684
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Key Paper Evaluation

The protective role of brain size in Alzheimer’s disease

Pages 1799-1801 | Published online: 09 Jan 2014

Abstract

Evaluation of: Perneczky R, Wagenpfeil S, Lunetta KL et al. Head circumference, atrophy, and cognition: implications for brain reserve in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 75, 137–142 (2010).

The brain reserve hypothesis suggests that larger brain size is associated with a greater ability to tolerate pathological damage before showing any cognitive decline. This theory has been used to explain why many patients with Alzheimer’s disease pathology are cognitively normal before death. However, the literature concerning the brain reserve hypothesis is mixed with evidence both for and against this theory. Perneczky and colleagues tested the theory by assessing whether premorbid brain size, measured using head circumference, alters the relationship between brain atrophy and cognitive decline in 270 Alzheimer’s disease patients. They found that head circumference was associated with a reduced impact of atrophy on cognitive performance. Hence, for a given degree of atrophy, cognitive performance was better in patients with a larger head circumference. Therefore, these findings support the brain reserve hypothesis. This article will discuss the brain reserve concept and potential limitations and significance of this study.

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia, affecting over 26 million people worldwide Citation[1]. Patients with AD typically show early episodic memory loss with progressive involvement of other cognitive domains and additional functional impairment. Progressive atrophy can be observed on MRI, particularly involving the hippocampi and temporoparietal neocortex, and shows a tight correlation with the degree of cognitive impairment. However, a definitive diagnosis of AD can only be made at autopsy and is based on the finding of specific pathological hallmarks, namely the presence of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. The majority of patients with a clinical diagnosis of AD do indeed have a pathological diagnosis of AD. Studies have found that the clinical diagnosis of AD has a high sensitivity and specificity for correctly predicting the presence of AD pathology at autopsy.

However, it has been demonstrated that approximately 20% of patients with pathological AD are actually clinically normal before death Citation[2]. It has been suggested that these patients do not manifest clinical changes of the disease because they are in some way protected against cognitive decline. In other words, they can tolerate more pathological damage before they become clinically effected than patients that do display clinical AD during life. One early study found that patients with AD pathology that were clinically normal had a greater brain weight and more large neurons than age-matched individuals without AD pathology Citation[2]. This finding has led to the theory that a larger premorbid brain volume, with more neurons and synaptic connections, could provide protection against cognitive decline, often referred to as the ‘brain reserve (BR) hypothesis’. Surrogates for premorbid brain volume include head circumference and measurements of intracranial volume on MRI. Evidence in support of this hypothesis comes from studies that have found that head size is related to the risk of developing AD and correlates with cognitive impairment and age at onset in patients with AD Citation[3]. However, there is disagreement in the field, with other studies not finding evidence for the BR hypothesis Citation[4,5]. It is likely that inconsistencies in the field relate to differences in study methodology, cohort characteristics, sample size and in how well other confounding factors are accounted for in the analyses.

Therefore, the concept of BR is, at present, still contentious. However, it is an important issue to resolve and has implications for the understanding of the relationship between pathology, cognition and atrophy in AD and in particular, to increase the understanding of factors that can help prevent or slow cognitive decline. The study under review aimed to test the BR hypothesis by assessing whether premorbid brain size alters the relationship between brain atrophy and cognitive decline in a large group of patients with AD Citation[6]. This indirect approach to assessing the relationship between premorbid brain size and cognition furthers previous studies that tend to investigate the direct relationship between these features.

Summary of methods & results

The authors assessed 270 patients with a clinical diagnosis of AD that were recruited from the Multi-Institutional Research in Alzheimer’s Genetic Epidemiology (MIRAGE) study. These patients had been recruited from 14 sites around the USA, one site in Canada and two sites in Europe. The degree of cerebral atrophy present on MRI was assessed using a visual rating scale and premorbid brain size was measured using head circumference. The authors used linear regression to examine the association between cerebral atrophy and performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), adjusting for head circumference and other demographic and clinical variables, including age, duration of AD symptoms, gender, apolipoprotein E genotype, diabetes mellitus, hypertension, major depression and ethnicity. They also assessed whether head circumference modified the effect of atrophy on the MMSE score by including an interaction term in the model.

