202
Views
38
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Taste loss in hospitalized multimorbid elderly subjects

, , , , , , , , , & show all
Pages 167-174 | Published online: 13 Feb 2013

Abstract

Background

Loss of the sense of taste is common among older people. Morbidities and polypharmacy may contribute to the age-related decline in gustatory function. The aims of the present study were to investigate taste perception in elderly hospitalized patients by comparing their taste recognition thresholds with those of healthy, free-living elderly individuals and to identify potential determinants of taste loss.

Methods

The participants in this observational study were 55 elderly patients hospitalized in the acute geriatric section of the Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences at Padova University and 41 free-living individuals aged older than 65 years, randomly recruited from elderly people attending mild fitness programs at public gymnasiums in Padova. Data were collected on nutrition, health, cognitive, and functional status for all participants. Gustatory capabilities were assessed using aqueous solutions of sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and quinine hydrochloride (representing sweet, salty, sour, and bitter stimuli, respectively), and taste recognition thresholds were measured in both groups.

Results

In comparison with the free-living elderly subjects, those in hospital were significantly less able to recognize the taste of citric acid (P < 0.05). Low citric acid sensitivity was independently associated with advanced age (≥75 years; odds ratio [OR] 3.01, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.01–9.82), polypharmacy (number of prescribed drugs ≥ 4; OR 2.74, 95% CI 1.01–7.72), and poor nutritional status (as assessed by Mini Nutritional Assessment score < 23.5; OR 5.08, 95% CI 1.76–14.6).

Conclusion

Because gustatory impairment may reduce a person’s appetite and lead to inadequate dietary intake, compensatory nutritional measures, such as the use of flavor-enhanced foods, should be strongly encouraged, particularly in the hospital setting.

Introduction

Loss of the sense of taste is common among older people,Citation1 and a decline in gustatory function, due to a physiological decline in the density of the taste buds and papillae,Citation2 is reportedly as part of the normal aging process.Citation3 Several studies on taste dysfunction have concluded that changes in taste bud density with increasing age may affect taste function differently at different regions of the tongue. There is consequently reason to believe that sensitivity to the four basic taste qualities, ie, sweet, salty, bitter, and sour, might not be uniformly impaired in the elderly.Citation4 Compared with younger adults, people aged 70 years and older seem to have a decreased sensitivity to sour, bitter, and salty stimuli, while their perception of sweet taste seems to be unaffected.Citation5

Distorted gustatory function in older age may also be influenced by disease, functional and cognitive impairment, depression, or polypharmacy, which have all been documented as significant but underestimated contributors to taste disorders in the elderly.Citation6,Citation7 Elderly hospitalized subjects, who are frequently exposed to a number of debilitating diseases and drugs, might therefore be more likely to have an impaired sense of taste. The gustatory distortion in elderly subjects is thought to have several clinical consequences, eg, diminished appreciation of food and beverages, and a declining appetite, which may increase the risk of anorexia, weight loss, and malnutrition.Citation3 Reliable data on taste perception in hospitalized elderly subjects are still lacking, and the clinical and biological features associated with taste impairment in such patients are still not clear.

We hypothesized that taste impairment would be more frequent in hospitalized than in free-living healthy elderly people. The aims of the present study were: first, to investigate whether the sense of taste among the hospitalized elderly differs in relation to certain tastants, comparing their recognition thresholds for four basic taste qualities with those of healthy free-living elderly people; and second, to analyze the associations between taste perception and clinical features in frail hospitalized older people.

Materials and methods

Study design and participants

A total of 96 subjects, comprising 55 hospitalized patients and 41 community-dwelling individuals, aged 65 years and over, with no previous experience of taste-related research, were enrolled in this study.

The hospitalized subjects (35 women and 20 men, aged 82.7 ± 7.0 years) were selected from patients consecutively admitted from April 2008 to February 2010 to the acute geriatric section of the Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences at Padua University, based on the following exclusion criteria: need for parenteral and/or enteral nutrition, severe comorbidity (grade 4 at the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale),Citation8 severe cognitive impairment, and dysphagia.

