288
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Cohort comparisons: emotional well-being among adolescents and older adults

, &
Pages 813-819 | Published online: 10 May 2014

Abstract

Background

There are several negative stereotypes about older adults that have negatively influenced people’s attitude about aging. The present study compared emotional well-being between older adults and adolescents.

Methods

Data for this study came from 1,403 community-dwelling elderly persons and 1,190 secondary school students and were obtained from two national cross-sectional surveys. Emotional well-being was measured using the World Health Organization-Five Well-Being Index. Data analysis was conducted using a multivariate analysis of covariance with SPSS software version 20 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Elderly people significantly scored higher levels of emotional well-being (mean, 62.3; standard deviation, 22.55) than younger people (mean, 57.9; standard deviation, 18.46; t, 5.32; P≤0.001). The findings from the multivariate analysis of covariance revealed a significant difference between older adults and younger people in emotional well-being [F(3, 2587)=120.21; P≤0.001; η2=0.122] after controlling for sex.

Conclusion

Contrary to negative stereotypes about aging, our findings show a higher level of emotional well-being among older adults compared with younger people.

Introduction

There are some commonly held beliefs that aging is associated with increased dependency, loss of self-control, incapacity, ailment, social isolation, and disengagement from life.Citation1 Misconceptions about older people and aging are called “ageism”, “gerontophobia”, or “the elderly mystique”.Citation2 According to Butler’s definition of ageism, it has been defined as “a process of systematic stereotyping and discrimination against people because they are old, just as racism and sexism accomplish this for color and gender”.Citation3 The ageism concept, which appears at the individual, institutional, and societal levels,Citation4 is a composite of three components, including cognitive (stereotypes), affective (prejudice), and behavioral (discrimination) components.Citation5 There are several cultural myths that tend to encourage ageism. The first myth is chronology, which considers people elderly by virtue of their age. The second myth is that of an inflexible personality, which means that as people age, they become more intolerant and conservative. Third is the myth of misery, which suggests that elderly people are unhappy just because they are older. Fourth, there is the myth of rejection and isolation, which holds that elderly people are rejected from society. Fifth is the myth of dependence and unproductiveness, which declares that older adults are not productive members of society because they are not usually engaged in paid employment. Sixth is the myth of physical ill health, which suggests that illness is part of the aging process and that old age is automatically linked to deterioration. Last is the myth of mental deterioration, which suggests that all older people suffer from mental health problems.Citation6 Unfortunately, these myths are reinforced by mass media and gerontological studies, wherein elderly people are generally depicted in a negative light. For example, they are portrayed as immobile; lacking in sex drive; having poor eyesight and hearing; being unable to take care of themselves; being slow, confused, bent, and dowdy; napping; and lacking cognitive abilities.Citation7,Citation8 In addition to the mass media, several gerontological studies reinforce, rather than challenge, such ageist stereotypes.Citation2 For example, the studies focusing on changes in physical and cognitive functioning of elderly people have extremely portrayed old age as a period of losses, loneliness, and declining physical and cognitive functioning.Citation9Citation11

These negative and prevalent impressions toward aging have dramatically influenced health professional decisions and practices, which consequently have resulted in a failure to deal with treatable symptoms.Citation1,Citation12,Citation13 For example, findings from a number of studies show that health professionals often prefer younger people to older adults, as they believe that young people are more productive and have a greater potential to live longer and healthier than older adults do. They may also see health-related problems of elderly people as normal signs of aging, rather than considering signs of illness that may be curable. Unfortunately, these misconceptions have contributed to negative psychological, physical, and health outcomesCitation14 and, consequently, to premature deaths of older adults.Citation12

All these misconceptions state that elderly people have more problems than younger people. Because this negative conception of older adults is becoming prevalent, resulting in the golden years of life becoming years of frustration and resentment, there is an urgent need for research to highlight the possible positive aspects of old age in society. Therefore, it is imperative to do research that may dispel these myths about aging and give a better idea of old age. Considering the above-mentioned misconceptions, this article compares emotional well-being as an important psychosocial marker of health and well-beingCitation15 between older adults and young people. Emotional well-being has been defined as the possession of the flexibility to deal with the inevitable challenges of life conditionsCitation16 and having a positive feeling state comprising the affective aspect (happiness) and cognitive aspect (life satisfaction).Citation17Citation19 It also refers to the quantitative and qualitative experiences of joy, fascination, sadness, anxiety, anger, and affection that make life pleasant or unpleasant.Citation20 The present study compares emotional well-being between older adults and adolescents. The alternative hypothesis of the study was that there would be a significant difference in emotional well-being between older adults and adolescents.

