158
Views
31
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Association of leisure-time physical activity with cognition by apolipoprotein-E genotype in persons aged 60 years and over: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES-III)

, , &
Pages 35-43 | Published online: 31 Jan 2012

Abstract

Objective

To test the hypothesis that aerobic-related leisure-time physical activity (PA) is associated with better cognitive function and that the effect varies among apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype groups.

Design

Cross-sectional study of persons examined in the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES-III; 1988–1994).

Setting

US noninstitutionalized population.

Participants and methods

From a sample of 7159, aged ≥60 years, we analyzed data for 1799 older American men and women who had information on PA, a short mental status examination (SMSE), and were genotyped at the apolipoprotein E gene locus.

Results

In the initial bivariate analysis, non-ɛ4 carriers and ɛ4-heterozygotes performed better than ɛ4-homozygotes in the 60–69 age group. After controlling for multiple confounders including mobility limitation, PA correlated with a higher SMSE score in non-ɛ4 carriers (P = 0.014), but not in ɛ4 carriers (P = 0.887). At ≥70 years, PA also correlated with higher adjusted SMSE scores in non-ɛ4 carriers (P = 0.02); but this association became nonsignificant after controlling for mobility limitation (P = 0.12).

Conclusion

In a nationally representative sample, PA was associated with enhanced cognition, an effect that was differentially influenced by apolipoprotein E genotype. Experimental studies are needed to determine whether or not PA can attenuate cognitive decline.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common type of dementia, constituting two-thirds of all late-life dementias.Citation1,Citation2 Its prevalence increases about 15-fold from 3% among individuals aged 65–74 years to 47% among persons aged ≥85 years.Citation3Citation6 In 2010, the annual US health care cost for AD reached about US$144 billion.Citation7 Similarly, total 2010 health care, including long-term care payments for AD and other dementia care, was expected to reach US$172 billion.Citation7 Given this cost and the projected increase in elderly population by the year 2050, identifying effective mechanisms to prevent AD is an important public health goal.

Until recently, the influence of lifestyle adaptation on AD risk had received very little attention in the scientific literature. Emerging evidence now indicates that PA can enhance cognitive processes.Citation8Citation18 Whereas many of such studies examined the type of physical activities performed, others focused on the relationship of the number,Citation19 frequency,Citation20 or intensityCitation21 of such activities with cognitive processes. A summary of published literature by Colcombe and Kramer, using a meta-analysis of 18 studies, concluded that fitness training benefitted an array of neurocognitive process in nondemented older adults.Citation22 Conclusions from these studies mostly suggest significant association of low and high levels of physical activity (PA) with reduced and enhanced cognitive processes, respectively; however, the sample sizes were often small, nonrepresentative, and lacked uniformity in their definition of PA and the characterization of the cognitive phenotype.

Despite the evidence showing that high levels of PA are mostly beneficial, significant interindividual variations of these benefits remain. This may suggest effect modification at the genetic level. Variation at the apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene locus is the most consistent nondeterministic genetic risk factor for late-onset Alzheimer’s dementia.Citation23Citation26 Its contribution to AD risk is graded across its three known common isoforms (E2, E3, and E4) in humans encoded by ɛ2, ɛ3, and ɛ4 alleles. Whereas, ɛ2 allele is associated with lower risk, ɛ4 associates with increased AD risk, an effect that is differentially influenced by age.Citation27Citation29 Interindividual variation in the ɛ4 allele-associated risk may also indicate additional effect modification at the environment level such as imposed by lifestyle. A prior longitudinal study examined the potential benefits of interactions between APOE and PA, with varying results.Citation19 Whether the APOE gene modifies the effects of PA on cognition in a nationally representative sample such as the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES-III) study is yet to be determined. We hypothesized that PA will associate with enhanced cognition and that this effect will differ with age and APOE genotype in a nationally representative sample of older Americans.

Methods

Study subjects

The NHANES-III was a cross-sectional, stratified, multistage probability sample of 33,994 civilian, noninstitutionalized Americans in the 50 states and the District of Columbia aged from 2 months to 99 years old, making it the first NHANES to include persons aged ≥75 years. The study, conducted by the Center for Disease Control’s National Center for Health Statistics between 1988 and 1994, oversampled Mexican Americans, African Americans, and those >60 years.Citation30,Citation31 Of persons aged ≥60 years, 5724 (86%) had an assessment of cognitive function. A subset of this sample (1799) included in this analysis were genotyped at the APOE locus.

