868
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial

Etiological pathways for developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: shared or discrete?

, &
Pages 491-494 | Published online: 09 Jan 2014

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and developmental coordination disorder (DCD) are two of the most common developmental disorders of childhood. Furthermore, children with a diagnosis of one developmental disorder are highly likely to be diagnosed with at least one other developmental disorder Citation[1,2]. However, there are gaps in our understanding of the underlying mechanisms of these disorders and why they so frequently co-occur.

The rate of co-occurrence of DCD and ADHD has been found to be close to 50% Citation[3]. Gillberg has published widely on deficits in attention, motor control and perception Citation[4]. Piek and colleagues found an association between fine motor ability, in particular, and inattentive symptomatology in ADHD Citation[5]. Fliers and colleagues proposed that ADHD with motor deficits might be a subtype of ADHD Citation[6].

It is not surprising that a number of studies support a shared etiology for ADHD and DCD. Kaplan and colleagues argued that there is an association between developmental disorders and atypical brain development, which they proposed as the reason why they so commonly co-occur Citation[7]. Fliers and colleagues argued a shared etiology for ADHD with co-occurring motor problems, which they attributed to genetic and/or shared environment effects Citation[6].

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – Fourth Edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) Citation[8] and the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Health-Related Problems Tenth Revision, Second Edition (ICD-10) Citation[9] classify psychopathology on the basis of phenomenology, rather than etiology Citation[10]. DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 have similar classification categories for ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder, respectively. For a diagnosis of ADHD, inattention and/or hyperactivity must be more frequently displayed and be more severe than is typical for the person’s developmental stage, and symptoms must be present before the age of 7 years; there must be impairment in two or more settings, and clinically significant impairment must be present in at least one of the following – academic, occupational or social functioning. DSM-IV-TR has separate diagnostic criteria for symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity, with ADHD diagnosable as three subtypes: inattentive, hyperactive/impulsive and combined. It specifies that, in ADHD, symptomotology involving movement is limited to increased motor activity, and notes that children with ADHD may knock things over or fall because of their impulsivity and distractibility, rather than due to motor impairment. However, Pitcher and colleagues provided evidence to suggest that motor deficits in ADHD were a result of poor motor ability Citation[11].

Developmental coordination disorder and specific developmental disorder of motor function are also described in DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10, respectively. DCD is defined as having motor coordination significantly lower than expected for the child’s age and intellect, which interferes significantly with activities of daily living. Both classification systems state that the movement disorder must not be due to a medical condition, such as cerebral palsy.

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and DCD have some factors in common:

  • • Both disorders have similar prevalence rates. ADHD is generally thought to affect some 3–10% of school age children Citation[12], although some estimate the prevalence rate to be as low as 1% Citation[13]. DCD affects approximately 6% of children aged 5–11 years Citation[8]. In their study of monozygotic (MZ) twins, discordant and concordant for ADHD and DCD, Pearsall-Jones and colleagues found similar prevalence rates of approximately 6% for both disorders Citation[14];

  • • Males are more vulnerable to both ADHD and DCD. Developmental disorders have been found to be more common in males than females Citation[15]. ADHD has been widely reported to affect more males than females Citation[14,16], with estimates in clinical samples varying from 2:1 to 9:1 Citation[8]. Rhee and colleagues concluded that males were more likely to be affected by ADHD than females, as males have a lower genetic threshold for the required liability to express ADHD Citation[16]. Some studies have shown similar numbers of females and males with DCD Citation[14,17–19], although others have reported a higher male incidence Citation[20–22]. Research has indicated that the male developing brain is more vulnerable than the female brain to certain types of brain injury, such as hypoxia Citation[23], which has been associated with DCD Citation[14,18]. This vulnerability has been related to distinct male and female chromosome complements Citation[23];

  • • Some children are said to ‘outgrow’ both disorders. The term ‘developmental’ implies that problems start early in life and interfere with ‘normal’ development Citation[24]. The hallmark of a developmental disorder is deviation and/or delay from normal development, and each developmental disorder is characterized by a specific pattern of developmental delay Citation[25]. Investigating the long-term prognosis of ADHD, Mannuzza and colleagues found that approximately two-thirds to three-quarters of their study participants experienced problematic symptoms of ADHD into early and middle adolescence, indicating that some children appear to ‘outgrow’ ADHD Citation[26]. Some ‘clumsy’ children – especially those with fewer deficits – appear to ‘grow out of it’ Citation[27]; however, it could be questioned whether this is DCD, or rather a developmental delay Citation[28];

  • • Both ADHD and DCD have been associated with other disorders. ADHD and DCD have been linked with mood disorders, such as depression, and children with comorbid ADHD and DCD are at a particular risk for depression Citation[29]. However, Piek and Dyck found that DCD, but not ADHD, was associated with poor visuomotor processing Citation[2].