Theoretical framework

The theoretical framework for this study was developed from Carstensen socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) and wisdom theory. The first theory, which was developed by Carstensen, is the SST. The SST comprises three central concepts: selectivity, emotion, and perception of time.Citation21 The awareness of lifetime as a fundamental human characteristic plays an essential role in motivation. Goals are often set within temporal contexts, and goal selection depends fundamentally on the perception of time. The SST focuses on two main classes of psychological goals. The first comprises expansive goals, such as acquiring knowledge or making new social networks. In contrast, the second encompasses goals related to feeling states, such as pursuing positive emotional experiences.Citation22 According to the SST, people’s goal selection is based on their perspectives regarding time. Since most elderly people have already lost their parents and other ascending kin, their awareness of the fact that life is finite increases, and they become more selective about how to spend their remaining lifetime. Instead of trying to spend their time in pursuit of varied experiences, they tend to use their time on activities that enhance their positive emotions and maximize their emotional well-being.Citation21

In contrast, when people consciously or unconsciously perceive time as open-ended (in the case of young people), they are likely to be less selective with their time. They seek to gain resources to build their future, even though the experience is stressful and unpleasant.Citation23 The SST maintains that perception of time horizons influences the selection of goals. When perceived as open-ended, time is associated with seeking new experiences and achievement-oriented stimuli; for example, gathering new information and experiencing new knowledge. In contrast, when time is perceived as finite, it is linked to a stronger focus on emotionally relevant stimuli and interpersonal connectedness. For example, goals emphasize feeling states, mostly regulation of emotional states to optimize well-being.Citation24 According to this theory, aging often involves a decrease in the experience of negative affect and is linked to a stabilization or even a slight increase in the experience of positive affect.Citation25 As people get older and their perceived remaining lifetime is decreased, current emotional goals associated with well-being become more important. Because people selectively attend to information that is consistent with their current goals, information processing in the elderly might be characterized by a positivity bias. Research shows that older adults are more likely to forget negative and more likely to remember positive information compared with younger adults.Citation26 Compared with young people, the elderly were less likely to reserve negative information, but not positive information.Citation25 Older adults people have a more positive perspective than younger people.Citation27

Another theory that may explain possible generational differences in emotional well-being is wisdom theory. Wisdom as a key factor in the creation of a good life and has been defined as expertise in the conduct and meaning of life that is associated with a high degree of personal and interpersonal competence, such as the ability to listen, evaluate, and give counsel.Citation28 Although wisdom has been widely defined, there are some agreements that it involves the use of certain kinds of pragmatic reasoning to navigate important life challenges.Citation29 Baltes and Smith also define wisdom as having useful knowledge to deal with life problems, including an awareness of the varied contexts of life, acknowledgement that values and life goals vary among individuals and groups, and recognition of the uncertainties of life, along with ways to deal with them.Citation30 This reflects that wisdom influences people’s approach to life conflicts. Because growing older may be related to the acquisition of wisdom-related concepts about life development,Citation31Citation34 it is believed that older persons are aware of ontogenetic changes, which may include the positivity effect. Within this theory, a wise person is someone who can understand what is most important in life and knows the way to reach it. The wise person knows the things that make life worthwhile and meaningful.Citation28 According to the earlier discussion, it could be speculated that older adults may have higher levels of emotional well-being compared with adolescents.

