in one study demonstrated a lack of VOR, as well as optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) that persisted Citation[12].

Saccadic movements are not present in the absence of fixing and following eye movements, but as this function returns, saccadic movements follow. In a study of infants with DVM, Harris et al. showed that all six had normal binocular full-field OKN, despite the inability to fix and follow Citation[21]. This suggests that the oculomotor centers for generating smooth eye movements and brainstem saccadic function are intact Citation[21]. However, immature monocular OKN is found in infants with DVM compared with controls and some authors suggest a cortical deficiency as one possible mechanism Citation[22].

Transient nystagmus has been reported in the literature in infants with DVM, although this seems to represent the minority of cases Citation[23]. A study of 16 infants with DVM showed three with transient nystagmus Citation[24]. Conversely, in a study of 11 infants with transient nystagmus, only two also had DVM, demonstrating that DVM possibly explains a minority of transient nystagmus cases.

Associated developmental problems

Delayed visual maturation represents a spectrum of disease and there are multiple instances where infants with presumed isolated DVM also had other developmental delays. As previously mentioned, one of the first cases Illingworth described with DVM had a transient delay in motor functioning that resolved Citation[4]. Of the 16 children described by Cole et al., two had delays in speech development Citation[24]. Hoyt et al. described eight children with DVM, with seven described as having general “delays in motor development” Citation[11]. Six out of these eight children were premature or small for gestational age. Of the nine DVM infants in the Lambert et al. study, four were described as having mild delay and one with severe delay Citation[12].

The diagnosis of DVM type I implies that infants with this disorder will regain visual function and have no future problems, but these early case reports indicate other consequences. A retrospective study by Hoyt of children diagnosed with isolated delayed visual maturation from 1981 to 2001 revealed a high level of neurodevelopmental and educational problems Citation[5]. This study included 98 infants with at least 3 years of follow-up. A total of 93 of those patients had excellent visual acuity (20/20), but 22 had learning disabilities and 11 had a diagnosis of attention deficit disorder Citation[5]. This suggests that this disease does not have a completely benign outcome.

It may be possible that some infants originally categorized as having isolated DVM actually have other associated delays or even DVM type II. Longer follow-up often reveals this link.

Type II DVM

Type II DVM is characterized as infants with attention and fixation deficit that additionally have neurological and or learning abnormalities. Resolution of visual symptoms is slower and often incomplete compared with DVM type I Citation[9]. Many infants in this classification have mental retardation associated with seizure disorder or infantile spasm related to ‘asphyxia, hypoglycemia, hypocalcemia, tuberous sclerosis, Aicardi’s syndrome and so on Citation[25]. Visual symptoms often improve when seizures are treated. Other causes of mental retardation not associated with seizures less often exhibit DVM Citation[25].

We suggest that infants in this category often exhibit signs of DVM owing to damage (often hypoxic) to the extrageniculostriate visual system that predominates in early infants Citation[22]. Symptoms may subsequently resolve when the geniculostriate system develops.

Full-field OKN are probably impaired in this population and neuroimaging is probably abnormal. Nystagmus may be present and could be due to another neurologic cause Citation[22].

Type III DVM

Infants in this category have associated congenital nystagmus and albinism. Their vision starts to improve later than infants with DVM type I and improve to low-normal levels Citation[9]. Often, nystagmus is absent during the period of DVM and can appear uniplanar with slow large oscillations at the time of visual recovery Citation[26]. Other presentations of DVM type III include chaotic eye movements with poor fixation and large-amplitude pendular nystagmus Citation[26].

Type IV DVM

Severe ocular disorders included in the DVM type IV classification include: retinal dystrophies, optic nerve hypoplasia and macular coloboma Citation[22].

Prognosis & resolution of symptoms

Resolution of vision appears to take progressively longer when comparing higher group I with group II and III. Tresidder et al. demonstrated varying rates of return to normal vision based on the classification of the patient Citation[9]. Under the classification system used by Tresidder et al., visual improvement commenced the following week (mean): group IA – 15 weeks, group IB – 15.7 weeks, group II – 45.7 weeks and group III – 21 weeks. This study also demonstrated that all infants in group I regained normal vision, none in group II regained normal acuity and all infants in group III attained low-normal visual acuity.

