2,771
Views
130
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: a systematic review and meta-analysis

, &
Pages 557-577 | Published online: 23 Mar 2017

Abstract

The use of dual-task training paradigm to enhance postural stability in patients with balance impairments is an emerging area of interest. The differential effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability still remain unclear. A systematic review and meta-analysis were conducted to analyze the effects of dual task and training application on static and dynamic postural stability among various population groups. Systematic identification of published literature was performed adhering to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) guidelines, from inception until June 2016, on the online databases Scopus, PEDro, MEDLINE, EMBASE, and SportDiscus. Experimental studies analyzing the effects of dual task and dual-task training on postural stability were extracted, critically appraised using PEDro scale, and then summarized according to modified PEDro level of evidence. Of 1,284 records, 42 studies involving 1,480 participants met the review’s inclusion criteria. Of the studies evaluating the effects of dual-task training on postural stability, 87.5% of the studies reported significant enhancements, whereas 30% of the studies evaluating acute effects of dual tasks on posture reported significant enhancements, 50% reported significant decrements, and 20% reported no effects. Meta-analysis of the pooled studies revealed moderate but significant enhancements of dual-task training in elderly participants (95% CI: 1.16–2.10) and in patients suffering from chronic stroke (−0.22 to 0.86). The adverse effects of complexity of dual tasks on postural stability were also revealed among patients with multiple sclerosis (−0.74 to 0.05). The review also discusses the significance of verbalization in a dual-task setting for increasing cognitive–motor interference. Clinical implications are discussed with respect to practical applications in rehabilitation settings.

Introduction

Postural stability is an integral component of the motor control and coordination process of the body, which is required for preserving steadiness during static and dynamic activities.Citation1 This component relies upon proprioceptive afferents and complex sensorimotor actions.Citation2Citation4 Posture is mediated by both higher “controlled” and lower “automatic” levels of processing,Citation5,Citation6 implying the involvement of basal ganglia–cortical loop for higher level processingCitation7 and brainstem synergies for lower level processing.Citation8 Studies have suggested that any alleviation in conscious-controlled attention toward postural control increases the likelihood of disrupting coordination and stability,Citation9,Citation10 possibly, as a consequence of movement-specific reinvestment.Citation9,Citation11 The theory of reinvestment suggests that directing attention internally to control movement, which is usually automatic, can disrupt its performance.Citation9,Citation10 The theory also suggests that agingCitation12 and neurological diseasesCitation9 are common conditions that increase reinvestment. Seidler et alCitation13 reaffirmed these suggestions and associated physiological changes with aging and injury to loss in gray/white matter within the central nervous system, resulting in differential-reorganized cortical activation. Here, the authors suggested that differential cortical activation within the higher neural centers can affect task prioritization, further allowing increased conscious attention while carrying out cognitive or motor tasks.Citation14

To resolve this issue, distracting dual tasks have been used in several studies.Citation9,Citation15Citation17 A dual task acutely directs the performer’s attention toward an external source of attention (eg, n-back, random letter generation tasks), while performing a primary task. According to the constrained action hypothesis, this attentional change might allow motor systems to function in an automatic manner, resulting in more effective performance.Citation10 Practical applications for enhancing the automation of postural control have been demonstrated in studies evaluating complex motor skills,Citation18,Citation19 postural stability,Citation17 and gait.Citation15

However, with an increase in complexity, a subsequent increase in cognitive processing and eventually cognitive–motor interference has been reported.Citation20Citation23 This increase in central interference adversely affects both cognitive and motor performance.Citation6,Citation23 Studies speculate that inhibition of cognitive and balance ability post dual-task inclusion can be because of the bottleneck and central sharing model theories.Citation21,Citation24 According to these theories, functioning of a neurological pathway mediating both cognitive and motor functions might be affected, when a continuous input as in a dual-task setting is directed with a primary task. This might adversely affect cognitive tasks or stability performance.

Similarly, a complexity-related decrease in cortical reciprocal inhibition in fall-prone population groups (elderly, patients with history of fall, with neurological diseases) has been identified as an important factor to promote postural instability.Citation25,Citation26 Studies suggest reduced gamma-aminobutyric acid B-mediated cortical inhibitionCitation27 and elevated muscular coactivationCitation26,Citation28 to be the primary reasons for this effect. Boisgontier et al,Citation6 Ruffieux et al,Citation26 and Smith et alCitation29 in their review studies concluded that application of dual task on fall-prone population groups results in postural instability and poor cognitive performance. However, minimal effects of cognitive–motor interference have been reported in a few reviews for diseased fall-prone population groups, which theoretically should exhibit poorer cognitive resources as compared to their healthy older counterparts.Citation30,Citation31 Therefore, there is a need to determine specific factors that in terms of complexity for a cognitive or motor task might result in differential effects on stability.

Furthermore, studies have extensively mentioned the beneficial effects of motor,Citation32,Citation33 dual-task training,Citation34Citation36 for enhancing cognitive and motor performance even in fall-prone population groups. Another important determinant that is commonly utilized to enhance stability and cognitive performance is physical exercise.Citation32,Citation33,Citation37 The studies report these training maneuvers to be crucial for smoothening of various cognitive abilities and reducing cognitive–motor interference.Citation38Citation40 Müller and BlischkeCitation41 suggested that the training allows modulation of consciousness-dependent motor activities to be more automatic, thereby reducing dual-task costs. Likewise, Bherer et alCitation42 while reporting the beneficial effects in fall-prone population groups suggested freeing up of cognitive resources meant for monitoring performance to be the primary reason. The change in modulation of motor activity has been suggested to allow automatization by “structural displacement”,Citation43,Citation44 where a shift in the operation control of motor planning and executive control occurs from higher cognitive centers to basic noncognitive centers.Citation45,Citation46 This training maneuver has recently drawn a lot of interest as compared to its older counterpart and speculations persist as to which protocol overlays beneficial effects on postural stability among different population groups.Citation47,Citation48 Recent review studies evaluating the effects of dual-task training in elderlyCitation38,Citation49 and population groups with neurological diseasesCitation50,Citation51 conclusively report the beneficial effects of dual-task training for enhancing cognitive abilities and stability, whereas some review studies report no identifiable benefits.Citation33,Citation52 The studies also mentioned the increased heterogeneity of the training protocols within the studies to cause difficulties in identifying a specific method’s effectiveness. Wang et al,Citation51 for instance, in their meta-analysis reported benefits of dual-task training on static stability, however, with considerable heterogeneity (I2: 88%). This review was an attempt to extend the efforts of the previous reviews and comparatively examine the effects of dual tasks, dual-task training methodologies on the postural stability of healthy and fall-prone population groups. The review also aimed to conduct meta-analysis across homogeneous groups for determining effective methodologies in terms of complexity and training methodologies for dual task and dual-task training scenarios.