The free-living subjects (27 women and 14 men, aged 74.5 ± 4.6 years) were randomly recruited on a voluntary basis from elderly people attending a twice-weekly mild fitness program at public gymnasiums in Padova. Their health was assessed by medical personnel, based on their clinical history and a clinical examination: current smokers (defined as daily or occasional smokers), heavy drinkers (defined by a daily consumption of five or more standard drinks, each one containing 14 grams of pure alcohol in men, and four or more standard drinks in women, according to the definition of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism), and individuals on chemotherapy or diagnosed with cancer were excluded.

This study was part of a Research Project of National Interest sponsored by the Italian Ministry for Education, University and Research and was approved by the local ethics committee. In accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, all participants were fully informed about the nature of the study, and its purpose, procedures, and risks, and gave their informed consent.

Measures

Data were obtained on all participants’ nutritional, health, cognitive and functional status by trained personnel at the Geriatrics Department of Padova University. Nutritional status was assessed by:

  • Serum albumin level (g/dL), for which fasting blood samples were obtained by venipuncture and then processed, aliquoted, and frozen at the Padova University laboratory for diagnostic analysis.

  • Anthropometric measures, ie, height, weight, mid arm circumference, and calf circumference, were measured with subjects’ barefoot and wearing light clothes.Citation9 For patients unable to stand up, height was estimated using the knee-heel length according to Chumlea’s technique.Citation10 Body mass index (kg/m2) was then calculated.

  • The Mini Nutritional Assessment (MNA) questionnaire, an internationally validated tool which consists of 18 items, including data on anthropometric measures, health status, dietary patterns, and subjective assessments of the participant’s nutritional and health status. A total score ≥ 23.5 distinguishes patients with good nutritional status from those at nutritional risk (MNA score, 17–23.5) and those with protein-calorie malnutrition (MNA score < 17).Citation11

Functional and cognitive status were assessed using the activity of daily living tool, the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) scale,Citation12,Citation13 according to Lawton and BrodyCitation14 and the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) devised by Folstein et al,Citation15 in which scores range from 0 to 30. The crude MMSE score was adjusted for age and formal education using the coefficients proposed for the Italian population;Citation16 participants with severe cognitive impairment (MMSE adjusted score < 18) were not enrolled. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the short form of the Geriatric Depression Screening (GDS) scale.Citation17

Health status was assessed using the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS),Citation8 which evaluates comorbidities and illness severity levels, classifying comorbidities for 13 organ systems and grading each condition from 0 (no problem) to 4 (severely incapacitating or life-threatening conditions). The CIRS comorbidity index is given by the number of conditions graded as ≥3.Citation8 The number of drugs prescribed was also considered and polypharmacy was defined as the consumption of four or more different medications, even of the same chemical class.Citation18

Taste functions

Solutions representing the four basic tastes (sweet, salty, sour, and bitter) were prepared by trained personnel using sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and quinine hydrochloride, respectively. Each compound was presented in four different concentrations, for a total of 16 aqueous solutions of sucrose (0.0032, 0.010, 0.032, 0.10 M), sodium chloride (0.0032, 0.010, 0.032, 0.10 M), citric acid (0.00037, 0.0011, 0.0037, 0.011 M), and quinine hydrochloride (1.0, 3.2, 10, 32 mM). All solutions were prepared using analytical grade reagents in deionized water and stored at 4°C in screw-capped glass bottles for no longer than one week to preserve their taste qualities. The taste functions were assessed using an ascending-series staircase methodology.Citation13 The task was a two alternative (test stimulus and control), forced-choice, single series procedure, with concentrations presented in ascending order as described elsewhere,Citation13 and based on the method used by Mojet et al.Citation19