Fielder et al. described similar time of improvement for type I, II and III DVM and also noted a wide range of improvement in type IV infants (5–46 months of age) Citation[8]. A study of 11 children with type IV DVM (five Leber’s congenital amaurosis, four optic nerve hypoplasia and two macular colobomata) showed three that remained blind (two Leber’s congenital amaurosis and one optic nerve hypoplasia) Citation[27]. No clinical characteristics were identified as predictive.

As shown in , DVM type I (lines 1 and 4) and type III (lines 1 and 3) show quick improvement of symptoms. Whereas type I improves to normal, type III does not exhibit completely normal visual acuity later in childhood. Type II and IV show variable improvement depending on the severity of the underlying dysfunction. In general, signs of improvement start late and may reach a significantly impaired, but functional level (line 2). However, there may be further deterioration in later life given the nature of the disorder.

Isolated DVM-associated conditions

Delayed visual maturation is associated with other conditions such as prematurity, perinatal problems and possibly subtle structural brain damage. It is possible that in isolated DVM, these insults are subtle enough to only cause visual symptoms and not present with neurologic abnormalities.

In addition to perinatal problems such as hypoxia or periventricular hemorrhage, there may be other maternal factors that could contribute to DVM. In a study of 13 cocaine-exposed infants four had isolated DVM, suggesting a role of dopamine in the development of visual attention Citation[28].

Another study of Karen refugees in Thailand noted abnormality in tracking and focusing compared with British control infants Citation[29]. These abnormalities corrected by the age of 4–5 months and low levels of micronutrients such as vitamin A, vitamin C, folate and thiamine were noted in this population. Although no cause for early visual deficiency in this population was found, these authors postulated that early differences are possibly the result of nutritional deficits during critical periods of gestation Citation[29].

Isolated DVM diagnostic approach

Evaluation of an infant lacking visual focus and attention requires careful ophthalmologic examination including anterior segment, lens and fundus. DVM is a diagnosis of exclusion and requires ruling out other possible causes. The initial examination should therefore include refraction. A case report of two infants with visual inattention showed immediate recovery after optical correction Citation[30]. Spectacle correction was performed on the two infants at 5 and 4 months and they had -4 diopters (D) oculus dexter (right eye; OD), -5 D oculus sinister (left eye; OS) and -9 D oculus uterque (both eyes; OU) myopic error. In addition to careful ophthalmologic examination, a careful perinatal history must be taken, including any possible hypoxic events or any maternal substance abuse during pregnancy.

Early in the evaluation, it should be noted whether spontaneous nystagmus is present. No nystagmus indicates a normal anterior visual pathway. If nystagmus is present, however, careful examination of the eye as well as neuroimaging should aid in the diagnosis. The next diagnostic algorithm indicated by some authors indicates testing VOR and watching for saccadic eye movements Citation[5]. If the fast motion movement is absent, then this suggests a deficiency in the saccadic mechanism. Global saccadic palsy or ocular motor apraxia should be considered Citation[5]. If VOR is normal, then cortical visual impairment versus DVM should be considered and neuroimaging is necessary to resolve the difference.

Although this diagnostic algorithm is useful, it is also worthy to note that exceptions can occur along the way. For example, cases of transient nystagmus are noted in some infants with DVM Citation[31]. In addition, another study showed six out of eight DVM infants with a lack of fast-phase VOR, which could be normal depending on the level of prematurity Citation[11]. Other testing that could help with the diagnosis include ERG (a normal result rules out retinal dystrophy), OKN and Teller acuity.

One of the hallmarks of DVM is that the child’s visual symptoms improve by 5–6 months of age. In the absence of any abnormality by eye examination, ERG, VOR, OKN and no history of perinatal complication, it is prudent to continue with neuroimaging if symptoms do not show signs of resolution by 5–6 months of age. MRI characterization of the lesion establishes the diagnosis and prognosis for the patient Citation[32].