General assessments

Overall, the questionnaires were designed for administration in a bilingual (English/Spanish) format so that respondents could be interviewed in their preferred language. During a home interview, an interviewer collected the demographic variables such as age, gender, and level of education to be used in this analysis.

PA

Participants were asked, “In the past month, did you engage in any leisure-time physical activity?” If their answer was “yes,” they were asked, “In the past month, how often did you …?” (specify number of times) for the following: jogging or running, riding a bicycle or exercise bicycle, swimming, aerobic dancing, other dancing, calisthenics or floor exercises, gardening or yard work, and weight lifting.Citation32,Citation33 Open-ended questions assessed up to four other activities. Participants were also asked about how often they walked ≥1 mile. Persons responding “no times” to all of the activities above were classified “no PA.” Mobility limitation was also categorized on the basis of participants’ responses to two questions asking about difficulty experienced walking two or three blocks and walking up to ten steps without resting. Persons reporting no difficulty were considered to not have limited mobility. These two questions have been used by other investigators to assess mobility limitation.Citation34,Citation35 Serious chronic illnesses included a self-reported history of chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, congestive heart failure, myocardial infarction, cancer, diabetes, and stroke.

Assessment of cognitive function

Questions assessing mental cognition were asked only of respondents aged ≥60 years. The neuropsychological measures used in the NHANES-III study were selected to assess cognitive functions typically affected in dementia. Citation36 The evaluations, including measures of learning and memory, were obtained during home interviews and at the mobile examination center. These questions, along with recall questions and “serial 3 subtraction” found on the adult questionnaire, constituted a short mental state examination (SMSE).Citation36,Citation37 Details of the content and administration of the SMSE used during the NHANES-III have been previously published.Citation38 Overall scores on the SMSE calculated from the sum of orientation, recall, and attention ranged from 0 to 17. The SMSE was carried out during a home examination in 8.6% of the sample for participants who were unable or unwilling to come to a mobile examination center for a complete examination.

Genetic data

Blood lymphocytes were collected from participants aged ≥12 years and stored in liquid nitrogen as cell cultures immortalized with Epstein–Barr virus. DNA in the form of crude cell lysates was made available for approved research projects for 7159 persons examined between 1991 and 1994. Genetic data generated from those projects from 1999 to 2006 were submitted to the National Center for Health Statistics and stored upon successful completion of a three-part quality-control analysis. We used data for participants genotyped at the APOE locus (dbSNP ID: rs7412 and rs429358). To maintain confidentiality of individual-level data, analysis occurred at the National Center for Health Statistics Research Data Center in Hyattsville, MD. Approval of the Howard University Institutional Review Board was obtained prior to the analysis.

Statistical analysis

Initial analyses using SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) included t-test and chi-square test and examined for group differences in age, gender, ethnicity, educational attainment, and body mass by level of self-reported history of PA, as well as the effects of PA on cognition within age strata. Using multiple regression analysis, we then examined the relationship of PA to cognition, while adjusting for a priori defined confounders. Given the declining levels of PA with age and lower levels of PA in Blacks than in Whites, model 1 included adjustment for age and ethnicity effects on cognition and physical function, whereas model 2 discounted the effects of chronic medical illnesses on cognitive processes and on PA levels. To discount the contribution of education on differences in performance on cognitive tasksCitation39 and the association of body mass index (BMI) with cognition across populations,Citation40,Citation41 model 3 additionally adjusted for education and BMI. Given that persons with mobility limitation are less likely to exercise, this variable was included in model 4. Because of evidence indicating that the effect of APOE on cognition is probably influenced by age,Citation27Citation29 and to further discern the effect of age from other predictors of cognitive outcome and important confounders, we constructed separate models for the age groups 60–69 and ≥70. Least square means adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity, and education were derived using analysis of variance (general linear model). Similar, but separate, models were constructed for APOE genotype groups. To mitigate sample-size limitation, we dichotomized the sample as non-ɛ4 carriers (ɛ4 allele absent), and ɛ4 carriers (at least one copy of ɛ4).

To address the peculiarity of the multistage complex survey design of NHANES-III, final analyses were performed using SUDAAN (RTI International, Research Triangle Park, NC), software that accounts for the multistage sampling of NHANES-III in computing variance estimates.Citation42 Appropriate sampling weights were used to account for oversampling and nonresponse. SUDAAN Proc Crosstab and Descript were used to estimate percentages and means, respectively, across groups. Proc Reg was used for linear regression.