Methods

This review was conducted according to the guidelines outlined in Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement.Citation53

Data sources and search strategy

The databases Scopus, PEDro, SportDiscus, EMBASE, and MEDLINE were searched from inception until June 2016. The search was limited to the abovementioned databases due to access regulations of the university. Keywords for search strategy were included using medical subject headings (MeSH). An example of the search strategy for EMBASE database has been provided in Table S1. The inclusion criteria for the studies were as follows: 1) studies that were randomized controlled trials (RCTs), cluster RCTs, and controlled clinical trials (CCTs); 2) measurement of postural stability using highly valid and reliable methods (static and dynamic posturographic analyses, center of pressure, center of gravity analysis, sensory orientation test, Berg balance scale, time up and go test, star excursion balance test, modified star excursion balance test, and active movement extent discrimination apparatus); 3) dual tasks performed during the research were reliable and valid; 4) studies that scored ≥ 4 on the PEDro methodological quality scale; 5) experiments that were conducted on human participants; 6) published in a peer-reviewed academic journal; and 7) articles that were published in English language. Studies evaluating the abovementioned parameters in participants below the age of 18 years were not included, as development of postural control centers has been reported to take place during this developmental phase.Citation54 Studies were excluded if they analyzed postural stability in a sitting position or while using a picture analysis software. All the studies identified during the search were independently screened () for eligibility by a primary researcher and every effort was undertaken to avoid subjective bias.Citation55 Preliminary analysis for selection was performed by analyzing titles and abstracts, and, wherever necessary, the entire text of the article was studied. Where further clarification of the published data was required, the first researcher attempted to contact the respective authors. Bibliographic sections of all the articles were retrieved for evaluations. Citation search for all the included articles was performed using Web of Science. A classification of studies based on their experimental designCitation56 and country of origin was also made (Supplementary material).

Figure 1 Flow diagram illustrating studies for inclusion in the review study (PRISMA flow diagram).

Abbreviation: PRISMA, Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis.
Figure 1 Flow diagram illustrating studies for inclusion in the review study (PRISMA flow diagram).

Data extraction

Upon selection for review, the following data were extracted from each article: author, date of publication, selection criteria, sample size, sample description (gender, age, health status), intervention, dual-task, outcome measures, results, and conclusions. The data were then summarized and tabulated. Furthermore, classification of studies was made based on their experimental application,Citation56 and the population groups were assessed.

Quality and risk of bias assessment

The quality of the studies was assessed using the PEDro methodological quality scale. The scale consists of eleven items addressing external validity, internal validity, and interpretability. The PEDro scale can detect potential bias with fair to good reliabilityCitation57 and is a valid measure of the methodological quality of trials. A blinded rating of the methodological quality of the studies was carried out by the primary reviewer. Ambiguous issues were discussed between reviewers, and consensus was reached. For the included CCTs, a scoring of 9–10, 6–8, and 4–5 was considered to be of “excellent”, “good”, and “fair” quality,Citation58 respectively. Likewise, the level of evidence was suggested to be of level 1a (strong) if more than one RCT (≥ 6), 1b if one RCT (≥ 6), and 2 if one RCT (<6), or CCTs with similar methodological approaches were consistent with the results.Citation58 With differential results among paired groups of studies, the result of the study(s) with higher PEDro score was given more consideration. Inadequate randomization, nonblinding of assessors, no intention to treat analysis, and no measurement of compliance were major threats to biasing.Citation2

Data analysis

This systematic review also included a random-effect meta-analysis approach to develop a better understanding of the incorporated interventions. A narrative synthesis of the findings structured around the intervention, population characteristics, methodological quality (), and the type of outcome is provided. Likewise, summaries of intervention effects for each study were provided in a tabular form (Table S1). A meta-analysis was conducted between pooled studies using comprehensive meta-analysis (CMA V 3.0; Englewood, NJ, USA). Heterogeneity between the studies was assessed using I2 statistics. The data in this review were systematically distributed and for each available variable pooled, dichotomous data were analyzed and forest plots with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are reported. The effect sizes were adjusted and reported as Hedge’s g. Thresholds for interpretation of effect sizes were as follows: a standard mean effect size of 0 means no change, negative effect size means a negative change, mean effect size of <0.1 is considered a small effect, 0.1–0.3 a medium effect and >0.30 a large effect.Citation59,Citation60 Interpretation of heterogeneity via I2 statistics was as follows: 0–40% might not be significant, 30%–60% represents moderate heterogeneity, 50%–90% represents substantial heterogeneity, and 75%–100% represents considerable heterogeneity. Meta-analysis reports including heterogeneity among studies were evaluated to determine the reason of heterogeneity, and the included studies were then pooled separately and analyzed again. The alpha level was set at 95%.

Table 1 Studies analyzing the effects of dual-task training and dual tasks on posture

Results

Characteristics of included studies

The initial search yielded 1,284 studies, which on implementing the inclusion/exclusion criteria were reduced to 42 (). Data from the included studies are summarized in . Of the 42 studies, three were RCTs,Citation34Citation36 and 39 were CCTs. Eight studies evaluated the effects of dual-task training on postural stability.Citation34Citation36,Citation48,Citation61Citation64 Eight studies evaluated the effects of dual tasks on participants suffering from neurological diseases, such as degenerative cerebellar disorder, Parkinson’s disease, and multiple sclerosis.Citation21,Citation65,Citation66 Twenty-six studies evaluated the effects of dual tasks on postural stability among healthy young and/or elderly participants.Citation16,Citation17,Citation20,Citation67Citation89 Within these 26 studies, 14 studies compared the effects between young and elderly participants, eleven studies evaluated only young and one study evaluated only elderly participants.

Participants

Of the included studies, 33 studies incorporated mixed-gender participant groups.Citation16Citation18,Citation20Citation22,Citation36,Citation61Citation67,Citation69Citation78,Citation81Citation84,Citation87,Citation88,Citation90Citation94 Four studies incorporated only female participants,Citation35,Citation79,Citation85,Citation86 and two studies incorporated only male participants.Citation34,Citation89 Three studies did not specify the gender of the included participants.Citation48,Citation61,Citation80 The included studies provided data on 1,480 participants (n=796 females/581 males). Descriptive statistics related to the age (mean ± standard deviation) of the participants were tabulated across the studies. Three studies provided the median age of participants,Citation75,Citation87,Citation88 and five studies mentioned the age of participants in range.Citation16,Citation81,Citation82,Citation85,Citation86

Risk of bias within studies

In order to efficiently reduce the risks of bias, the studies had to score ≥ 4 on PEDro scale to be included in the review. The criteria for research studies to be included in the review were limited to gold standard RCTs, cluster RCTs, and CCTs. The individual scores attained by the studies using the PEDro scale are reported in and S2. The average PEDro score for the 42 included studies was computed to be 4.7 out of 10, indicating fair quality of the overall studies. One study scored 7,Citation36 three studies scored 6,Citation21,Citation35,Citation65 20 studies scored 5,Citation16,Citation17,Citation20,Citation34,Citation69Citation72,Citation74,Citation77,Citation79Citation84,Citation86,Citation92Citation95 and 18 studies scored 4.Citation22,Citation48,Citation61Citation63,Citation66Citation68,Citation73,Citation75,Citation76,Citation78,Citation81,Citation85,Citation87Citation89,Citation91

Risk of bias across studies

Common methodological shortfalls observed in this review were inadequate concealment, intention-to-treat, nonblinding of participants, therapists, and assessors. One study reported blinding of assessors and confirmed intention-to-treat the included participants. Furthermore, only two studies confirmed concealed allocation of subjects.Citation35,Citation36 The authors could not interpret concealed allocation of participants in three studies,Citation65,Citation82,Citation85 and, therefore, no points were awarded to the studies. The overall risk of bias for quality assessment within studies is illustrated in .

Figure 2 Risk of bias across studies.

Figure 2 Risk of bias across studies.

Meta-analysis

The evaluation of research studies via meta-analysis requires strict inclusion criteria to efficiently limit the heterogeneity.Citation96 However, among the pooled group of studies the authors observed unexplained heterogeneity, suggesting incorporation of a random-effect meta-analysis under such conditions. The researchers added that a random-effect meta-analysis involves an assumption that the estimated effects in various studies are unidentical but follow some distribution. Therefore, studies analyzing similar variables were pooled, and a random-effect meta-analysis was conducted across four categories (dual-task training: elderly participants, dual task: multiple sclerosis, young, old). The main reason for not including the statistical approach within the studies was major differences in training duration, assessment methods, age/gender, complexity of dual tasks, and lack of descriptive statistics within the manuscript. The descriptive statistics mentioned within illustrative figures were not included in the study. The authors included ten studies in the meta-analysis which incorporated evaluation of postural stability in participants similar at baseline and were evaluated with similar methodological approaches, but with different dual tasks. The aim of such analysis was to demonstrate the differential effects of complexity of dual tasks on postural stability. Additionally, the reasons for specific studies are mentioned subsequently.