Methodology

Data for this study were obtained from two surveys in Malaysia: Mental and Spiritual Well-Being among Adolescents (MSWBA)Citation35 and Patterns of Social Relationship and Psychological Well-Being Among Older Persons in Peninsular Malaysia (PSRPWO).Citation36 In both of these surveys, emotional well-being was measured using the World Health Organization (WHO)-Five Well-Being Index (WHO-5).Citation35,Citation36 The MSWBA was conducted from 2009 to 2010. The survey employed a self-group-administered questionnaire, which was distributed to 1,200 Malay students selected using a multistage proportional stratified random sampling method. The MSWBA was carried out at two stages. In the first stage, a simple random sampling technique was used to select schools from different districts. Finally, the survey questionnaires were administered to all students in the selected schools, resulting in a sample of 1,200 respondents. Of the 1,200 questionnaires distributed, 1,190 were returned. The PSRPWO was carried out from 2007 to 2009. The focus of the study was on a pattern of social relationship and psychological well-being in Peninsular Malaysia. The details of methodology have been published in full elsewhere.Citation35,Citation36 Briefly, a multistage cluster sampling design was used to obtain a representative sample of community-dwelling elderly persons living in Peninsular Malaysia. The PSRPWO employed a face-to-face interview technique to collect data in respondents’ homes with standardized questionnaires by trained interviewers. A subsample of 1,403 elderly Malay aged 60 years and older was used for the present study.

Measure

Emotional well-being was measured using the WHO-5, a well-validated and widely used measure of positive well-being. The scale covers positive mood (good spirits, relaxation), vitality (being active and waking up fresh and rested), and general interests (being interested in things).Citation37 Because the WHO-5 well-being index was originally developed for adults, it is important to assess the readability of the measure. For this purpose, the Flesch Reading Ease (FRE) was used. The FRE rates text on a 100-point scale on the basis of the average number of syllables per word and words per sentence. The higher the FRE score, the better the readability.Citation38 The FRE score obtained was 85.58, indicating that the WHO-5 could be read and understood by those with a ninth-grade reading level.

Results

The elderly group comprised 1,403 community-dwelling elderly persons aged 60 years or older, with approximately equal sex distribution (50.8% women, 49.2% men). The young group included 534 male and 656 female secondary school students with a median age of 16 years. displays the sociodemographic characteristics of the subjects by age group.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the subjects by age group

Elderly people scored significantly higher levels of emotional well-being (mean, 62.3; standard deviation, 22.55) than younger participants (mean, 57.9; standard deviation, 18.46; t=5.32; P≤0.001). Using the commonly used criterion of a WHO-5 score lower than 52, suggesting poor emotional well-being,Citation39,Citation40 29.4% of older adults compared with 34.7% of young people reported poor emotional well-being (χ2=8.46; P<0.01).

A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used to compare the emotional well-being of older adults and young people (positive mood, emotional vitality, and general interests) after controlling for sex. Before conducting the MANCOVA, a series of Pearson correlations were conducted between all of the dependent variables to examine the assumption that the dependent variables would be moderately correlated with each other.Citation41 As shown in , a meaningful pattern of correlations was observed among all of the dependent variables, suggesting the appropriateness of a MANCOVA. In addition, Box’s test of equality of covariance was significant (Box’s M=74.94; P<0.001). This means that the assumption of equality of covariance matrices has been violated, which tests the null hypothesis that the covariance matrices between the groups are equal. However, this is not much of a problem with a large sample size. Therefore, because Pillai’s criterion is most robust for violations of test assumptions,Citation42 it was used in interpreting the MANCOVA results.

Table 2 Means, standard deviations, and correlation matrix of the emotional well-being subscales

As presented in , the results of MANCOVA revealed a statistically significant difference between older adults and young people on the combined dependent variables [F(3, 2587)=120.21; P≤0.001; η2=0.122]. The main effect of sex was also significant [F(3, 2587)=18.06; P≤0.001; η2=0.02]. However, the multivariate effect size for main effect of sex was small.

Table 3 The results of multivariate analysis of covariance

Finally, univariate analyses for each dependent variable as follow-up tests to the MANCOVA, using the Bonferroni method for controlling type I error rates, were conducted to examine individual mean difference across two groups and well-being subscales. As shown in and , the results revealed a significant mean difference between older adults and young people in positive mood [F(1, 2591)=125.63; P≤0.001; η2=0.05], vitality [F(1, 2591)=25.09; P≤0.001; η2=0.01], and general interests [F(1, 2591)=57.70; P≤0.001; η2=0.02].

Table 4 The means and standard deviations of the emotional well-being subscales for two groups

Overall, MANCOVA results indicate significant group differences in emotional well-being, wherein elderly people score higher on the emotional well-being scale than young respondents.