Results

Overall, 56.9% had any PA, while 43.1% did not. Those who engaged in any PA were relatively younger, more likely to be men, and had more formal education (). Compared with other ethnic groups, Whites in the sample were about twice as likely to engage in PA as Blacks and Mexican Americans (61.6%, 32.8%, and 38.9%, respectively). However, both groups had comparable BMI. APOE genotype was in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (P < 0.05). The respective frequencies of allele combinations paralleled those reported in the literature. In the entire sample, Blacks were more likely to be carriers and homozygotes for the risk allele (ɛ4) (39.19 and 4.89, respectively), compared with Whites (28.02 and 2.19, respectively), and Mexican Americans (19.34 and 1.61), respectively.

Table 1 Characteristics of the sample (percent by leisure-time physical activity)

Between the ages of 60 and 69 years ( and ), persons who performed PA tended to score higher on the SMSE among heterozygotes and noncarriers but not among homozygotes. At age ≥ 70 years, similar associations were observed not only in heterozygotes and noncarriers but also in homozygotes.

Figure 1 Mean short mental state examination (SMSE) score by physical activity and apolipoprotein E genotype (age 60 to 69 years).

Figure 1 Mean short mental state examination (SMSE) score by physical activity and apolipoprotein E genotype (age 60 to 69 years).

Figure 2 Mean short mental state examination (SMSE) score by leisure-time physical activity and apolipoprotein E genotype (age ≥ 70 years).

Figure 2 Mean short mental state examination (SMSE) score by leisure-time physical activity and apolipoprotein E genotype (age ≥ 70 years).

Multiple regression analysis

In the initial analysis restricted to the non-ɛ4 carrier genotype group aged 60–69 years and adjusted for age and ethnicity (model 1), PA was significantly associated with cognitive performance (β = 0.68 ± 0.2; P = 0.006) (). This model accounted for 9.8% of the observed variance. With additional adjustment for serious chronic medical illnesses (congestive heart failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, cancer, diabetes, myocardial infarction, and stroke) (model 2), this statistically significant association remained unchanged (β = 0.68 ± 0.2; P = 0.007). However, after accounting for differences in BMI and education (model 3), PA maintained a statistically significant but reduced association with cognitive function (β = 0.52 ± 0.2; P = 0.007). In the last model, controlled for mobility limitation in non-ɛ4 carriers (model 4), the significant association of PA (β coefficient 0.43 ± 0.2, P = 0.014) with SMSE score persisted though slightly attenuated. However, among the ɛ4 carriers of this age group, PA did not significantly associate with cognitive performance.

Table 2 Regression analysis dependent variable: short mental state examination (age 60–69 years)

We also examined the relationship of PA to SMSE in the >70 year old age group (). In noncarriers, PA associated with performance on the cognitive test in models 1, 2, and 3. With the introduction of mobility limitation (β = −0.78 ± 0.2, P < 0.001) in model 4, the association of PA to cognitive performance became attenuated and nonsignificant (β = 0.37 ± 0.2, P = 0.117). Among ɛ4 carriers, similar associations with cognitive performance were observed in models 1–4, but did not attain statistical significance. This could be due to the smaller sample size.

Table 3 Regression analysis dependent variable: short mental state examination (age ≥ 70 years)

Discussion

PA was associated with better performance on cognitive tasks in a sample of age ≥ 60 non-ɛ4 carriers but not in carriers. These findings were independent of linear age, gender, ethnicity, education, income, and history of serious chronic illness. This adds to growing evidence that increased levels of PA may offer an important primary intervention strategy to attenuate neurocognitive loss, especially in non-ɛ4 carriers. Delay in significant cognitive loss and considerable public heath impact are likely to be realized if these findings are confirmed in experimental studies.

Evidence showing that PA can enhance cognition includes cross-sectional, prospective studies and a limited number of controlled trials.Citation8Citation11,Citation17Citation20,Citation22 Many studies examined the effects of aerobic fitness on cognition in nondemented older adults, while others determined whether or not lifestyle factors can reduce or delay late-onset Alzheimer’s dementia. In concordance with our observations from this study, a beneficial effect of PA was often reported.Citation19Citation21,Citation43 Whereas Albert and colleagues reported that strenuous but not moderate intensity daily PA enhanced cognition,Citation44 Yaffe et al showed that moderate daily PA can also improve cognitive processes.Citation45 Together, these reports are consistent with our hypothesis and results from this study that PA is beneficial to cognitive health. However, unlike previous cross-sectional studies, the use of a representative sample of US noninstitutionalized population enhanced the generalizability of our findings beyond that previously reported in cross-sectional studies.