Dual-task training

Eight studies under this category analyzed the effects of dual-task training on postural stability, whereas four studies, in two different categories, were included in the meta-analysis.Citation35,Citation48,Citation61,Citation62 From the nonincluded studies, one study analyzed the effects of dual-task training on subacute stroke patients,Citation34 two studies analyzed the effects on elderly participants,Citation36,Citation63 and one study on younger participants.Citation64 The two studies analyzing the effects of dual-task training on elderly participants considerably differed based on training duration and incorporated dual tasks. Hiyamizu et alCitation36 incorporated Stroop task with a dual-task training duration of two sessions per week for 3 months; however, Li et alCitation63 used an n-back task with a training duration spread over five sessions with a 2-day gap within each session.

Neurological impairment

Eight studies under this category analyzed the effects of dual task on postural stability of participants affected by neurological disorders. Two studies analyzing the effects of dual task on multiple sclerosis were included in the meta-analysis. However, the third study despite having similar variables could not be included as the descriptive statistics were not available in the text and were not obtained even after contacting the corresponding author. Similarly, three other studies analyzing patients affected by Parkinson’s disease could not be included due to lack of descriptive statistics.Citation22,Citation92,Citation93 Only one study analyzed the effects of dual tasks on postural stability of participants affected from degenerative cerebellar disorder.Citation91

Young and elderly participants

Twenty-six studies under this category analyzed the effects of dual task on young and/or elderly participants. Four studies analyzing the effects of dual task on young and elderly participants were included in the meta-analysis.Citation17,Citation20,Citation80,Citation94 Thirteen studies analyzing similar variables in terms of age and dual tasks were not included in the meta-analysis as they did not include descriptive statistics explicitly, but in figures, ie, bar diagrams.Citation16,Citation67,Citation68,Citation72,Citation75,Citation76,Citation79,Citation82,Citation85Citation89 Shumway-Cook and WoollacottCitation83 and Shumway-Cook et alCitation84 evaluated the effects of dual task on postural stability during balance perturbations in participants predisposed to falls, their healthy counterparts, and young participants, while the studies differed in terms of utilized dual tasks. Mak et al,Citation78 on the contrary, included a rather novel aspect of visual feedback during standing and utilized dual tasks in conjugation with this feedback approach. Hwang et alCitation77 also utilized one leg standing as compared to the counterpart studies, which utilized a basic two-legged standing under different conditions. Brauer et alCitation69,Citation70 analyzed postural recovery post balance perturbation with dual tasks among participants predisposed to falls, their healthy counterparts, and young participants with similar dual tasks. Likewise, Brown et alCitation71 also utilized a similar approach and effective comparisons could have been drawn between studies to evaluate the effects of dual task on postural stability. Due to lack of descriptive statistics, and not heterogeneity, the studies could not be included in the analysis.

Outcomes

The results suggest clear evidence for a positive impact of dual-task training for enhancing postural stability among fall-prone elderly population groups and participants affected from stroke. A negative impact of dual tasks was observed in studies evaluating the effects of dual tasks on postural stability among fall-prone population groups affected by neurological disorders and/or with prior history of fall, as compared to their younger healthier counterparts.

Meta-analysis report

Dual-task training

Eight studies evaluated the effects of dual-task training on postural stability.Citation34Citation36,Citation48,Citation61Citation64 One RCTCitation34 and two CCTs evaluated the effects of dual-task training on postural stability in subacute and chronic stroke patients, respectively. Two RCTsCitation35,Citation36 and three CCTs evaluated the effects of dual-task training on elderly and young participants. Significant enhancements in postural stability were reported in one goodCitation35 and six fair-quality studies.Citation34,Citation48,Citation61Citation64 However, one good-quality study reported no significant enhancements in postural stability. A random-effect meta-analysis was conducted across two categories. First, two studies evaluated the effects of fixed and variable priority dual-task training on postural stability among elderly population groups.Citation35,Citation61 A random letter generation task was utilized during the training phase which lasted for a 45-min session, three times a week for 4 weeks. Scores from Berg balance scale were utilized to assess the postural stability. Upon analysis, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: 1.63), and 95% CI (1.16–2.10) was reported in the positive domain, demonstrating a beneficial effect of variable task priority within dual-task training to enhance postural stability (). Heterogeneity tests reported negligible heterogeneity (I2: 20.26%, P<0.01). Moreover, the studies were then reevaluated on the basis of fixed and variable priority dual-task training. In the condition of fixed priority dual-task training, upon analysis, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: 1.42) and 95% CI (0.79–2.05) in the positive domain. Similarly, in the condition of variable priority dual-task training, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: 1.91) and 95% CI (1.19–2.63) in the positive domain. Thereby, demonstrating a beneficial effect of variable priority over fixed priority dual-task training method.

Figure 3 Forest plot illustrating individual studies evaluating the effects of (A) dual-task training with fixed (FP) and variable (VP) priority in elderly participants, (B) dual-task training in elderly participants affected from stroke, (C) dual-task in postural stability of participants affected from multiple sclerosis, (D) dual-task in postural stability of elderly participants, (E) dual-task in postural stability of young participants.

Notes: Adjusted effect sizes; Hedge’s g (boxes), and 95% CI (whiskers) are presented, demonstrating repositioning errors for individual studies. Diamond represents pooled effect sizes and 95% CI. A negative mean difference indicates a favorable outcome for control groups; a positive mean difference indicates a favorable outcome for experimental groups.
Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.
Figure 3 Forest plot illustrating individual studies evaluating the effects of (A) dual-task training with fixed (FP) and variable (VP) priority in elderly participants, (B) dual-task training in elderly participants affected from stroke, (C) dual-task in postural stability of participants affected from multiple sclerosis, (D) dual-task in postural stability of elderly participants, (E) dual-task in postural stability of young participants.

Second, two studies analyzing the effects of dual-task training on postural stability among patients affected from chronic stroke were included in the meta-analysis.Citation48,Citation62 The studies utilized a similar dual-task training duration phase of a 30-min session, three times a week for 8 weeks. Postural stability in the studies was assessed using functional reach test. Upon analysis, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: 0.32), and 95% CI (−0.22 to 0.86) cm was reported in the positive domain, demonstrating a beneficial effect of within dual-task training to enhance postural stability (). Heterogeneity tests reported negligible heterogeneity (I2: 23.2%, P=0.24). The studies according to the PEDro methodological scale computed an average score of 4.8, indicating the average quality of the studies to be fair.