Higher MMSE scores were associated with less severe atrophy (r = -0.23; p < 0.01), shorter duration of symptoms (r = -0.15; p < 0.05) and younger age (r = -0.43; p = 0.05), although only atrophy and age were significant predictors of the MMSE score in a linear regression model. No correlation was observed between the MMSE score and head circumference (r = -0.12; p = 0.77). Head circumference was significantly associated with a reduced impact of atrophy on the MMSE score (p = 0.04; β = -0.21; standard error = 0.01), such that the effects of atrophy on the MMSE score were more pronounced in patients with a smaller head circumference.

The authors concluded that their data supports the BR hypothesis and suggests that larger brains protect against the clinical manifestations of the disease. Since the majority of brain growth occurs in the early years of life, the authors also suggest that improvement of prenatal and early life conditions could increase BR in the population and help protect against the effects of AD.

Discussion

Therefore, this study suggests that although head circumference is not directly associated with cognitive function in patients with AD, it does modify the relationship between atrophy and cognition. Hence, for a given degree of atrophy, cognitive performance was better in patients with a larger head size. These findings are supported by the results from a previous study that assessed the relationship between AD pathology and cognitive function in men and women Citation[7]. They found that AD pathology was more likely to be clinically expressed as dementia in women than men. Since it is well known that men have a larger head size than women, and atrophy provides an excellent marker of the degree of pathology Citation[8], these results could also be interpreted to support the suggestion that head size modifies the relationship between atrophy and cognition. These findings would, therefore, suggest that cognition is not a good marker of underlying disease pathology in AD, since the degree of cognitive decline will be dependent upon premorbid brain size. Imaging measures of atrophy may instead provide more accurate markers of pathological stage, although it is possible that the degree of atrophy may also be affected by some sort of reserve mechanism.

While the current study accounted for a large number of variables that could have confounding effects on the analysis, one demographic variable that was conspicuous in its absence from the statistical model was education. Years of education could be an important confounder since it would have a direct influence on performance on cognitive testing, and some have suggested that it may correlate with head size Citation[4]. However, perhaps more importantly, it has been hypothesized that individuals with greater cognitive reserve (CR) can withstand a greater degree of AD pathology without becoming demented, owing to a greater efficiency in engaging brain networks or use of alternative functional strategies Citation[9]. Individuals with high CR perform better on cognitive tasks, and hence it takes longer before they are given a diagnosis of AD. Education or occupational attainment are commonly used as proxies of CR, with studies finding that a greater pathological burden is required in order to show an effect on cognition in subjects with more education Citation[10]. Hence, one might expect the relationship between atrophy and cognition in AD to be modified by the level of education, as well as head circumference. However, the relationship or interaction between these two reserve mechanisms is unclear and was not addressed in this study.

One other potential limitation of the current study was the relatively crude methods that were employed to measure premorbid brain size and atrophy. Head circumference is likely to prove inferior to detailed measurements of intracranial volume in assessing premorbid brain size. The degree of atrophy was measured using a simple visual assessment, with no reported intra-rater reliability. It is unclear whether the visual assessment provided equal weight to hippocampal and temporoparietal regions, or was predominantly biased towards the degree of hippocampal atrophy, since it is easier to assess. This method of measurement could have introduced error into the statistical analysis and also did not allow the assessment of whether head size modifies the relationship between atrophy of specific regions of the brain and cognitive function. Automated techniques that would allow more accurate assessment of regional structures are available. Measurements of the degree of white matter hyperintensities could also be performed on MRI. Directly accounting for the presence of white matter hyperintensities could be important since this will influence the relationship between cognition and AD pathology Citation[11].