Levy and colleagues concluded that ADHD is one of the most heritable disorders of childhood Citation[30]. In their study of MZ twins, discordant and concordant for ADHD, Lehn and colleagues noted that with an estimated 60% heritability, environmental factors contributed 40% Citation[31]. They found that low birth weight and delayed motor development were markers for ADHD in infancy. In their study of MZ twins discordant for ADHD, Shaw and colleagues found that in children with ADHD the developmental trajectory of cortical maturation was typical, but reached its peak thickness at a later age than was the case in normally developing children Citation[32]. This was particularly evident in prefrontal regions of the brain, which are important in the control of attention and motor planning. ADHD children who had the DRD4 seven-repeat allele, which is frequently associated with ADHD, had a thinner cortex during childhood in brain regions that are important for attentional control, but to a great extent this resolved during adolescence Citation[33]. This suggests that genes play an important role in brain development in children with developmental ADHD – and possibly explains why some children ‘outgrow’ ADHD. However, environmental factors, such as brain damage caused by intrauterine infection, forceps delivery, hypoxia or anoxia during pregnancy or at birth, often go unrecognized Citation[34,35], and have been linked to secondary ADHD. van ‘t Ent and colleagues in their study of MZ twins, concordant and discordant for ADHD, concluded that MRI findings indicated a genetic etiology of ADHD in concordant high-risk sets, whereas an environmental etiology was implicated in ADHD twins in discordant sets Citation[36]. This suggests that there might be more than one causal pathway to ADHD symptomotology.

There is conflicting evidence for etiology in DCD. Using structural equation modeling, Martin and colleagues found a strong shared additive genetic component for subtypes of DCD and ADHD Citation[37]. On the other hand, DCD has been linked to damage to the developing or immature brain, and it has been proposed that DCD and cerebral palsy may fall on a continuum of motor disorder Citation[14,18,28,38,39], rather than being discrete categories as defined by current classification systems. Jongmans and colleagues examined the relationship between the duration of ‘flares’ (echodensities in the periventricular white matter) in preterm infants and motor competence at 6 years of age Citation[40]. They found that the longer the duration of the flares, the greater the decrease in motor performance, and that premature infants with extensive perceptual-motor difficulties were more likely to have shorter gestational ages and to have shown a brain lesion soon after birth Citation[41]. An association has been found between DCD, perinatal oxygen perfusion problems, low birth weight and gestational ages of less than 37 weeks Citation[14,18,28].

While ADHD and DCD appear to be developmental disorders that overlap in some children, they seem to have somewhat different developmental pathways, and it is likely that multiple causal pathways define both disorders. This could result from different but overlapping genetic and/or environmental influences, differentially affecting susceptibilities of neural pathways in the womb. For instance, Volpe pointed out the highly complex development of the neurological system during prenatal development Citation[42]. Timing of exposure to infections and anoxia is important, and may have long-term cumulative effects, for example, increased sensitization to hypoxia at birth and in later life Citation[43].

The causal pathways of developmental disorders are a combination of genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors that evolve over time. Although subtypes of co-occurring movement and attention disorders may have a shared etiology, in other instances we would argue different etiological pathways for DCD and ADHD. This has important implications for clinical practice, classification systems, such as DSM and ICD, research and policy development, and warrants further exploration of the nature of movement deficits associated with ADHD, and the nature of attention deficits associated with DCD.

Financial & competing interests disclosure

The authors have no relevant affiliations or financial involvement with any organization or entity with a financial interest in or financial conflict with the subject matter or materials discussed in the manuscript. This includes employment, consultancies, honoraria, stock ownership or options, expert testimony, grants or patents received or pending, or royalties.

No writing assistance was utilized in the production of this manuscript.