Neurological impairments

Eight studies evaluating the effects of dual-task performance on postural stability among participants affected by neurological disorders, such as cerebellar disorder, Parkinson’s disease,Citation22,Citation92Citation94 and multiple sclerosis,Citation21,Citation65,Citation66,Citation91 were included in the review. Significant enhancements in postural stability were reported in one goodCitation65 and one fair-quality study.Citation94 Additionally, five fair-quality studies reported a significant reduction in postural stability among individuals affected by Parkinson’s disease,Citation22,Citation93,Citation94 multiple sclerosis,Citation66 and degenerative cerebellar disorder.Citation91 One good-quality study reported a reduction in postural stability (not significant) among participants affected by Parkinson’s disease.Citation92 Five studies evaluated the comparative effects between healthy participants and participants affected by neurological disorders,Citation66,Citation91,Citation92,Citation94 but one study evaluated the comparison between participants affected by mild and moderate multiple sclerosis.Citation21 Also, two studies evaluated the inclusion of stable and unstable surfaces for maintaining postural stability while performing a dual task.Citation65,Citation91

A random-effect meta-analysis was conducted across one category, for evaluation of the effects of dual task on multiple sclerosis.Citation21,Citation65 Even though the two included studies conducted the tests using different dual tasks, the methodology and included participants were similar at baseline. The meta-analysis comprehensively demonstrated the differential effects of complexity of dual tasks on postural stability, ie, where on the one hand silent backward counting task improved the postural stability of the participants with multiple sclerosis, on the other hand incorporating word list generation task, incorporated by Boes et al,Citation21 adversely impacted postural stability. Upon analysis, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: −0.34) and 95% CI (−0.74 to 0.05) cm was reported marginally in the negative domain, demonstrating a differential effect of dual-task complexity on the postural stability of participants with multiple sclerosis (). Heterogeneity tests reported considerable heterogeneity (I2: 63.6%, P=0.08). The increased heterogeneity could be attributed to the differential complexity of dual tasks within the studies, which according to Vuillerme and VincentCitation97 might affect the outcome of the primary task. According to PEDro methodological scale, the studies overall scored an average of 4.8, indicating the quality of the studies to be fair.

Young and elderly

Twenty-six studies evaluated the effects of dual-task performance on postural stability among young, elderly, young/elderly, and participants with/without history of falls.Citation16,Citation17,Citation20,Citation67Citation80,Citation82Citation89 Eleven fair-quality studies evaluated the effects of dual tasks on young participants.Citation16,Citation17,Citation20,Citation67,Citation72,Citation75,Citation77,Citation81,Citation82,Citation85,Citation89 Four fair-quality studies reported significant enhancements in postural stability,Citation16,Citation17,Citation77,Citation85 whereas seven fair-quality studies reported significant reduction in postural stability.Citation20,Citation67,Citation72,Citation75,Citation81,Citation82,Citation89

Two fair-quality studies evaluated the effects of dual tasks on elderly participants.Citation70,Citation74 Both the studies reported a significant reduction in postural stability post dual-task intervention.

Thirteen fair-quality studies compared the effects of dual tasks between young and elderly participants.Citation68,Citation69,Citation71,Citation73,Citation76,Citation78Citation80,Citation83,Citation84,Citation86Citation88 Four studies included a comparison between elderly participants with/without history of falls.Citation69,Citation70,Citation83,Citation84 Three studies reported significant enhancements in postural stability among both young and elderly participants.Citation68,Citation76,Citation86 Eight studies reported significant reductions in postural stability of elderly participants as compared to younger participants where enhancements in postural stability were observed.Citation21,Citation73,Citation79,Citation80,Citation83,Citation84,Citation87,Citation88 Two studies reported reduced postural stability (nonsignificant) among elderly participants; however, enhancements were observed in younger counterparts. Similarly, significantly reduced posturasl stability was reported for participants with prior history of fall as compared to their healthy counterparts.Citation69,Citation70,Citation83,Citation84 A random-effect meta-analysis was conducted across two categories for evaluation of the effects of dual task on healthy young participants. The two studies analyzed the postural stability using sensory orientation test; however, differential dual tasks were incorporated in the review.Citation17,Citation20 The methodology and included participants were similar at baseline. The meta-analysis comprehensively demonstrated the differential effects of complexity of dual tasks on postural stability, ie, where on the one hand auditory switch task improved the postural stability of the participants,Citation17 on the other hand, incorporating a complex mental arithmetic task adversely impacted postural stability among young participants. Upon analysis, a trivial effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: −0.02) and 95% CI (−0.45 to 0.41)% was reported marginally in the negative domain, demonstrating a differential effect of dual-task complexity on the postural stability of young participants (). Heterogeneity tests reported considerable heterogeneity (I2: 48.2%, P=0.93), which could possibly be related to the differential complexity of the dual tasks incorporated within the studies. A second random-effect meta-analysis was conducted to evaluate the effects of dual task on elderly participants. The two studies analyzed the postural stability using length of center of pressure path; however, different dual tasks were included in the studies. Despite the complexity, these cognitive tasks demonstrated detrimental effects of dual tasks on postural stability of elderly participants. The methodology and included participants were similar at baseline. Upon analysis, a large effect size was observed (Hedge’s g: −1.15) and 95% CI (−1.67 to −0.63) cm was reported considerably in the negative domain, demonstrating a negative effect of dual-task complexity on the postural stability of elderly participants (). Heterogeneity tests reported negligible heterogeneity (I2: 0%, P>0.01). The studies according to the PEDro methodological scale computed an average score of 4.2, indicating the average quality of the studies to be fair.

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to extend our understanding of the effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on static and dynamic postural stability among healthy and fall-prone population groups. Beneficial effects of dual-task training on postural stability of participants especially with poor balance capabilities were observed in this review. A PEDro 1b level of evidence and random-effect meta-analysis demonstrated the beneficial effects of dual-task training for enhancing postural stability among fall-prone population groups.

The review observed beneficial effects of dual-task training in studies analyzing patients affected from sub-acuteCitation34 and chronic stroke.Citation48,Citation62 The studies reported patients affected from stroke to possess considerable impairments in their cognitive–motor domain. Because of this, altered weight distribution has been reported in stroke patients while maintaining static and dynamic postures.Citation98 However, An et alCitation48 and Kim et alCitation62 performed a dual-task training regime (30-min session, three times a week for 8 weeks) and reported beneficial effects on postural stability even for conditions with visual restriction and/or unstable base when presented with dual tasks. These enhancements were also evident in the meta-analysis where enhancements in functional reach test (Hedge’s g: 0.32) and 95% CI (−0.22 to 0.86) cm were observed. The authors justified the beneficial effects by suggesting prevention of tipping effect.Citation48 This review, however, believes training could have possibly allowed skill acquisition for the cognitive and motor task while making the use of reactive forces, which in turn has been shown to reduce active muscular contraction.Citation99 This can possibly aid in reduction of muscular coactivation and muscle guarding-related decrements in postural stability.Citation6 A meta-analysis conducted by Wang et alCitation51 also reported similar beneficial effects among stroke patients; 95% CI (0.54–5.21).

Furthermore, Silsupadol et alCitation35 and Buragadda et alCitation61 in their respective studies demonstrated a differential aspect of dual-task training with variable task prioritization. Meta-analysis revealed a beneficial effect of 95% CI (1.19–2.63) in variable priority as compared to 95% CI (0.79–2.05) in the fixed priority condition. The authors in their respective studies also reported enhancements in cognitive task performance, rate of learning, and ability to maintain skill level during follow-up period. Silsupadol et alCitation35 interestingly affirmed the enhancements obtained because of dual-task training toward the task integration hypothesis, which states better development of task coordination skills following practicing with two tasks together. Likewise, Kramer et alCitation100 in their study reported similar benefits during variable priority training and suggested that participants under variable priority conditions can learn to coordinate between two tasks during training. The authors speculated that the processing demand needed to perform a task was less when the attention was divided between two tasks. Moreover, the authors also reported a training effect during a 3-month follow-up within the variable priority condition as compared to fixed priority condition.Citation35 According to Shigematsu et al,Citation101 the training phase with a motor component enhances neural functioning and reduces response latency by effectively recruiting postural muscles resulting in improved sensory information processing. The review also identified radiological evidence by Erickson et al,Citation102 which suggested enhanced cerebral hemodynamics in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex within the dual-task training group, and associated this effect with improved performance. In addition, certain centers of the brain associated with dual-task processing showed less activation posttraining, implying reduced processing demands posttraining.Citation102 Some studies have also implied this training maneuver to act as a cognitive therapy for patients with attentional deficits and cognitive impairments.Citation34,Citation52 Furthermore, this review identified dual-task training regimes to also allow benefits in cognitive performance.Citation38,Citation52 According to Hiyamizu et alCitation36 and Wollesen and Voelcker-Rehage,Citation38 enhancements in cognitive performance might lead toward smoothening of cognitive activities while maintaining static and dynamic postures, resulting in preventing falls. The authors of the present review also believe that the enhancements in stability and dual-task performance are highly associated with the findings of Wolpert et alCitation103 and Masters and Maxwell.Citation9 In the present study, the initial phase of learning is suggested to be more cognitively driven as compared to the later stages of learning, which in a dual-task training setting might get more fluent and independent. Our results are in line with previously conducted systematic reviews, where dual-task training has been reported to enhance postural stability and cognitive performance.Citation38,Citation49,Citation50,Citation52 However, this review is the first to reveal beneficial effects of dual-task training in a meta-analysis and a level of evidence analysis.