Approach to parents

On initial presentation, parents need to understand that evaluation of children with DVM involves a multidisciplinary approach between the ophthalmologist and pediatrician. If there are no other abnormalities noted, (isolated DVM), then the parents need to be reassured that prognosis is good for their child’s visual acuity. Symptoms should resolve by 6 months of age with no increased risk of visual abnormalities in the future. However, not much is known about the long-term developmental sequelae in these patients and regular check-ups with a pediatrician are recommended.

Isolated DVM differential diagnosis

The differential diagnosis (see Box 2) for an infant that is apparently blind is initially broad. However, after appropriate ophthalmologic evaluation and testing, this list can be narrowed down to a few options. If the infant has a normal ophthalmologic examination, including refraction, has no nystagmus and evidence of saccadic eye movements, DVM or cortical visual impairment (CVI) are the main choices. CVI is often associated with some history of perinatal event such as perinatal hypoxia, cerebral vascular accident, meningitis and acquired hypoxia. In one study, seizures were associated with 53% of cases and cerebral palsy with 26% Citation[32].

However, if no history of obvious perinatal injury or no other neurologic symptoms are present, MRI may be needed to differentiate between CVI and DVM.

Etiology of isolated DVM

Initially, the main hypothesis regarding the etiology of DVM involved a possible delay in myelination of the optic nerve. This dates from the early descriptions of DVM infants with gray optic nerves Citation[2–4]. During a fetus’ and infant’s maturation, myelination occurs from the brain and proceeds distally towards the eye. A postmortem examination of 18 infants by light and electron microscopy showed that myelin in the optic nerve near the globe started to appear at term and became fully developed by the age of 7 months Citation[33].

Although the continued myelination of the optic nerve during early infancy may theoretically cause some visual symptoms, it is unclear whether this occurs in practice. Although little consensus has been established regarding VEPs and infants with DVM, several studies show normal responses when adjusted for age Citation[12,16]. Thus, functionally, delay in myelination probably represents an insignificant contribution to the visual innatention symptoms of DVM. In addition, MRI studies of the optic chiasm and nerve suggest no difference between DVM patients and controls Citation[18].

When considering the possible etiology of DVM, we should consider all the pathways that need to be working in order to achieve normal vision with fixation and follow. The sensory, motor and attentional mechanisms all need be functioning in order to provide a normal response to a visual stimulus by the infant.

The afferent visual system involves the sensory visual experience from the retina, optic nerve and visual pathways throughout the brain. There is little evidence of malfunction in this system. Patients with DVM show normal results on ERG and fundoscopic examination is age appropriate, making macular immaturity an unlikely cause. Additionally, neuroimaging of the optic nerve are within control groups Citation[18]. The general absence of nystagmus shows a lack of anterior visual pathway disease. More importantly, normal results for tests such as vernier acuity and VEP, suggest an afferent pathway that is intact from start to finish Citation[16].

The efferent (motor) visual system also appears intact as the brainstem saccadic generator is functional when measured. Studies have demonstrated normal age-related OKN in infants with DVM Citation[21]. In addition, other studies have demonstrated normal VOR with fast phase, consistent with age-specific patterns for infants Citation[11,12,20]. Thus, despite ability to fix and follow, these infants display normal motor function when measured by other proxies.

Many authors have attempted to explain a possible mechanism for DVM based on their results. Lambert et al.’s review of infants with DVM led to the elimination of myelination of the visual system and macular immaturity on the basis of VEPs and ERGs Citation[12]. In addition, they believed that the striate cortex was not involved, based on the normal pattern VEPs in the literature. Based on this, they suggested visual inattention originating from immaturity of the visual association area Citation[12].