Discussion

The current study was designed to compare emotional well-being between older adults and young people. The findings from this study, as hypothesized, revealed that older adults had a higher level of emotional well-being compared with young people. Our findings supported and extended existing research attempting to dispel misconceptions about aging. For example, Lee and Knight,Citation43 in their study on a sample of 103 young people (mean age, 19.95 years; standard deviation, 2.12) and 44 older adults (mean age, 71.75; standard deviation, 6.35) found that older persons have significantly lower anxiety scores than the young people.Citation43 Likewise, the findings from another study aiming to compare the psychological well-being of elderly people (mean age, 73 years) with that of young people (mean age, 20 years) showed that older adults significantly scored lower than young people on several indices of psychopathology including trait anxiety, neuroticism, schizotypal personality, rumination, and thought suppression.Citation44 Our results are also consistent with findings from a recent study conducted on a random large sample size among adult Americans. It is generally believed that elderly people suffer from sleep problems more than younger people, but the findings from Grandner et al’s study reveal that sleep quality appears to improve over a lifetime.Citation45 The results are also consistent with Carstensen et al’s studyCitation46 showing that older adults, compared with young people, have a higher ability to escape bad moods. Similarly, our findings are also consistent with the recent study conducted by Stone et al,Citation47 who found that older persons were happier and less stressed than younger people.

These findings can also support the hypothesis that emotional well-being may remain stable and intact in old age, and that despite having social losses, loneliness, declining physical and mental functioning, and cognitive impairments, elderly people have higher emotional well-being.Citation48,Citation49

The higher levels of emotional well-being of older adults compared with adolescents can be explained by Carstensen et al’sCitation50 SST, which maintains that as people age and perceive their time horizons become shorter, they focus on more immediate goals that elicit positive emotions. Young people, in contrast, often need to explore or take risks to achieve longer-range goals, and they experience stress and frustration.Citation50 The results also agree with Baltes’Citation51 theory, in which wisdom and emotional intelligence increase with age. In other words, as people get older, their abilities to understand and develop strategies to avoid or at least lessen the possible emotional conflicts increase.

A further explanation to justify why elderly people compared with younger adults have a higher level of emotional well-being has been provided by Isaacowitz,Citation52 who found that elderly people, when compared with young people, prefer positive outlook patterns and show the most positive outlook, which helps them regulate their mood and, consequently, leads to better emotional well-being.Citation52 He also found that in certain situations, young people mostly pay more attention to emotionally negative information, and elderly people are more likely to pay attention to emotionally positive information. Compared with young age, elderly people are more able to process positive stimuli while ignoring spontaneous reactions to negative stimuli.Citation53 As people get older, they are more emotionally balanced and better able to solve emotional problems. There is also evidence that older adults are more positive and less negative than younger adults.Citation54 There is also more evidence that as people grow older, negativity preference reduces compared with increased positivity preference. In other words, elderly people tend to see the good things in life more easily and are less likely to be affected by negative events. This evidence implies that aging is not characterized by an overoptimistic view of life but, instead, by an increasing ability to be less affected by negative events.Citation55 The theory maintains that increased attention to emotional goals leads to greater complexity of emotional experience and, consequently, better regulation of emotions experienced in everyday life.Citation22 Another possibility that can explain higher levels of emotional well-being among older adults compared with young people is that they are more religious than young people and attach a high value to religious beliefs that strengthen them to enable effective coping with stress and losses.Citation56,Citation57 In Malaysia, where culture and religion overlap, the elderly Malays view the process of aging as God’s will and believe that this world is temporary and there is a life after death, which enables them to view stressful experiences as less threatening and to cope more effectively with age-related changes and losses. Therefore, their religious beliefs and practice positively influence their emotional well-being.Citation58,Citation59

Although the results of this study show that elderly people had higher levels of emotional well-being compared with adolescents, it is worth mentioning that the pattern of greater well-being in old age may represent the well-being paradox, which refers to the notion that older adults report high psychological well-being despite increased age-related changes and life challenges.Citation60 The review of the literature focusing on the well-being in old age reveals several reasons that many older adults paradoxically report high levels of well-being and life satisfaction. First, the high pattern of well-being in old age may not generalize to all dimensions of well-being. Second, this pattern of the association between well-being and age may exist only in rich countries. Third, age: well-being associations may vary across sex and ethnicity. Another reason that older adults may, paradoxically, report higher levels of well-being is as a result of the inevitable influence of survivorship and the fact that people with lower well-being die earlier.Citation61,Citation62