This review observed detrimental effects of dual tasks on postural stability for the participants with higher predisposition to fall. For instance, complexity-associated reduction in postural stability was reported for patients affected with multiple sclerosisCitation21 and Parkinson’s disease.Citation93 Researchers suggest incorporation of two underlying theories for this detrimental effect, ie, bottleneck and capacity model theories.Citation21,Citation104 Boes et alCitation21 suggested that since the patients with neurological impairments such as multiple sclerosis, stroke, Parkinson’s disease, and elderly participants have cognitive deficits, it is possible that the neurological capacity for these patients would be even less in terms of the aforementioned models. However, the findings of systematic reviews conducted by Learmonth et alCitation31 and Wajda and SosnoffCitation30 concluded minimal effects of cognitive–motor interferences on postural stability for patients with multiple sclerosis and their healthy counterparts. The meta-analysis conducted by Learmonth et alCitation31 revealed a small effect size of −0.11.

Furthermore, explaining the factors causing additional balance discrepancies in patients with parkinsonism, Bohnen et alCitation105 and Andrade et alCitation22 discussed that the dopaminergic and cholinergic pathways play a significant role in stabilizing the control of posture. These pathways play an important role in affecting the prioritization of posture and dual tasks within the central sharing model. The review conducted by Dirnberger and JahanshahiCitation106 supported these results and pointed out the considerable reduction in dopaminergic neuron in posterior putamen, anterior striatum, limbic nuclei, and neocortical extensions.Citation107,Citation108 As mentioned earlier, the basal ganglia–cortical network is involved in managing the “conscious” aspects of postural stability.Citation6 Therefore, it might play an extensive role in causing considerable cognitive–motor interferences to reduce dual-task performance and postural stability and even promote posture “second” strategy.Citation109 Marchese et alCitation92 added that the dual task, ie, calculation, motor sequence of thumb opposition task, might have caused the Parkinson’s patients to shift their attention, further leading to disturbed conscious control and reduced stability. Interestingly, one study analyzing patients with parkinsonism revealed beneficial effects of dual-task application. The authors from the study suggested that the patients constrained their posture for directing attention toward the dual task, which ironically also enhanced their posture. However, the authors of the review argue that factors of complexity within a dual task have played a role for enhancing stability, ie, reduced anterior posterior sway during nonspeech conditions.

Brauer et alCitation69,Citation70 and Shumway-Cook et alCitation84 reported postural stability and its recovery to be poorer among participants with prior history of fall as compared to their healthy counterparts, while performing a dual task (verbal reaction to auditory tone task and sentence completion with visual perception tasks). Radiological evidence by Herath et alCitation110 and Szameitat et alCitation111 reported the involvement of cortical areas along inferior frontal sulcus, middle frontal gyrus, and the intraparietal sulcus while performing auditory and visual reaction dual tasks. Therefore, suggesting that superimposing a dual task over already weak reorganized cortical structures may impart more stress and adversely impact postural stability.Citation14 The findings of the present review are in line with recent review studies,Citation6,Citation26 where poor postural stability was also observed in fall-prone population groups as compared to their healthy younger counterparts.

Interestingly, the review found differential effects of dual tasks in studies evaluating healthy young participants and participants with balance deficits. For instance, researchers such as Vuillerme et al,Citation89 Ramenzoni et al,Citation82 Pellecchia,Citation64 and Lanzarin et alCitation20 reported detrimental effects of dual tasks on young participants; on the other hand Donker et al,Citation16 Bergamin et al,Citation68 Huxhold et al,Citation76 Mak et al,Citation78 Resch et al,Citation17 and Hwang et alCitation77 reported beneficial effects even among fall-prone elderly participants. In addition, beneficial effects of the dual-task application were also observed in participants with multiple sclerosisCitation65 and Parkinson’s disease.Citation94 Conventionally, according to published reports fall-prone population groups experience poor postural stability under the influence of higher information processing constraints. However, this review observed these differential results and suggests an inverse correlation between the complexity of the dual tasks and the postural stability. Researchers suggest that according to the Yerkes–Dodson law a U-shaped relation between cognitive demand and postural sway might reflect the level of arousal associated with dual cognitive task demand,Citation76 thereby suggesting an increase in postural sway with added complexity in a cognitive task.

Jacobi et alCitation91 analyzed the postural stability of ataxic and healthy controls using a verbal working memory task. The authors reported less center of pressure sway with reduced dual-task complexity for the ataxic group during a sensory orientation test. According to the authors, the involvement of cerebellum in both cognitive and motor tasks can result in increased interference,Citation112 thereby affecting dual-task and postural performance. Also, the role of cerebellum has been reported especially during the performance of dual tasks while maintaining executive control including working memory, language, and visuospatial information.Citation113 Radiological evidence also demonstrates increased BOLD (blood oxygen level dependent) response in the cerebellar vermis and anterior lobe while simultaneous performance of cognitive–motor tasks.Citation114 This review also observed articulation as a major factor for complexity in terms of a dual task, yielding differential effects upon postural stability. Bensoussan et al,Citation115 Marchese et al,Citation92 and Yardley et al,Citation116 for instance, reported detrimental effects of aloud verbal, arithmetic tasks on postural stability. On the contrary, Negahban et alCitation65 and Lanzarin et alCitation20 reported beneficial effects of nonverbal tasks on postural stability of fall-prone participants. Literature analysis revealed research studies identifying commonly used dual tasks such as verbal recital, n-back, and counting backward to be considered as more cognitively driven.Citation117 This review also observed the studies to ignore the verbal and hearing component incorporated in a dual-task paradigm. A functional magnetic resonance imaging analysis by Behroozmand et alCitation118 revealed the involvement of bilateral superior temporal gyrus, Heschl’s gyrus, precentral gyrus, supplementary motor area, Rolandic operculum, postcentral gyrus, putamen insula, and right inferior frontal gyrus during speech production.Citation119 Moreover, the authors mentioned that speech production is also followed by a feedback error detection system in the sensory cortex that again activates the motor areas for speech adjustments, therefore suggesting the auditory feedback as an additional factor for increasing complexity in a dual-task setting.