Tresidder et al. speculated that the cause for DVM may be a defect in subcortical extrageniculostriate visual system in infants Citation[9]. This system may be damaged in some infants owing to hypoxia or some other insult, but visual symptoms may improve as the cortical system develops. The timing of visual recovery in infants with DVM may correspond with the development of other functions (binocular vision and smooth eye movements) that are dependent on cortical development Citation[34].

Cocker et al. reported a case of identical twins, one of whom had type Ib DVM and was found to have abnormal acuity card procedure and grating pupillometry Citation[35]. The authors concluded “that although the underlying defect is primarily subcortical, secondarily it delays the emergence of cortically mediated responses Citation[35].”

Other authors believe that evidence of temporo-nasal OKN suggest that the geniculostriate system is intact in infants with DVM Citation[21]. These authors suggest a delay in the development of extrastriate cortical structures involving an abnormality in attentional pathways or figure-ground segregation Citation[21].

There is an abundance of information in the visual world and certain processes are in place to direct and focus our attention to any one part of the world. It has been shown that as a result of our brain’s limited processing capacity, multiple stimuli compete for representation Citation[36]. There are systems in place that bias towards the selection of one stimulus versus another. There is a bottom-up process that is a function of the stimulus’ salience and there is a top-down mechanism that is influenced by attention centers in the brain Citation[37].

The top-down attention centers derive from multiple areas outside of the visual cortex. The anterior system appears to help select the stimulus. Anatomically it includes the frontal eye fields and supplemental eye fields of the frontal cortex as well as globus pallidus, caudate, putamen and perhaps the posterior thalamus Citation[5]. The posterior system also aids in attention and consists of the inferior parietal cortex, superior colliculus and pulvinar Citation[5].

Although the exact pathways behind visual attention are not completely understood, it is possible that the mechanism behind DVM may be related to delayed development of this network in the brain. We need to also consider that DVM has multiple etiologies. However, these authors believe that dysfunction must exist in a common pathway to explain the symptoms.

Expert commentary & conclusion

Delayed visual maturation does not represent a single disease, but probably a symptom common to several neurologic abnormalities. Given the high correlation with prematurity and perinatal events, it is likely that isolated DVM represents a neurologic insult that is subtle and eventually overcome as the infant continues to develop. These insults may manifest in subsequent years as developmental problems, as seen in Hoyt’s retrospective study Citation[5]. Thus, we are dealing with a spectrum of neurologic disease with isolated DVM as the most benign and with the best prognosis.

The approach to the care of an infant with DVM should be interdisciplinary and coordinated among multiple pediatric specialties. Isolated DVM, with resolution of symptoms over time, has little long-term ophthalmologic significance. However, even those infants with isolated DVM should be carefully monitored by pediatricians, looking for any future signs of developmental delay.

Based on much evidence, it appears as though afferent and efferent visual pathways are intact in patients with DVM. It is possible that the initial delay is due to undetectable damage to the visual attention pathway. Thus, as Beauvieux first described, it may be more accurate to describe this phenomenon as a visual inattention rather than delayed visual maturation Citation[1].

Five-year view: need for further study

Hoyt’s prospective study of infants with DVM demonstrated a high incidence of neurodevelopmental delays Citation[5]. However, a prospective study would be valuable in order to help us determine the true incidence of these neurologic developments so that we may better estimate prognosis. In addition, as suggested by Hoyt, a prospective study could identify sequelae of DVM that has not yet been discovered Citation[5].

If we hypothesize that DVM is due to some neurologic insult that is too subtle to identify, we should look for other possible causes for this injury. It has been well established that unconjugated bilirubin is neurotoxic at high levels and can cause such extreme damage in infants such as bilirubin encephalopathy. No studies have correlated the level of bilirubin levels with absence or presence of DVM. There is evidence to suggest that even moderate levels of hyperbilirubinemia can be neurotoxic to the developing infant Citation[38]. An auditory processing syndrome has been described in children with elevated bilirubin Citation[39].

A couple of studies have shown a possible, perhaps transient relationship between bilirubin levels and VEPs Citation[40,41]. Studying a possible link between DVM and elevated bilirubin levels could provide insight on a possible mechanism.