In each trial, a participant received two 30 mL disposable plastic cups, one containing 10 mL of taste solution and one containing 10 mL of distilled water, previously brought up to room temperature (22°C–25°C). The two cups were placed side by side in front of the subjects in a left-right position randomly chosen from trial to trial. The sip and spit method was used. At the start of the section and before each trial, the subject rinsed with distilled water and expectorated. The samples, both the blanks and taste solutions, were swirled around the mouth briefly and spat out. After indicating which cup contained the compound, the participant received another pair of samples. Whenever the subject selected incorrectly, the next trial took place at the next higher step. When the subjects selected correctly, the same concentration was presented again. If a given taste was not recognized at any trials, the result of the test was recorded as “scale out”. The lowest concentration at which participants correctly identified the taste was recorded as their recognition threshold, which could range from the lower concentrated solution, numbered as 1, to the higher concentrated solution, numbered as 4. Reduced taste perception was defined as the inability to recognize a taste at the concentrations numbered ≤ 2, according to the results for taste thresholds obtained from a large population of healthy normal-eating free-living elderly individuals, previously enrolled in the multicentric project of which this study formed part.Citation13

Statistical analysis

Normality of distribution was tested with Shapiro–Wilk statistics. Results were expressed as the mean ± standard deviation for normally distributed variables, and as the median with first and third quartiles for non-normally distributed variables. Categorical variables were described as frequency distributions. Differences between free-living and hospitalized elderly subjects were tested using the Student’s unpaired t-test or analysis of variance for normally distributed variables and the non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test for non-normally distributed variables. The Chi-square test was applied to compare categorical distributions. Comparisons of mean recognition thresholds between the inpatients and free-living people were drawn using analysis of variance, and Tukey’s method for multiple comparisons was applied. Differences between groups in the percentages of subjects with reduced taste perception were assessed by logistic regression analysis adjusting for age and interaction term (hospitalization*age). All comparisons between groups were adjusted for age, by the means of linear regression analysis for continuous variables and logistic regression analysis for categorical ones. For each compound, the relationship between reduced taste perception, as the dependent variable (defined as the inability to recognize the taste at concentrations numbered 1 or 2), and several clinical/biological features was assessed using separate simple logistic regression analysis performed on the sample as a whole. The covariates considered in the analysis were age, body mass index [BMI], CIRS comorbidity index, serum albumin levels, number of prescribed drugs, and GDS, MMSE, and MNA scores. The logistic regression analysis were applied by entering covariates one by one, both as quantitative and dichotomous/ranked. Age was dichotomized on the first quartile of the sample distribution and categorized as 65–75 years and over 75 years; BMI was classified in two categories, ie, normal weight (20–24.9 kg/m2), and overweight/obese (≥25 kg/m2). Only four subjects in our sample were underweight (BMI < 20). The BMI threshold of 20 kg/m2 has been commonly accepted as defining underweight in elderly people.Citation20 For the present study, a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 was assumed as a reference category, because overweight/obese status has been associated with a better taste recognition ability in the elderly.Citation21 Adjusted odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were estimated. Variables significantly associated with reduced taste perception were entered in the multiple logistic regression models and adjusted OR with 95% CI were estimated on the whole sample population with “group” (hospitalization) as a confounder, also evaluating for the interaction term (hospitalization*age). The level of statistical significance was set at 0.05. All statistical analysis was performed using SAS software, release 9.2 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC).

Results

Clinical features

The general characteristics of the free-living and hospitalized participants are outlined in . There were no significant differences in gender composition between the two groups (Chi-square test, P = 0.49). The hospitalized subjects were significantly older than the free-living elderly subjects and had a lower mean BMI. Most of the hospitalized subjects were normal-weight or overweight (36% and 42%, respectively, details not shown). The hospitalized elderly were significantly more disabled, depressed, and cognitively impaired. For the inpatients, total MNA scores were lower, whereas comorbidity index and number of prescribed drugs were significantly higher. The community-dwelling participants were almost all fully independent in activities of daily living, functioned well, and had no diseases interfering with their well-being or nutritional status.