Yardley et alCitation116 speculated the interaction between muscular control of speech-associated respiration and posture to cause perturbation in posture. The authors compared complex articulated, mental tasks while analyzing postural stability and reported beneficial effects on stability in the absence of articulation. This present review also suggests that the reinvolvement of higher motor centers during speech production in a dual task might possibly result in central interference, which might impact the person’s dual task and stability performance. This review also adds to the existing knowledge that dual-task paradigms involving only a mental component, such as mental arithmetic task, might also include a motor component. As mentioned earlier, hearing also incorporates activation of cortical structures, precisely bilateral superior temporal gyrus, and Heschl’s gyrus.Citation118 The phase of instructions might activate this cortical pathway and can add to the certain amount of complexity in the dual-task scenario, which although trivial might result in considerable adverse effects in fall-prone population groups. This review did not find any study that analyzed the effects of dual-task posture in the absence of auditory information, ie, via noise canceling headphones, white noise; therefore possibly explaining the reduction in stability for studies employing nonverbal dual tasks.Citation20,Citation67

In summary, a systematic review was conducted across five online academic search databases: Scopus, PEDro, MEDLINE, EMBASE, and SportDiscus. A total of 1,284 articles were incorporated in our initial search, which later on implementing our inclusion criteria were reduced to 42 (). The meta-analysis conducted on studies suggested beneficial effects of dual-task training with variable priority for enhancing postural stability, especially among elderly participants. Moreover, an inverse relation was observed between the complexity of dual task and postural stability. This review also observed an articulation component within a dual task to be a component of added complexity, which further might enhance cognitive–motor interference in fall-prone population groups. This study also reveals detrimental effects of complex dual tasks among population groups with a higher predisposition to fall, as compared to their healthy counterparts.

Strengths

This present review is the first to analyze and compare the effects of dual-task training and dual task on postural stability. Respective authors of the included papers were contacted for additional descriptive data or information. The review conformed to PRISMA guidelines in all applicable areas. A meta-analysis and a PEDro level of evidence were included for the studies included in this present review. The data used to compute the meta-analysis were used from the descriptive statistics and not identified from figures to reduce the incidence of bias. This present review was also an effort to address the limitations pertained by previously conducted reviews. For instance, a few of the previous systematic reviews carried out the search across few academic databases. For instance, Ruffieux et alCitation26 conducted the search across two academic databases, Boisgontier et alCitation6 across three databases, and Agmon et alCitation49 across four academic databases. This present review identified five widely utilized and reputed academic databases and continuously updated the data over a duration of 9 months. Additionally, few keyword search terms were identified as a possible limitation factor in the previous systematic reviews. However, during our literature search the authors utilized a broad variety of MeSH keyword search terms (Supplementary material), which might have increased the possibilities of including a wide array of studies. The meta-analysis carried out in this present review is the first to evaluate the effects of dual-task training on elderly participants. However, it also aimed to replicate previous findings, while addressing the increased heterogeneity.

Limitations

Several limitations persisted in the systematic review, which are to be considered while interpreting the results. The average quality of the included studies according to PEDro methodological quality scale was found to be 4.7, indicating a fair quality of the studies. A high risk of bias prevailed because of the limited number of RCTs. The restriction of search strategy limited to English language, exclusion of conference proceedings and observational studies might have resulted in omission of relevant research. Inability to retrieve descriptive statistics from the respective studies and including fewer studies in the meta-analysis was also a major limitation of this study.

This present study did not impose restrictions on the type of included dual task, in order to analyze the differential effects of complexity of dual task. Therefore, a higher chance of biasing and differential outcomes can be expected. Likewise, the systematic difference between the population group base statistics related to age, weight, gender, and disease severity led to difficulty in comparing studies. A majority of the incorporated studies had a small sample size, which generates a high possibility of a type II error.Citation120 The conclusions derived in the review based on incorporation of dual-task training in rehabilitation protocol are based on limited research.

Future directions

Future studies should focus on combining easier, nonverbal dual tasks in training during rehabilitation. Neuroimaging studies can provide additional insights for mechanisms involved during execution of nonverbal dual tasks. The review also suggests training fall-prone population groups to prioritize balance, ie, posture “first” in complex fall-prone environments; for instance escalators, narrow alleyways.Citation121 Likewise, nonverbal tasks utilized during activity of daily living can be analyzed in dual-task training regimes. Together, real-life implications can be drawn from these studies.