The mechanism and site of damage for DVM is currently unknown, but study involving more advanced imaging modalities such as functional MRI could provide new insight. We could gain further clarity on which parts of the brain are affected. Further investigations such as those described could yet provide incredible insight in our understanding of this complex visual phenomenon.

Table 1. Differential diagnosis of delayed visual maturation.

Box 1. Classification of delayed visual maturation.

Type I

  • • DVM as only abnormality

  • • Normal development, perinatal period

  • • Perinatal problems or systemic illness

Type II

  • • DVM with residual neurologic abnormalities

Type III

  • • DVM with congenital nystagmus and albinism

Type IV

  • • DVM with other severe ocular abnormalities

Box 2. Differential diagnosis for isolated delayed visual maturation.

  • • Cortical visual impairment

  • • Ocular motor apraxia

  • • Saccadic palsy

  • • Hereditary retinal dystrophy (including Leber’s congenital amaurosis)

  • • Global developmental delay

  • • Epilepsy

  • • Optic nerve hypoplasia

  • • Cocaine exposure

  • • Autism

  • • High refractive error

Key issues

  • • Delayed visual maturation (DVM) or visual inattention is a source of much anxiety for families of infants with this entity.

  • • DVM is classified as type I or isolated if it is not associated with any other neurologic or ophthalmologic abnormalities.

  • • Other classifications exist depending on whether neurologic, ophthalmologic or other conditions are also present.

  • • Isolated DVM patients have normal clinical examination, normal imaging and normal or near normal central eye movements.

  • • Prognosis of visual function in infants with isolated DVM is excellent with resolution of symptoms expected by 6 months of age.

  • • Prognosis in patients with other types of DVM are variable.

  • • Isolated DVM merits little long-term ophthalmologic attention, although developmental or other neurologic abnormalities may manifest with time.

  • • The etiology of DVM is unclear, although a delay in or injury to the visual attention centers are implicated.

  • • Further investigated is needed on the long-term sequelae of this entity.