Table 1 Characteristics of free-living and hospitalized elderly subjects

Taste functions

shows the cumulative percentage of distribution of the free-living and hospitalized elderly subjects according to their recognition thresholds for quinine, sodium chloride, citric acid, and sucrose. The pattern of the curves for quinine, sodium chloride, and sucrose was similar in the two groups, suggesting that free-living and hospitalized subjects did not differ in their ability to recognize bitter, salty, and sweet stimuli. For citric acid taste, the slope of the curve was markedly flatter for the hospitalized patients, indicating that sour recognition thresholds were significantly higher in hospitalized than in healthy elderly people (P = 0.002, Chi-square test for differences between groups).

Figure 1 Cumulative percentage of distribution of hospitalized and free-living subjects, according to their recognition thresholds, for the four basic taste qualities.

Notes: Concentrations of solution were numbered in ascending order from 1 to 4; “scale out” refers to subjects who failed to recognize the taste at any concentration. Differences in distribution between groups were assessed using the Chi-squared test.
Figure 1 Cumulative percentage of distribution of hospitalized and free-living subjects, according to their recognition thresholds, for the four basic taste qualities.

shows the mean recognition thresholds for the four basic taste qualities in the two groups: hospitalized subjects needed higher concentrations in order to recognize each of the four tastes, although the difference between groups was only statistically significant for citric acid (P = 0.01). The percentage of participants with a reduced taste perception for the four basic taste qualities is shown in . Compared with the free-living elderly, the hospitalized patient group tended to have a higher prevalence of individuals unable to recognize the taste of the solutions at lower concentrations, although the two groups differed to a statistically significant degree only in the case of citric acid (P = 0.0002).

Figure 2 Recognition thresholds of free-living and hospitalized subjects for the four basic taste qualities.

Notes: Values are given as mean solution number and standard error. Asterix indicates a significant difference (P = 0.01) between the mean thresholds for hospitalized and free-living elderly subjects. Differences between groups were assessed using analysis of variance, adjusting for age and for multiple comparisons with Tukey’s method.

Figure 2 Recognition thresholds of free-living and hospitalized subjects for the four basic taste qualities.Notes: Values are given as mean solution number and standard error. Asterix indicates a significant difference (P = 0.01) between the mean thresholds for hospitalized and free-living elderly subjects. Differences between groups were assessed using analysis of variance, adjusting for age and for multiple comparisons with Tukey’s method.

Figure 3 Percentage of free-living and hospitalized subjects with a reduced taste perception for the four basic tastes.

Notes: Differences between groups were tested using simple logistic regression analysis, considering reduced taste perception (inability to recognize the taste at lower concentrations) as the dependent variable, and adjusting for age and interaction effect (hospitalization*age).
Figure 3 Percentage of free-living and hospitalized subjects with a reduced taste perception for the four basic tastes.

Simple logistic regression analysis was conducted for each test solution in the sample as a whole. Statistically significant associations with recognition thresholds were only seen for citric acid () and not for the other three tastes. A significant association emerged between advanced age (> 75 years) and reduced citric acid perception (OR 4.55, CI 95% 1.53–13.41). After adjusting for age, the reduced sour perception was associated, in the whole group, with scores obtained on the GDS, MMSE, and MNA, and with number of drugs. The results of the logistic regression analysis did not change when covariates were entered as quantitative rather than dichotomous/ranked (details not shown).