Consistent with our second hypothesis, the relationship of PA to performance on the SMSE test varied with APOE genotype and age group. In the adjusted model, the apparent relationship between APOE and PA observed in the unadjusted model became attenuated in those aged >70 years. This may suggest that the APOE effect becomes attenuated with age, allowing other determinants of cognitive function such as PA to become more apparent. Similar to the cognitively beneficial effects of PA observed in non-ɛ4 carriers in our study, Podewils et al demonstrated that performance of ≥4 types of activity lowered dementia risk at age ≥ 65 years in the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study, an effect that was marked in non-ɛ4 carriers but absent in ɛ4 carriers.Citation19 Conversely, we confirmed a lack of association of PA with cognitive performance in ɛ4 carriers in the 60–69 age group. Persons suffering from serious medical illnesses may perform at lower levels of PA and have lower cognition. However, the statistically significant association of PA with enhanced cognition at age 60–69 years in our final model suggests that the association of PA with enhanced cognition in the younger non-ɛ4 carriers is likely independent of mobility limitation and chronic medical illnesses.

An additional important finding from this study is the improved performance in the >70 age group exercisers compared with nonexercisers in all genotype groups, suggesting that the favorable effects of PA in this group may not be significantly influenced by the ɛ4 allele of the APOE gene (). The favorable effects of PA on memory in non-ɛ4 carriers remained unaltered, notwithstanding adjustment for serious chronic medical illness, but not after accounting for the effect of mobility limitation. However, a three-group (ɛ4-homozygote, ɛ4-heterozygote, and noncarriers) analysis to confirm that PA can enhance cognition at age ≥ 70 years regardless of genotype group was constrained by sample size.

Because mobility limitation is a surrogate marker for PA, its significant association with poorer performance on cognitive tests after discounting the effects of serious chronic medical illness supports its role as an indicator of sedentary lifestyle. Previously, it was suggested that pathologies underlining frailty may partially explain the association of low levels of PA with poor cognitive performance.Citation46 The coexistence of frailty with serious chronic medical condition and low BMI prompted us to examine their contributions to cognitive outcome in our sample. However, the significant effect of PA after adjustments for serious chronic medical conditions and BMI, makes frailty a less tenable explanation for our findings in older adults but supports mobility limitation as a surrogate marker for low levels of PA. Given the nonspecific pattern of association of APOE genotype groups with limitation in physical function, this relationship probably does not explain our findings either.

Contrary to studies reporting a beneficial effect of PA and/or aerobic fitness on neurocognitive processes, few have reported negative results.Citation47,Citation48 Verghese et al reported that a one-point increment in the PA score did not significantly improve performance on cognitive tasks,Citation47 and that levels of PA did not associate with incident amnestic mild cognitive impairment.Citation48 However, the authors failed to account for a few important components of PA such as walking, gardening/ yard work, and house chores. Interestingly, our observation of no association in the 60–69 age group ɛ4-carriers is also analogous to the overall conclusions of Verghese et al, except that such subgroup analyses were not considered by the authors. Such methodological differences in the assessments of cognition and PA may contribute to inconsistencies in the literature. In comparison with previous cross-sectional studies, this study has the advantage of adjustment for serious chronic illnesses and mobility limitation, is age-specific, and involves APOE genotype-stratified analysis in addition to a representative sample. Though the mechanisms by which APOE comediates differential PA-induced changes in cognition may involve its role in lipid metabolism, further clarification of this process is needed.

Advantages and limitations

NHANES-III is the largest study to provide population-based data on the association of cognitive function and PA in a nationwide representative sample of older Americans. Because age is one of the most important risk factors for dementia, the relatively large sample size of the NHANES-III permits age-stratified analysis, adjustment for multiple confounders, and, therefore, more detailed assessment of the relationship of PA with cognitive outcome. Limitations of this study include possible bias from survey nonresponse and from missing values for some variables and the large number without APOE data.