References

  • Beauvieux J. La pseudo-atrophie optique des nouveau-nes. Ann. Ocul. (Paris)163, 881–921 (1926).
  • Beauvieux J. La cecite apparente chex le nouveau-ne la pseudo-atrophie grise du nerf optique. Ann. Ocul. (Paris)7, 241–249 (1947).
  • Doggart JH. Infantile fundus lesions in relation to mental capacity. Br. Med. J.2(5050), 933–935 (1957).
  • Illingworth RS. Delayed visual maturation. Arch. Dis. Childhood36, 407–409 (1961).
  • Hoyt CS. Constenbader lecture. Delayed visual maturation: the apparently blind infant. J. AAPOS8(3), 215–219 (2004).
  • Uemura Y, Oguchi Y, Katsumi O. Visual developmental delay. Ophthalmic Paediatr. Genet.1(1), 49–58 (1981).
  • Fielder AR, Russell-Eggitt IR, Dodd KL, Mellor DH. Delayed visual maturation. Trans. Ophthalmol. Soc. UK104(Pt 6), 653–661 (1985).
  • Fielder AR, Mayer DL. Delayed visual maturation. Semin. Ophthalmol.6(4), 182–193 (1991).
  • Tresidder J, Fielder AR, Nicholson J. Delayed visual maturation: ophthalmic and neurodevelopmental aspects. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.32(10), 872–881 (1990).
  • Law F. The problem of the visually defective infant. Trans. Ophthalmol. Soc. UK80, 3–12 (1961).
  • Hoyt CS, Jastrzebski G, Marg E. Delayed visual maturation in infancy. Br. J. Ophthalmol.67(2), 127–130 (1983).
  • Lambert SR, Kriss A, Taylor D. Delayed visual maturation. A longitudinal clinical and electrophysiological assessment. Ophthalmology96(4), 524–528 (1989).
  • Mellor DH, Fielder AR. Dissociated visual development: electrodiagnostic studies in infants who are ‘slow to see’. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.22(3), 327–335 (1980).
  • Harel S, Holtzman M, Feinsod M. Delayed visual maturation. Arch. Dis. Child58(4), 298–309 (1983).
  • Kraemer M, Sjostrom A. Lack of short-latency-potentials in the VEP reflects immature extra geniculate visual function in delayed visual maturation (DVM). Doc. Ophthalmol.97(2), 189–201 (1998).
  • Good WV, Hou C. Normal vernier acuity in infants with delayed visual maturation. Am. J. Ophthalmol.138(1), 140–142 (2004).
  • Weiss AH, Kelly JP, Phillips JO. The infant who is visually unresponsive on a cortical basis. Ophthalmology108(11), 2076–2087 (2001).
  • Hoyt CS, Good WV. Visual factors in the developmental delayed and neurologic disorders in infants. In: Early Visual Development, Normal and Abnormal. Simons K (Ed.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 505–512 (1993).
  • Mercuri E, Atkinson J, Braddick O et al. The aetiology of delayed visual maturation: short review and personal findings in relation to magnetic resonance imaging. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol.1(1), 31–34 (1997).
  • Eviatar L, Eviatar A, Naray I. Maturation of neurovestibular responses in infants. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.16(4), 435–446 (1974).
  • Harris CM, Kriss A, Shawkat F, Taylor D, Russell-Eggitt I. Delayed visual maturation in infants: a disorder of figure-ground separation? Brain Res. Bull.40(5–6), 365–369 (1996).
  • Russell-Eggitt I, Harris CM, Kriss A. Delayed visual maturation: an update. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.40(2), 130–136 (1998).
  • Bianchi PE, Salati R, Cavallini A, Fazzi E. Transient nystagmus in delayed visual maturation. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.40(4), 263–265 (1998).
  • Cole GF, Hungerford J, Jones RB. Delayed visual maturation. Arch. Dis. Child.59(2), 107–110 (1984).
  • Hoyt CS. Delayed visual maturation. In: Paediatric Ophthalmology. Taylor D (Ed.). Blackwell Science Ltd., Chichester, UK, 29–32 (1997).
  • Reinecke RD, Guo S, Goldstein H. Waveform evolution in infantile nystagmus: an electro-oculo-graphic study of 35 cases. Binocular Vision3, 191–202 (1998).
  • Fielder AR, Fulton AB, Mayer DL. Visual development of infants with severe ocular disorders. Ophthalmology98(8), 1306–1309 (1991).
  • Good WV, Ferriero DM, Golabi M, Kobori JA. Abnormalities of the visual system in infants exposed to cocaine. Ophthalmology99(3), 341–346 (1992).
  • McGready R, Simpson JA, Arunjerdja R et al. Delayed visual maturation in Karen refugee infants. Ann. Trop. Paediatr.23(3), 193–204 (2003).
  • Winges KM, Zarpellon U, Hou C, Good WV. Delayed visual attention caused by high myopic refractive error. Strabismus13(2), 75–77 (2005).
  • Good WV, Hou C, Carden SM. Transient, idiopathic nystagmus in infants. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol.45(5), 304–307 (2003).
  • Huo R, Burden SK, Hoyt CS, Good WV. Chronic cortical visual impairment in children: aetiology, prognosis, and associated neurological deficits. Br. J. Ophthalmol.83(6), 670–675 (1999).
  • Magoon EH, Robb RM. Development of myelin in human optic nerve and tract. A light and electron microscopic study. Arch. Ophthalmol.99(4), 655–659 (1981).
  • No authors listed. Delayed visual maturation. Lancet337(8747), 950–952 (1991).
  • Cocker KD, Moseley MJ, Stirling HF, Fielder AR. Delayed visual maturation: pupillary responses implicate subcortical and cortical visual systems. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.40(3), 160–162 (1998).
  • Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Ann. Rev. Neurosci.23, 315–341 (2000).
  • Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann. Rev. Neurosci.18, 193–222 (1995).
  • Soorani-Lunsing I, Woltil HA, Hadders-Algra M. Are moderate degrees of hyperbilirubinemia in healthy term neonates really safe for the brain? Pediatr. Res.50(6), 701–705 (2001).
  • De Vries LS, Lary S, Whitelaw AG, Dubowitz LM. Relationship of serum bilirubin levels and hearing impairment in newborn infants. Early Hum. Dev.15(5), 269–277 (1987).
  • Chen YJ, Kang WM. Effects of bilirubin on visual evoked potentials in term infants. Eur. J. Pediatr.154(8), 662–666 (1995).
  • Chen WX, Wong V. Visual evoked potentials in neonatal hyperbilirubinemia. J. Child Neurol.21(1), 58–62 (2006).
  • Fielder AR, Dobson V, Moseley MJ, Mayer DL. Preferential looking – clinical lessons. Ophthalmic. Paediatr. Genet.13(2), 101–110 (1992).