Acknowledgments

The publication of this article was funded by the Open Access fund of Leibniz Universität Hannover. The authors would like to thank Dr Matthew W Driller, Prof Dr Rich SW Masters for their constructive comments. The coauthor expresses sincere gratitude toward Education New Zealand and the University of Waikato for awarding research scholarships.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • WikstromEATillmanMDSmithANBorsaPAA new force-plate technology measure of dynamic postural stability: the dynamic postural stability indexJ Athl Train200540430530916404452
  • GhaiSDrillerMGhaiIEffects of joint stabilizers on proprioception and stability: a systematic review and meta-analysisPhys Ther Sport In press2016
  • GobleDJCoxonJPWenderothNVan ImpeASwinnenSPProprioceptive sensibility in the elderly: degeneration, functional consequences and plastic-adaptive processesNeurosci Biobehav Rev200933327127818793668
  • VaugoyeauMVielSAmblardBAzulayJAssaianteCProprioceptive contribution of postural control as assessed from very slow oscillations of the support in healthy humansGait Posture200827229430217509884
  • RaftopoulosACognitive Penetrability of Perception: Attention, Action, Strategies, and Bottom-Up ConstraintsNew York, NYNova Publishers2005
  • BoisgontierMPBeetsIADuysensJNieuwboerAKrampeRTSwinnenSPAge-related differences in attentional cost associated with postural dual tasks: increased recruitment of generic cognitive resources in older adultsNeurosci Biobehav Rev20133781824183723911924
  • JacobsJHorakFCortical control of postural responsesJ Neural Transm2007114101339134817393068
  • HoneycuttCFGottschallJSNicholsTRElectromyographic responses from the hindlimb muscles of the decerebrate cat to horizontal support surface perturbationsJ Neurophysiol200910162751276119321638
  • MastersRSWMaxwellJThe theory of reinvestmentInt Rev Sport Exer Psychol200812160183
  • WulfGMcNevinNSheaCHThe automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focusQ J Exp Psychol A20015441143115411765737
  • MastersRSWKnowledge, knerves and know-how: the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressureBr J Psychol1992833343358
  • SchaeferSSchellenbachMLindenbergerUWoollacottMWalking in high-risk settings: do older adults still prioritize gait when distracted by a cognitive task?Exp Brain Res20152331798825224704
  • SeidlerRDBernardJABurutoluTBMotor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effectsNeurosci Biobehav Rev201034572173319850077
  • TalelliPEwasAWaddinghamWRothwellJWardNNeural correlates of age-related changes in cortical neurophysiologyNeuroimage20084041772178118329904
  • SchaeferSJagenowDVerrelJLindenbergerUThe influence of cognitive load and walking speed on gait regularity in children and young adultsGait Posture201541125826225455434
  • DonkerSFRoerdinkMGrevenAJBeekPJRegularity of center-of-pressure trajectories depends on the amount of attention invested in postural controlExp Brain Res2007181111117401553
  • ReschJEMayBTomporowskiPDFerraraMSBalance performance with a cognitive task: a continuation of the dual-task testing paradigmJ Athl Train201146217021391802
  • BeilockSLCarrTHOn the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure?J Exp Psychol Gen2001130470172511757876
  • GhaiSDrillerMWMastersRSThe influence of below-knee compression garments on knee-joint proprioceptionGait Posture Epub201689
  • LanzarinMParizzotoPLibardoniTDCSinhorimLTavaresGMSSantosGMThe influence of dual-tasking on postural control in young adultsFisioter Pesquisa20152216168
  • BoesMKSosnoffJJSocieMJSandroffBMPulaJHMotlRWPostural control in multiple sclerosis: effects of disability status and dual taskJ Neurol Sci20123151444822236888
  • AndradeLPDRinaldiNMCoelhoFGDMTanakaKStellaFGobbiLTBDual task and postural control in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseaseMotriz Rev Ed Fís20142017884
  • Montero-OdassoMBergmanHPhillipsNAWongCHSourialNChertkowHDual-tasking and gait in people with mild cognitive impairment. The effect of working memoryBMC Geriatr2009914119723315
  • WoollacottMShumway-CookAAttention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of researchGait Posture200216111412127181
  • HortobágyiTdel OlmoMFRothwellJCAge reduces cortical reciprocal inhibition in humansExp Brain Res2006171332232916307241
  • RuffieuxJKellerMLauberBTaubeWChanges in standing and walking performance under dual-task conditions across the lifespanSports Med201545121739175826253187
  • FujiyamaHHinderMRSchmidtMWGarryMISummersJJAge-related differences in corticospinal excitability and inhibition during coordination of upper and lower limbsNeurobiol Aging2012337e1481e1484
  • FujitaHKasubuchiKWakataSHiyamizuMMoriokaSRole of the frontal cortex in standing postural sway tasks while dual-tasking: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study examining working memory capacityBiomed Res Int20162016705386727034947
  • SmithECusackTBlakeCThe effect of a dual task on gait speed in community dwelling older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysisGait Posture20164425025827004667
  • WajdaDASosnoffJJCognitive-motor interference in multiple sclerosis: a systematic review of evidence, correlates, and consequencesBiomed Res Int2015201572085625839039
  • LearmonthYCEnsariIMotlRWCognitive motor interference in multiple sclerosis: insights from a systematic quantitative reviewArch Phys Med Rehabil Epub8162016
  • ZanottoTBergaminMRomanFEffect of exercise on dual-task and balance on elderly in multiple disease conditionsCurr Aging Sci20147211513624679341
  • GobboSBergaminMSieverdesJCErmolaoAZaccariaMEffects of exercise on dual-task ability and balance in older adults: a systematic reviewArch Gerontol Geriatr201458217718724188735
  • ChoiJHKimBRHanEYKimSMThe effect of dual-task training on balance and cognition in patients with subacute post-strokeAnn Rehabil Med2015391819025750876
  • SilsupadolPShumway-CookALugadeVEffects of single-task versus dual-task training on balance performance in older adults: a double-blind, randomized controlled trialArch Phys Med Rehabil200990338138719254600
  • HiyamizuMMoriokaSShomotoKShimadaTEffects of dual task balance training on dual task performance in elderly people: a randomized controlled trialClin Rehabil2012261586721421689
  • Gomez-PinillaFHillmanCThe influence of exercise on cognitive abilitiesCompr Physiol20133140342823720292
  • WollesenBVoelcker-RehageCTraining effects on motor–cognitive dual-task performance in older adultsEur Rev Aging Phys Act20131115
  • ParkDCReuter-LorenzPThe adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffoldingAnnu Rev Psychol20096017319035823
  • BrownSBennettEThe role of practice and automaticity in temporal and nontemporal dual-task performancePsychol Res2002661808911963281
  • MüllerHBlischkeKGrundlagen der Sportpsychologie. [Basics of sports psychology]Limpert VerlagWiesbaden2009 German
  • BhererLKramerAFPetersonMSColcombeSEricksonKBecicETraining effects on dual-task performance: are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control?Psychol Aging200520469516420143
  • HeuerHMotor Learning as a Process of Structural Constriction and DisplacementBerlinSpringer1984295305
  • BlischkeKReiterCBewegungsautomatisierung durch Doppeltätigkeits-Üben. [Movement automation by double action practicing.]Spectrum der Sportwissenschaften200214829
  • BlischkeKAutomatisierung einer großmotorischen Kalibrierungsaufgabe durch Prozeduralisierung. [Automation of a large-scale calibration task by proceduralization]Psychol Sport200181938
  • BlischkeKWagnerFZehrenBBruecknerSDual-task practice of temporally structured movement sequences augments integrated task processing, but not automatizationJ Human Kinet201025515
  • BhererLKramerAFPetersonMSColcombeSEricksonKBecicETransfer effects in task-set cost and dual-task cost after dual-task training in older and younger adults: further evidence for cognitive plasticity in attentional control in late adulthoodExp Aging Res200834318821918568979
  • AnH-JKimJ-IKimY-RThe effect of various dual task training methods with gait on the balance and gait of patients with chronic strokeJ Phys Ther Sci20142681287129125202199
  • AgmonMBelzaBNguyenHQLogsdonRGKellyVEA systematic review of interventions conducted in clinical or community settings to improve dual-task postural control in older adultsClin Interv Aging2014947749224741296
  • FritzNECheekFMNichols-LarsenDSMotor-cognitive dual-task training in persons with neurologic disorders: a systematic reviewJ Neurol Phys Ther201539314215326079569
  • WangXQPiYLChenBLCognitive motor interference for gait and balance in stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysisEur J Neurol2015223555e3725560629
  • PichierriGWolfPMurerKde BruinEDCognitive and cognitive-motor interventions affecting physical functioning: a systematic reviewBMC Geriatr2011111121223578
  • MoherDLiberatiATetzlaffJAltmanDGPreferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statementAnn Intern Med2009151426426919622511
  • SteindlRUlmerHStandstabilität im kindes-und jugendalter. [Stability in childhood and adolescence]HNO200452542343015138648
  • Centre for Reviews and DisseminationSystematic Reviews: CRD’s Guidance for Undertaking Reviews in Health Care. Centre for Reviews and DisseminationYorkYork Publishing Services Ltd2009
  • HigginsJPGreenSCochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Version 5Wiley Online Library2008 Available from: http://handbook.cochrane.orgAccessed August 3, 2016
  • MaherCGSherringtonCHerbertRDMoseleyAMElkinsMReliability of the PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trialsPhys Ther200383871372112882612
  • TeasellRWFoleyNCBhogalSKSpeechleyMREvidence-Based Review of Stroke RehabilitationTopics in Stroke Rehabilitation2003101295812970830
  • CohenJStatistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences2nd edHillsdale, NJLawrence Erlbaum Associates1988
  • HigginsJPTGreenSCochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.0.2 [updated September 2009]The Cochrane Collaboration2008
  • BuragaddaSAlyaemniAMelamGRAlghamdiMAEffect of dual-task training (fixed priority-versus-variable priority) for improving balance in older adultsWorld Appl Sci J2012206884888
  • KimDKoJWooYEffects of dual task training with visual restriction and an unstable base on the balance and attention of stroke patientsJ Phys Ther Sci201325121579158224409024
  • LiKZRoudaiaELussierMBhererLLerouxAMcKinleyPBenefits of cognitive dual-task training on balance performance in healthy older adultsJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci201065121344135220837662
  • PellecchiaGLDual-task training reduces impact of cognitive task on postural swayJ Mot Behav200537323924615883121
  • NegahbanHMofatehRArastooAAThe effects of cognitive loading on balance control in patients with multiple sclerosisGait Posture201134447948421802303
  • ProsperiniLCastelliLSellittoGInvestigating the phenomenon of “cognitive-motor interference” in multiple sclerosis by means of dual-task posturographyGait Posture201541378078525770078
  • AnderssonGHagmanJTalianzadehRSvedbergALarsenHCEffect of cognitive load on postural controlBrain Res Bull200258113513912121823
  • BergaminMGobboSZanottoTInfluence of age on postural sway during different dual-task conditionsFront Aging Neurosci2014627127725374539
  • BrauerSWoollacottMShumway-CookAThe influence of a concurrent cognitive task on the compensatory stepping response to a perturbation in balance-impaired and healthy eldersGait Posture2002151839311809584
  • BrauerSGWoollacottMShumway-CookAThe interacting effects of cognitive demand and recovery of postural stability in balance-impaired elderly personsJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2001568M489M49611487601
  • BrownLAShumway-CookAWoollacottMHAttentional demands and postural recovery: the effects of agingJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci1999544M165M17110219006
  • DaultMCGeurtsACMulderTWDuysensJPostural control and cognitive task performance in healthy participants while balancing on different support-surface configurationsGait Posture200114324825511600328
  • DoumasMSmoldersCKrampeRTTask prioritization in aging: effects of sensory information on concurrent posture and memory performanceExp Brain Res2008187227528118273609
  • HaggertySJiangL-TGaleckiASienkoKHEffects of biofeedback on secondary-task response time and postural stability in older adultsGait Posture201235452352822406291
  • HunterMCHoffmanMAPostural control: visual and cognitive manipulationsGait Posture2001131414811166553
  • HuxholdOLiS-CSchmiedekFLindenbergerUDual-tasking postural control: aging and the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attentionBrain Res Bull200669329430516564425
  • HwangJHLeeC-HChangHJParkD-SSequential analysis of postural control resource allocation during a dual task testAnn Rehabil Med201337334735423869332
  • MakLYehTTBouletJCluffTBalasubramaniamRInteraction between delayed visual feedback and secondary cognitive tasks on postural control in older adultsScience & Motricité2011748188
  • MarshAPGeelSEThe effect of age on the attentional demands of postural controlGait Posture200012210511310998606
  • MelzerIBenjuyaNKaplanskiJAge-related changes of postural control: effect of cognitive tasksGerontology200147418919411408723
  • PellecchiaGLPostural sway increases with attentional demands of concurrent cognitive taskGait Posture2003181293412855298
  • RamenzoniVCRileyMAShockleyKChiuC-YPPostural responses to specific types of working memory tasksGait Posture200725336837316806935
  • Shumway-CookAWoollacottMAttentional demands and postural control: the effect of sensory contextJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2000551M1010719767
  • Shumway-CookAWoollacottMKernsKABaldwinMThe effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of fallsJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci1997524M232M2409224435
  • SwanLOtaniHLoubertPVReducing postural sway by manipulating the difficulty levels of a cognitive task and a balance taskGait Posture200726347047417161948
  • SwanLOtaniHLoubertPVSheffertSMDunbarGLImproving balance by performing a secondary cognitive taskBr J Psychol2004951314015005866
  • TeasdaleNBardCLaRueJFleuryMOn the cognitive penetrability of posture controlExp Aging Res19931911138444263
  • TeasdaleNSimoneauMAttentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegrationGait Posture200114320321011600323
  • VuillermeNIsableuBNougierVAttentional demands associated with the use of a light fingertip touch for postural control during quiet standingExp Brain Res2006169223223616273399
  • de MortonNAThe PEDro scale is a valid measure of the methodological quality of clinical trials: a demographic studyAust J Physiother200955212913319463084
  • JacobiHAlfesJMinneropMKonczakJKlockgetherTTimmannDDual task effect on postural control in patients with degenerative cerebellar disordersCerebellum Ataxias201521626331049
  • MarcheseRBoveMAbbruzzeseGEffect of cognitive and motor tasks on postural stability in Parkinson’s disease: a posturographic studyMov Disord200318665265812784268
  • MorrisMIansekRSmithsonFHuxhamFPostural instability in Parkinson’s disease: a comparison with and without a concurrent taskGait Posture200012320521611154931
  • HolmesJJenkinsMJohnsonAMAdamsSSpauldingSDual-task interference: the effects of verbal cognitive tasks on upright postural stability in Parkinson’s diseaseParkinsons Dis2010201069649220976093
  • TeasellRMarshallSCullenNEvidence Based Review of Moderate to Severe Acquired Brain InjuryToronto, ONOntario Neurotrauma Foundation2005
  • BolierLHavermanMWesterhofGJRiperHSmitFBohlmeijerEPositive psychology interventions: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled studiesBMC Public Health201313111923390882
  • VuillermeNVincentHHow performing a mental arithmetic task modify the regulation of centre of foot pressure displacements during bipedal quiet standingExp Brain Res2006169113013416374632
  • TysonSFHanleyMChillalaJSelleyATallisRCBalance disability after strokePhys Ther2006861303816386060
  • VereijkenBWhitingHBeekWA dynamical systems approach to skill acquisitionQ J Exp Psychol1992452323344
  • KramerAFLarishJFStrayerDLTraining for attentional control in dual task settings: a comparison of young and old adultsJ Exp Psychol Appl19951150
  • ShigematsuROkuraTNakagaichiMSquare-stepping exercise and fall risk factors in older adults: a single-blind, randomized controlled trialJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2008631768218245764
  • EricksonKIColcombeSJWadhwaRTraining-induced functional activation changes in dual-task processing: an FMRI studyCereb Cortex200717119220416467562
  • WolpertDMDiedrichsenJFlanaganJRPrinciples of sensorimotor learningNat Rev Neurosci2011121273975122033537
  • TombuMJolicoeurPAll-or-none bottleneck versus capacity sharing accounts of the psychological refractory period phenomenonPsychol Res200266427428612466925
  • BohnenNMüllerMKoeppeRHistory of falls in Parkinson disease is associated with reduced cholinergic activityNeurology200973201670167619917989
  • DirnbergerGJahanshahiMExecutive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: a reviewJ Neuropsychol20137219322424007368
  • KishSJShannakKHornykiewiczOUneven pattern of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s diseaseN Engl J Med1988318148768803352672
  • KalaitzakisMEPearceRKThe morbid anatomy of dementia in Parkinson’s diseaseActa Neuropathol2009118558759819820956
  • Yogev-SeligmannGHausdorffJMGiladiNDo we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritizationMov Disord201227676577022419512
  • HerathPKlingbergTYoungJAmuntsKRolandPNeural correlates of dual task interference can be dissociated from those of divided attention: an fMRI studyCereb Cortex200111979680511532885
  • SzameitatAJSchubertTMüllerKVon CramonDYLocalization of executive functions in dual-task performance with fMRIJ Cogn Neurosci20021481184119912495525
  • SchmahmannJDPandyatDNThe cerebrocerebellar systemInt Rev Neurobiol19974131609378595
  • TimmannDDaumICerebellar contributions to cognitive functions: a progress report after two decades of researchCerebellum20076315916217786810
  • WuTLiuJHallettMZhengZChanPCerebellum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processingNeuroimage20136546647523063842
  • BensoussanLVitonJ-MSchieppatiMChanges in postural control in hemiplegic patients after stroke performing a dual taskArch Phys Med Rehabil20078881009101517678663
  • YardleyLGardnerMLeadbetterALavieNEffect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural controlNeuroreport199910221521910203311
  • KaneMJConwayARMiuraTKColfleshGJWorking memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validityJ Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn200733361517470009
  • BehroozmandRShebekRHansenDRSensory–motor networks involved in speech production and motor control: an fMRI studyNeuroimage201510941842825623499
  • ParkinsonALFlagmeierSGManesJLLarsonCRRogersBRobinDAUnderstanding the neural mechanisms involved in sensory control of voice productionNeuroimage201261131432222406500
  • FreimanJAChalmersTCSmithHJrKueblerRRThe importance of beta, the type II error and sample size in the design and interpretation of the randomized control trial: survey of 71 negative trialsN Engl J Med197829913690694355881
  • BloemBRGrimbergenYAvan DijkJGMunnekeMThe “posture second” strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s diseaseJ Neurol Sci2006248119620416806270
  • RamseyLWinderRJMcVeighJGThe effectiveness of working wrist splints in adults with rheumatoid arthritis: a mixed methods systematic reviewJ Rehabil Med201446648149224715196