Table 2 Association of reduced citric acid perception with clinical and biological features

shows the results of the multiple logistic regression analysis used to assess the independent effect of the associated variables on reduced sour taste perception in the sample as a whole. Advanced age (>75 years) and polypharmacy (≥four drugs) were positively related to an increased risk of reduced sour taste perception. When malnutrition risk (MNA score < 23.5) was entered in the model, this remain the only covariate associated with a significantly increased risk (OR 3.6, 95% CI 1.3–11.4, P = 0.03) of a poor ability to recognize citric acid. Similar results were obtained even considering the covariates as continuous variables (details not shown). Forcing into the analysis the confounding effect of the “group” (hospitalization: yes/no) and the interaction term, being hospitalized was associated with an increased risk (OR 7.9, 95% CI 2.7–11.4, P = 0.0001) of poor ability to recognize sour taste.

Table 3 Results of logistic regression analysis, using stepwise selection, on significant independent factors, associated with reduced citric acid perception

Discussion

The results of the present study support the hypothesis that an impaired sense of taste might be more frequent among the hospitalized elderly, but their ability to identify the four basic taste qualities does not seem to be uniformly impaired by comparison with the sense of taste among healthier, free-living elderly subjects. It might be expected that factors such as advanced age, multidrug therapy, comorbidities and malnutrition, conditions which are frequently observed in the frail hospitalized elderly, might be responsible for an overall reduction in taste perception and gustatory ability. However, as evidenced by our findings, sensitivity to sour taste seemed to be the only one affected in our hospitalized elderly patients, while their recognition thresholds for bitter, salty, and sweet tastes did not differ significantly from that of healthier, free-living elderly subjects.

The results of our study highlight that disease status and medications contribute to the well-known age-related taste losses, mainly by altering the ability to perceive sour taste, without interfering with the ability to perceive other tastants.

To our knowledge, only one study has documented a reduced sour perception in frail elderly hospitalized patients, but the sample consisted of only 15 subjects.Citation22 In another study, which included 100 hospitalized subjects, the mean age of participants was 56 years and the taste loss documented was not related to a specific taste.Citation7

It has been recognized that gustatory decline commonly starts towards the end of the sixth decade and becomes more pronounced before 75 years of age.Citation4 Although the design of the present study did not envisage a control group of younger adults, the mean recognition thresholds recorded in our elderly subjects were similar to those reported in a previous study of old people,Citation23 and were significantly higher than those reported for a control group of younger adults (aged 18–25 years), except for the sucrose recognition thresholds, which did not differ between the two age groups. These findings suggest that age affects a person’s sense of sour, salty, and bitter taste perception, but this does not apply to sweet taste. The age-related gustatory decline documented in our study seems to persist even in old-old subjects (>75 years) but only for sour taste, as reported elsewhere.Citation22 Reduction in sour perception also seemed to be related to disease status and drug consumption, as observed in previous research.Citation22,Citation24Citation26

A reduced sense of taste is a common side effect of many drugs, and a larger sample would have been needed to identify the relationship between any one of these drugs and the patients’ taste thresholds. However, it has been well documented that side effects increase exponentially when the number of drugs taken rises to four or more.Citation27 Supratherapeutic drug concentrations in plasma resulting from drug interactions can also alter taste cell biochemistry and/or modify other aspects of the taste pathways,Citation6 so regardless of the types of medication involved, polypharmacy is likely to affect gustatory function, as reported previously.Citation7,Citation22

It is well known that critically ill elderly patients in hospitals and nursing homes consume on average from four to nine drugs.Citation27 Hundreds of medications, including all major drug classes, have been associated clinically with taste complaints such as “loss of taste”, “altered taste”, and “metallic taste”,Citation28 so limiting the number of prescribed drugs might be an insufficient strategy to avoid taste perception losses in frail elderly hospitalized or institutionalized subjects.

Like advanced age and polypharmacy, nutritional status also seems to be related to gustatory function. An impaired sense of taste has been associated with loss of appetite, leading to anorexia, weight loss, and malnutrition.Citation4 Vice versa, a poor nutritional status might induce a decline in gustatory capacity.Citation6 The MNA tool used in the present study to assess our participants’ nutritional status was shown to correlate with several outcomes, including mortality from all causes, hospital stay, and comorbidity,Citation29 and identifying frailer elderly patients.Citation30 To our knowledge, ours is the first study to document an association between malnutrition, or even only the risk of malnutrition, and reduced sensitivity to sour taste. Therefore, further studies are needed to clarify the real nature of this relationship.