Delayed visual maturation: a visual inattention problem

To obtain credit, you should first read the journal article. After reading the article, you should be able to answer the following, related, multiple-choice questions. To complete the questions (with a minimum 70% passing score) and earn continuing medical education (CME) credit, please go to www.medscape.org/journal/expertophth. Credit cannot be obtained for tests completed on paper, although you may use the worksheet below to keep a record of your answers. You must be a registered user on Medscape.org. If you are not registered on Medscape.org, please click on the New Users: Free Registration link on the left hand side of the website to register. Only one answer is correct for each question. Once you successfully answer all post-test questions you will be able to view and/or print your certificate. For questions regarding the content of this activity, contact the accredited provider, [email protected]. For technical assistance, contact [email protected]. American Medical Association’s Physician’s Recognition Award (AMA PRA) credits are accepted in the US as evidence of participation in CME activities. For further information on this award, please refer to http://www.ama-assn.org/ama/pub/category/2922.html. The AMA has determined that physicians not licensed in the US who participate in this CME activity are eligible for AMA PRA Category 1 Credits™. Through agreements that the AMA has made with agencies in some countries, AMA PRA credit may be acceptable as evidence of participation in CME activities. If you are not licensed in the US, please complete the questions online, print the AMA PRA CME credit certificate and present it to your national medical association for review.

Activity Evaluation: Where 1 is strongly disagree and 5 is strongly agree

1. Your patient is a 3-month-old girl who was referred to you by the patient’s pediatrician. Her parents noticed that she does not fix her gaze on them or attempt to follow objects with her eyes. The patient has a normal birth history and appears to be otherwise healthy. What should you consider regarding the clinical presentation of type I delayed visual maturation (DVM) at this time?

  • A Strabismus is often present during the stage of visual impairment

  • B Convergent strabismus is more common than divergent strabismus

  • C Patients rarely present with symptoms prior to 1 year of age

  • D DVM type I rarely resolves prior to 1 year of age

2. Which of the following signs is most helpful in diagnosing this child with DVM?

  • A Normal visual evoked potential

  • B Abnormalities in the optic nerve at the optic chiasm

  • C The inability of the child to fix her gaze and follow movement

  • D Defects in multiple central eye movement systems

3. The patient’s parents are concerned about other problems that might be related to their daughter’s DVM. What should you consider regarding type II DVM?

  • A Visual symptoms are often slower to resolve in type II DVM compared with type I DVM

  • B Seizure control in type II DVM control has no effect on visual symptoms

  • C Infants with type II DVM have congenital nystagmus and albinism

  • D Type II DVM is associated with macular coloboma

4. The parents want to know why their daughter is affected by DVM. Which of the following defects appears most likely to explain the pathology of DVM?

  • A Delay in myelination of the optic nerve

  • B Defects in visual attention centers within the cerebral cortex

  • C Defects in the afferent visual system

  • D Defects in the efferent (motor) visual system

Notes

DVM: Delayed visual maturation.

Data taken from Citation[6–8].

See .

Data taken from Citation[5,22].

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.