Expert commentary & five-year view

Despite the methodological limitations of the study, the article does provide solid evidence for the BR hypothesis. However, the concept of BR is still somewhat difficult to fully accept since if it were true then one would expect to observe clinical and prevalence differences in AD across both genders and ethnic groups, which are known to differ according to height and head size. This evidence is currently weak at best. In addition, the literature is still dogged by contradictory publications, with almost as many publications disproving the hypothesis as confirming it. It will, therefore, be important for these types of findings to be reproduced and validated in other AD cohorts, and in population-based cohorts. Ultimately, confirmation of the hypothesis will require studies that assess the degree of pathology in the brain, rather than using atrophy as a surrogate marker of pathology. Amyloid PET imaging could provide an invaluable way of assessing the degree of pathology, at least the degree of amyloid pathology, during life. Studies have already begun to use amyloid PET markers to assess the CR hypothesis in AD Citation[12].

In addition, other imaging modalities are likely to play an important role in assessing other aspects and mechanisms of brain reserve, such as premorbid levels of metabolic activity measured using fluorodeoxyglucose F18-PET or functional connectivity measured using resting-state functional MRI in the brain, which could be as, or more important than simple brain size. These modalities may also allow the assessment of compensatory mechanisms and connectivity in the brain, and perhaps even the degree of plasticity in the brain. A better understanding of these mechanisms will be crucial for the development of future treatments for AD, including strategies that could potentially aim to modify and increase brain or cognitive reserve through environmental enrichment or targeted neurogenesis Citation[13].

Key issues

  • • The brain reserve hypothesis suggests that larger brain size is associated with a greater ability to tolerate pathological damage before showing any cognitive decline.

  • • This concept has been extensively studied in Alzheimer’s disease with mixed findings.

  • • Perneczky et al. assessed whether head circumference alters the relationship between brain atrophy and cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. They found that head circumference was associated with a reduced impact of atrophy on cognitive performance.

  • • These findings provide convincing support for the brain reserve hypothesis.

  • • The study has the advantage that they accounted for many demographic, genetic and clinical features that could confound cognitive performance, although future studies will need to relate these findings to pathology, levels of education and use more sensitive measures of regional brain atrophy.

Financial & competing interests disclosure

Jennifer Whitwell is supported by NIH grants R21-AG38736 (PI), R01-DC010367 (Co-I), R01-AG037491 (Co-I) and the Dana Foundation (Co-I). The author has no other relevant affiliations or financial involvement with any organization or entity with a financial interest in or financial conflict with the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript apart from those disclosed.

No writing assistance was utilized in the production of this manuscript.

References

  • Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM. Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer‘s Dement.3, 186–191 (2007).
  • Katzman R, Terry R, DeTeresa R et al. Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Ann. Neurol.23, 138–144 (1998).
  • Wolf H, Julin P, Gertz HJ, Winblad B, Wahlund LO. Intracranial volume in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: evidence for brain reserve? Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry19, 995–1007 (2004).
  • Espinosa PS, Kryscio RJ, Mendiondo MS et al. Alzheimer’s disease and head circumference. J. Alzheimers Dis.9, 77–80 (2006).
  • Christensen H, Batterham PJ, Mackinnon AJ, Anstey KJ, Wen W, Sachdev PS. Education, atrophy, and cognitive change in an epidemiological sample in early old age. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry17, 218–226 (2009).
  • Perneczky R, Wagenpfeil S, Lunetta KL et al. Head circumference, atrophy, and cognition: implications for brain reserve in Alzheimer disease. Neurology75, 137–142 (2010).
  • Barnes LL, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Schneider JA, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Sex differences in the clinical manifestations of Alzheimer disease pathology. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry62, 685–691 (2005).
  • Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Murray ME et al. MRI correlates of neurofibrillary tangle pathology at autopsy: a voxel-based morphometry study. Neurology71, 743–749 (2008).
  • Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia47, 2015–2028 (2009).
  • Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Cairns NJ, Morris JC. Interaction of neuritic plaques and education predicts dementia. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord.22, 188–193 (2008).
  • Brickman AM, Siedlecki KL, Muraskin J et al. White matter hyperintensities and cognition: testing the reserve hypothesis. Neurobiol. Aging DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.013 (2009) (Epub ahead of print).
  • Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA et al. Cognition, reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann. Neurol.67, 353–364 (2010).
  • Sachdev PS, Valenzuela M. Brain and cognitive reserve. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry17, 175–178 (2009).

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.