In the multiple logistic regression models, when the “group” effect (hospitalization: yes/no) was forced in the analysis as a confounder covariate, hospitalization remained the only variable strongly associated with risk of reduced taste sour recognition. This result does not suggest that admission to an acute geriatric ward represents a risk factor in itself, but it might be possible that being hospitalized sums up the main risk factors for sour perception impairment, ie, advanced age, drug to drug interactions, and poor nutritional status.

The main weakness of our study lies in the fact that the sample cannot be regarded as representative of the general population. Potential selection biases particularly concern the age distribution, given that our hospitalized elderly were significantly older than the free-living control group. The study design did not include a control group of younger subjects or consider the role of olfaction in gustatory properties.Citation31 Moreover, we do not have data on recognition thresholds for the “umami” taste, which has been recognized as the fifth basic taste, as well as sour, sweet, bitter, and salty. Monosodium glutamate has a unique taste quality which is called “umami” in the Japanese language. There is no analogous word to describe this taste quality in the Italian language. Umami taste preference has been shown to be affected by nutritional status, and poorly nourished subjects prefer foods with a higher monosodium glutamate concentration than do well nourished subjects.Citation32 Nevertheless, the results of our study derive from strict methods for testing gustatory capabilities, and a broad array of tests to measure nutritional, health, and cognitive status that have been standardized for older people.

In conclusion, advanced age, polypharmacy, and malnutrition, which are conditions frequently observed in acute geriatric settings, seem to be strongly related to a reduced perception of sour taste, whereas the perception of the other basic tastes seems not to differ significantly from that of healthy, free-living elderly subjects. Given that age-related taste losses cannot be avoided, and limiting the number of prescribed drugs seems to be an unrealistic strategy for preventing taste complaints, much more effort is needed to promote policies improving nutritional status in hospital settings.