Due to its cross-sectional nature, the study does not provide information on the temporal sequence of PA with SMSE score. For questionnaires administered in the Hispanic language, translational bias cannot be excluded in spite of rigorous staff training and standardizations. Randomized controlled trials using a more rigorous standardized aerobic exercise-training protocol together with detailed neuropsychological measures are needed to determine the duration, intensity, frequency, and ultimately the volume of exercise that are beneficial.

Conclusion

In a large national sample of noninstitutionalized Americans aged ≥60 years, PA associated with better cognitive outcomes. Between the ages of 60 and 69 years, this effect varied by APOE gene group, the effects being observed only in non-ɛ4 carriers. Randomized controlled trials are needed to establish causality. Also, the APOE genotype or its genotype must be taken into account while designing such studies. These may lay the groundwork for the prescription of aerobic training as an intervention to attenuate AD risk in those at risk.

Acknowledgments

Thomas Obisesan and Richard Gillum participated in the conceptualization, data analysis, interpretation, and manuscript preparation. Nisser Umar and Nivedh Paluvoi participated in the interpretation and manuscript preparation. Biostatistician support was provided by Dr John Kwagyan of Howard University General Clinical Research Center.

Disclosure

This work is supported by K23 AG00980 (TOO), RO1 AG031517-01A2, RR10284 (HU), and AG02213 (RCG). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health or the National Center for Health Statistics.