References

  • Kaplan BJ, Dewey DM, Crawford SG, Wilson BN. The term comorbidity is of questionable value with reference to developmental disorders: data and theory. J. Learn. Disabil.34(6), 555–565 (2001).
  • Piek JP, Dyck MJ. Sensory-motor deficits in children with developmental coordination disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autistic disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci.23(3–4), 475–488 (2004).
  • Barkley R. Attention Deficit Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. Guilford Press, NY, USA (20090).
  • Gillberg C. Deficits in attention, motor control, and perception: a brief review. Arch. Dis. Child.88, 904–910 (2003).
  • Piek JP, Pitcher TM, Hay DA. Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.41(3), 159–165 (20099).
  • Fliers E, Vermeulen S, Rijsdijk F et al. ADHD and poor motor performance from a family genetic perspective.J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry.48(1), 25–34 (2009).
  • Kaplan BJ, Wilson BN, Dewey D et al. DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci.17(4–5), 471–490 (20098).
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV 4th Edition Text Revision. American Psychiatric Publishing Inc., DC, USA (2000).
  • World Health Organisation. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Health Related Problems, Tenth Revision (2nd edition). World Health Organisation, Geneva, Switzerland (2004).
  • Cantwell DP. Classification of child and adolescent psychopathology. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatr.37(1), 3–12 (20096).
  • Pitcher TM, Piek JP, Hay DA. Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.45(8), 525–535 (2003).
  • Wender PH. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adults. Oxford University Press, NY, USA (2000).
  • Swanson J, Castellanos FX, Murias M, LaHoste G, Kennedy J. Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.8(2), 263–271 (20098).
  • Pearsall-Jones JG, Piek JP, Martin NC, Rigoli D, Levy F, Hay DA. A monozygotic twin design to investigate etiological factors for DCD and ADHD.J. Pediatr. Neurol.6(3), 209–219 (2008).
  • Tioseco JA, Aly H, Essers J, Patel K, El-Mohandes AA. Male sex and intraventricular hemorrhage. Pediatr. Crit. Care Med.7(1), 40–44 (2006).
  • Rhee SH, Waldman ID, Hay DA, Levy F. Aetiology of the sex differences in the prevalence of DSM-III-R ADHD: a comparison of two models. In: Attention, Genes and ADHD. Levy F, Hay DA (Eds). Brunner-Routledge, Hove, UK 139–156 (2001).
  • Foulder-Hughes LA, Cooke RW. Developmental co-ordination disorder in preterm children born ≤32 weeks gestational age. Dysprax. Found. Prof. J.1(2), 3–13 (2003).
  • Pearsall-Jones JG, Piek JP, Rigoli D, Martin NC, Levy F. An investigation into aetiological pathways of DCD and ADHD using a monozygotic twin design. Twin Res. Hum. Genet.12(4), 381–391 (2009).
  • Skinner RA, Piek JP. Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescencts. Hum. Mov. Sci.20(1), 73–94 (2001).
  • Kadesjö B, Gillberg C. Developmental coordination disorder in Swedish 7-year-old children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry38(7), 820–828 (20099).
  • Lingam R, Hunt L, Golding J, Jongmans M, Emond A. Prevalence of developmental coordination disorder using the DSM-IV at 7 years of age: a UK population-based study. Pediatrics123(4), E693–E700 (2009).
  • Maeland AF. Identification of children with motor coordination problems. Adapt. Phys. Activ. Q.9(4), 330–342 (20092).
  • Johnston MV, Hagberg H. Sex and the pathogenesis of cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.49, 74–78 (2007).
  • Rispens J, van Yperen TA. How specific are ‘specific developmental disorders’? The relevance of the concept of specific developmental disorders for the classification of childhood developmental disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr.38(3), 351–363 (20097).
  • Dyck MJ, Hay DA, Anderson M, Smith LM, Piek JP, Hallmayer J. Is the discrepancy criterion for describing developmental disorders valid? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr.45(5), 979–995 (2004).
  • Mannuzza S, Klein RG. Long-term prognosis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Clin. N. Am.9(3), 711–726 (2000).
  • Cantell MH, Smyth MM, Ahonen TP. Two distinct pathways for developmental coordination disorder: persistence and resolution. Hum. Mov. Sci.22(4–5), 413–431 (2003).
  • Pearsall-Jones JG, Piek JP, Levy F. Developmental coordination disorder and cerebral palsy: categories or a continuum? Hum. Mov. Sci. (2010) (In press).
  • Piek JP, Rigoli D, Pearsall-Jones JG et al. Depressive symptomatology in child and adolescent twins with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or developmental coordination disorder. Twin Res. Hum. Genet.10(4), 587–596 (2007).
  • Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I. ADHD a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry36(6), 97–103 (20097).
  • Lehn H, Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Heutink P, van Beijsterveldt TC, Boomsma DI. Attention problems and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in discordant and concordant monozygotic twins: evidence of environmental mediators. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry46(1), 83–91 (2007).
  • Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA104(49), 19649–19654 (2007).
  • Shaw P, Gornick M, Lerch J et al. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome, and cortical structure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry64(8), 921–931 (2007).
  • Henderson-Smart DJ. Postnatal consequences of chronic intrauterine compromise. Reprod. Fertil. Dev.7(3), 559–565 (20095).
  • Lou HC. Hypoxic-hemodynamic pathogenesis of brain lesions in the newborn. Brain Dev.16(6), 423–431 (20094).
  • van ‘t Ent D, Lehn H, Derks EM et al. A structural MRI study in monozygotic twins concordant or discordant for attention/hyperactivity problems: evidence for genetic and environmental heterogeneity in the developing brain. Neuroimage35(3), 1004–1020 (2007).
  • Martin NC, Piek JP, Hay DA. DCD and ADHD: a genetic study of their shared aetiology. Hum. Mov. Sci.25(1), 110–124 (2006).
  • Fawke J. Neurological outcomes following preterm birth. Semin. Fetal Neonatal. Med.12(5), 374–382 (2007).
  • Rosenbloom L. Definition and classification of cerebral palsy. Definition, classification and the clinician. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.49(S109), 43 (2007).
  • Jongmans MJ, Mercuri E, Dubowitz LMS, Henderson SE. Perceptual-motor difficulties and their concomitants in six-year-old children born prematurely. Hum. Mov. Sci.17(4–5), 629–653 (20098).
  • Jongmans M, Henderson S, de Vries L, Dubowitz L. Duration of periventricular densities in preterm infants and neurological outcome at 6 years of age. Arch. Dis. Child69(1 Spec. No), 9–13 (20093).
  • Volpe J. Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurol.8(1), 110–124 (2009).
  • Gunn AJ, Bennet L. Timing of injury in the fetus and neonate. Curr. Opin. Obstet. Gynecol.20(2), 175–181 (2008).

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.