Commercial flavors and monosodium glutamate added to food during and after preparation of hospital meals seem to enhance smell and taste properties, improve food palatability and acceptance, increase salivary flow, immunity, plasma protein levels, and grip strength, and reduce oral complaints in critically ill elderly patients.Citation33,Citation34 Given that monosodium glutamate has been shown to add an additional taste without enhancing any other tastes, amplification of flavor and taste (with monosodium glutamate) might be regarded as a useful clinical practice to be adopted in geriatric settings.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • FukunagaAUematsuHSugimotoKInfluences of aging on taste perception and oral somatic sensationJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci20056010911315741292
  • MillerIJJrVariation in human taste bud density as a function of ageAnn N Y Acad Sci19895613073192735687
  • SchiffmanSSTaste and smell losses in normal aging and diseaseJAMA1997278135713629343468
  • NordinSBrämersonABringlövESubstance and tongue-region specific loss in basic taste-quality identification in elderly adultsEur Arch Otorhinolaryngol200726428528917004086
  • NordinSRazaniLJMarkisonSMurphyCAge-associated increases in intensity discrimination for tasteExp Aging Res20032937138112775444
  • SchiffmanSSEffects of aging on the human taste systemAnn N Y Acad Sci2009117072572919686219
  • KettanehAParièsJStirnemannJClinical and biological features associated with taste loss in internal medicine patients. A cross-sectional study of 100 casesAppetite20054416316915808890
  • ParmeleePAThurasPDKatzIRLawtonMPValidation of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale in a geriatric residential populationJ Am Geriatr Soc1995431301377836636
  • LeeRDNiemanDCAssessment of the hospitalized patientLeeRDNiemanDCNutritional Assessment3rd edNew York, NYMcGraw-Hill Higher Education2003
  • ChumleaWCRocheAFSteinbaughMLEstimating stature from knee height for persons 60 to 90 years of ageJ Am Geriatr Soc19953311611203968366
  • VellasBGuigozYBaumgartnerMRelationships between nutritional markers and the mininutritional assessment in 155 older personsJ Am Geriatr Soc2000481300130911037019
  • KatzSDownsTDCashHRGrotzRCProgress in development of the index of ADLGerontologist19701020305420677
  • DoniniLMDominguezLJBarbagalloMSenile anorexia in different geriatric settings in ItalyJ Nutr Health Aging20111577578122089227
  • LawtonMPBrodyEMAssessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily livingGerontologist196991791865349366
  • FolsteinMFRobinsLNHelzerJEThe Mini-Mental State ExaminationArch Gen Psychiatry1983408126860082
  • MagniEBinettiGBianchettiRRozziniRTrabucchiMMini-Mental State Examination: a normative study in the Italian elderly populationEur J Neurol1996319820221284770
  • YesavageJABrinkTLRoseTLDevelopment and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary reportJ Psychiatr Res19821737497183759
  • GoldbergRMMabeeJChanLWongSDrug-drug and drug-disease interactions in the ED: analysis of a high-risk populationAm J Emerg Med1996144474508765105
  • MojetJChrist-HazelhofEHeidemaJTaste perception with age: generic or specific losses in threshold sensitivity to the five basic tastes?Chem Senses20012684586011555480
  • SergiGPerissinottoEPisentCILSA Working Group. An adequate threshold for body mass index to detect underweight condition in elderly persons: the Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging (ILSA)J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci20056086687116079209
  • SimchenUKoebnickCHoyerSIssanchouSZunftHJOdour and taste sensitivity is associated with body weight and extent of misreporting of body weightEur J Clin Nutr20066069870516435003
  • SpitzerMETaste acuity in institutionalized and non-institutionalized elderly menJ Gerontol1988437174
  • AhneGErrasAHummelTKobalGAssessment of gustatory function by means of tasting tabletsLaryngoscope20001101396140110942148
  • SchiffmanSSCritical illness and changes in sensory perceptionProc Nutr Soc20076633134517637085
  • SchiffmanSSThe role of taste and smell in appetite and satiety: impact of chemosensory changes due to aging and drug interactionsWahlqvistMLTruswellASSmithRNestlePJNutrition in a Sustainable EnvironmentProceedings of the XV International Congress of NutritionAdelaideLondon, UKSmith-Gordon1994
  • SchiffmanSSGrahamBGSuggsMSSattely-MillerEAEffect of psychotropic drugs on a range of taste responses in young and elderly personsChem Senses199722786
  • SergiGDe RuiMSartiSManzatoEPolypharmacy in the elderly: can comprehensive geriatric assessment reduce inappropriate medication use?Drugs Aging20112850951821721596
  • SchiffmanSSZervakisJTaste and smell perception in the elderly: effect of medications and diseaseAdv Food Nutr Res20024424734611885138
  • MartinsCPCorreiaJRdo AmaralTFUndernutrition risk screening and length of stay of hospitalized elderlyJ Nutr Elder200525221
  • IzawaSKuzuyaMOkadaKThe nutritional status of frail elderly with care needs according to the mini-nutritional assessmentClin Nutr20062596296716859814
  • KoskinenSKalviainenNTuorilaHPerception of chemosensory stimuli and related responses to flavoured yogurts in the young and elderlyFood Qual Prefer200314623635
  • MurphyCFlavor preference for monosodium glutamate and casein hydrolisate in young and elderly personsKawamuraYKareMRUmami: A Basic TasteNew York, NYMarcel Dekker1987
  • SchiffmanSSIntensification of sensory properties of foods for the elderlyJ Nutr2000130927S930S10736354
  • SchiffmanSSSensory enhancement of foods for the elderly with monosodium glutamate and flavorsFood Rev Int199814321333