References

  • RitchieKDementia in the elderlyNeurology19954511211221137501172
  • de Jesús LlibreJFernandezYMarchecoBPrevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease in a Havana Municipality: a community-based study among elderly residentsMEDICC Rev20091122935
  • EvansDAFunkensteinHHAlbertMSPrevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in a community population of older persons. Higher than previously reportedJAMA198926218255125562810583
  • BrookmeyerREvansDAHebertLNational estimates of the prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in the United StatesAlzheimers Dement201171617321255744
  • CornutiuGThe incidence and prevalence of Alzheimer’s diseaseNeurodegener Dis201181–291420689245
  • GuttermanEMMarkowitzJSLewisBFillitHCost of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementia in managed-medicareJ Am Geriatr Soc19994791065107110484247
  • Alzheimer’s Association2010Alzheimer’s disease facts and figuresAlzheimer’s and Dementia20106158194
  • BurnsAIliffeSAlzheimer’s diseaseBMJ2009338b15819196745
  • SteinbergMLeoutsakosJMPodewilsLJLyketsosCGEvaluation of a home-based exercise program in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: the Maximizing Independence in Dementia (MIND) studyInt J Geriatr Psychiatry200924768068519089875
  • WeihMAbu-OmarKEsselmannHPhysical activity and prevention of Alzheimer’s dementia: current evidence and feasibility of an interventional trialFortschr Neurol Psychiatr2009773146151 German19283649
  • WuSLiangJMiaoDPhysical activity and cognitive function in Alzheimer diseaseJAMA20093013273 author reply 273–27419155451
  • AngevarenMVanheesLNooyensACWendel-VosCGVerschurenWMPhysical activity and 5-year cognitive decline in the Doetinchem cohort studyAnn Epidemiol201020647347920470975
  • MiddletonLEYaffeKTargets for the prevention of dementiaJ Alzheimers Dis201020391592420413867
  • MiddletonLEYaffeKPromising strategies for the prevention of dementiaArch Neurol200966101210121519822776
  • BarnesDEWhitmerRAYaffeKPhysical activity and dementia: The need for prevention trialsExerc Sport Sci Rev2007351242917211190
  • LuciaARuizJRExercise is beneficial for patients with Alzheimer’s disease: a call for actionBr J Sports Med201145646846919553220
  • YuFNelsonNWSavikKWymanJFDyskenMBronasUGAffecting Cognition and Quality of Life via Aerobic Exercise in Alzheimer’s DiseaseWest J Nurs Res2011Epub Sep12
  • VreugdenhilACannellJDaviesARazayGA community-based exercise programme to improve functional ability in people with Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized controlled trialScand J Caring Sci2011Epub May12
  • PodewilsLJGuallarEKullerLHPhysical activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition StudyAm J Epidemiol2005161763965115781953
  • LarsonEBWangLBowenJDExercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and olderAnn Intern Med20061442738116418406
  • RovioSKareholtIHelkalaELLeisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s diseaseLancet Neurol200541170571116239176
  • ColcombeSKramerAFFitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic studyPsychol Sci200314212513012661673
  • SaundersAMSchmaderKBreitnerJCApolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele distributions in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease and in other amyloid-forming diseasesLancet199334288737107118103823
  • SaundersAMStrittmatterWJSchmechelDAssociation of apolipoprotein E allele epsilon 4 with late-onset familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s diseaseNeurology1993438146714728350998
  • WolkDADickersonBCfor the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging InitiativeApolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype has dissociable effects on memory and attentional-executive network function in Alzheimer’s diseaseProc Natl Acad Sci U S A201010722102561026120479234
  • BasakJMKimJDifferential effects of ApoE isoforms on dendritic spines in vivo: linking an Alzheimer’s disease risk factor with synaptic alterationsJ Neurosci201130134526452720357102
  • ChristensenHBatterhamPJMackinnonAJThe association of APOE genotype and cognitive decline in interaction with risk factors in a 65–69 year old community sampleBMC Geriatr200881418620605
  • CrivelloFLemaitreHDufouilCEffects of ApoE-epsilon4 allele load and age on the rates of grey matter and hippocampal volumes loss in a longitudinal cohort of 1186 healthy elderly personsNeuroimage20105331064106920060049
  • GreenwoodPMLambertCSunderlandTParasuramanREffects of apolipoprotein E genotype on spatial attention, working memory, and their interaction in healthy, middle-aged adults: results From the National Institute of Mental Health’s BIOCARD studyNeuropsychology200519219921115769204
  • Plan and operation of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988–1994. Series 1: programs and collection proceduresVital Health Stat 11994321407
  • SeidelGKMillisSRLichtenbergPADijkersMPredicting bowel and bladder continence from cognitive status in geriatric rehabilitation patientsArch Phys Med Rehabil19947555905938185455
  • CrespoCJKeteyianSJHeathGWSemposCTLeisure-time physical activity among US adults. Results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination SurveyArch Intern Med1996156193988526703
  • WhiteLPetrovitchHHardmanJCerebrovascular pathology and dementia in autopsied Honolulu-Asia Aging Study participantsAnn N Y Acad Sci200297792312480729
  • Kelley-MooreJAFerraroKFFunctional limitations and religious service attendance in later life: barrier and/or benefit mechanism?J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci2001566S36537311682597
  • GillumRFFrequency of attendance at religious services and leisure-time physical activity in American women and men: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination SurveyAnn Behav Med2006311303516472036
  • AlbertMSmithLAScherrPATaylorJOEvansDAFunkensteinHHUse of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s diseaseInt J Neurosci1991573–41671781938160
  • LichtenbergPAChristensenBExtended normative data for the Logical Memory subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised: responses from a sample of cognitively intact elderly medical patientsPsychol Rep.1992713 Pt 17457461454917
  • ObisesanTOObisesanOAMartinsSHigh blood pressure, hypertension, and high pulse pressure are associated with poorer cognitive function in persons aged 60 and older: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination SurveyJ Am Geriatr Soc200856350150918179496
  • Dilworth-AndersonPHendrieHCManlyJJKhachaturianASFazioSDiagnosis and assessment of Alzheimer’s disease in diverse populationsAlzheimers Dement20084430530918631983
  • LuchsingerJAGustafsonDRAdiposity and Alzheimer’s diseaseCurr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care2009121152119057182
  • ProfennoLAPorsteinssonAPFaraoneSVMeta-Analysis of Alzheimer’s Disease Risk with Obesity, Diabetes, and Related DisordersBiol Psychiatry201067650551219358976
  • KleinbaumDLogistic Regression: A Self Learning text. Statistics in the Health SciencesNew YorkSpringer1992
  • KramerAFEricksonKIColcombeSJExercise, cognition, and the aging brainJ Appl Physiol200610141237124216778001
  • AlbertMSJonesKSavageCRPredictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of successful agingPsychol Aging19951045785898749585
  • YaffeKBarnesDNevittMLuiLYCovinskyKA prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walkArch Intern Med2001161141703170811485502
  • Samper-TernentRAl SnihSRajiMAMarkidesKSOttenbacherKJRelationship between frailty and cognitive decline in older Mexican AmericansJ Am Geriatr Soc200856101845185218811611
  • VergheseJLiptonRBKatzMJLeisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderlyN Engl J Med2003348252508251612815136
  • VergheseJLeValleyADerbyCLeisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in the elderlyNeurology200666682182716467493