Conclusion

Contrary to negative ageist stereotypes of miserable elderly, the findings from the present study showed that elderly people compared with young people have better emotional well-being. The findings support SST. With regard to the well-known powerful influence of mass media on people’s opinions, it is suggested that the findings from the present study be employed by the media to demonstrate more positive images of older people, which may promote positive attitudes toward aging. As a last word, it is noteworthy that although negative perceptions of older people are not totally without foundation,Citation63 the problems of the elderly are greatly exaggerated. In other words, it can be argued that increasing problems with age are a nonlinear phenomenon that may be mediated and moderated by factors other than physiologic aging.

The most important limitation of the present study that should be highlighted is its cross-sectional design, which makes it difficult to make a precise conclusion regarding emotional well-being trends over time. Therefore, a further study using a longitudinal approach is needed to assess changes of emotional well-being across the life span.

Acknowledgments

This article is based on two studies funded by Universiti Putra Malaysia through the Research Universiti Grant Scheme (RUGS-91827) and the Ministry of Science, Technology and Innovation under ScienceFund Grant (04-01-04-SF0479).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • GridleyHWellsYBrowningCPsychology and ageing: Contributions to the International Year of Older PersonsPaper presented at: 34th annual meeting of the Australian Psychological SocietySeptember 1999Hobart
  • PalmoreEAdvantages of agingGerontologist1979192220224263601
  • ButlerRNWhy Survive? Being Old in AmericaNew YorkHarper & Row1975
  • ButlerRNCombating ageismInt Psychogeriatr200921221118817584
  • IversenTNLarsenLSolemPEA conceptual analysis of ageismNordic Psychology2009613422
  • McInnesAMeldrew or Methuselah: The mythology of old ageGenerations Rev20041442224
  • VasilLWassHPortrayal of the elderly in the media: literature review and implications for educational gerontologistsEducational Gerontology1993197185
  • CuddyAJCFiskeSTDoddering but dear: process, content, and function in stereotyping of older personsNelsonTAgeism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older PersonsBostonMIT Press2002326
  • MomtazYAHamidTAIbrahimRUnmet needs among disabled elderly MalaysiansSoc Sci Med201275585986322632847
  • MomtazYAHamidTAYusoffSLoneliness as a risk factor for hypertension in later lifeJ Aging Health201224469671022422758
  • MomtazYAHamidTANurizanYIbrahimRWidowhood and psychological well-being among older Malayslans: Mediating effect of social networkIndian J Social Work2009703375390
  • NolanLCDimensions of aging and belonging for the older person and the effects of ageismBYU J Public Law2011252317339
  • RosowskyEAgeism and professional training in aging: Who will be there to help? Generations20052935558
  • RashidAOngEKWongESYThe attitude towards ageing among residents of an elderly care institution in Penang MalaysiaInt J Collaborative Res Intern Med Public Health20124610691083
  • BrownHEGilsonNDBurtonNWBrownWJDoes physical activity impact on presenteeism and other indicators of workplace well-being?Sports Med201141324926221395366
  • Tom-OrmeLTraditional beliefs and attitudes about diabetes among Navajos and UtesJoeJRYoungRSDiabetes as a Disease of Civilization: Impact of Culture in Indigenous PeopleNew YorkMouton de Gruyter1994271291
  • HillmanKMcMillanJLife Satisfaction of Young Australians: Relationships between Further Education, Training and Employment and General and Career SatisfactionMelbourneAustralian Council for Educational Research2005 Available from: http://research.acer.edu.au/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1046&context=lsay_researchAccessed March 31, 2014
  • CicognaniEAlbanesiCZaniBThe impact of residential context on adolescents’ subjective well beingJo Community Appl Social Psychol2008186558575
  • LeeGRIshii-KuntzMSocial interaction, loneliness, and emotional well-being among the elderlyRes Aging1987944594823438563
  • KahnemanDDeatonAHigh income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-beingProc Natl Acad Sci U S A201010738164891649320823223
  • CarstensenLLIsaacowitzDMCharlesSTTaking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivityAm Psychol199954316518110199217
  • CarstensenLLFungHHCharlesSTSocioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of lifeMotivation Emotion2003272103123
  • KalatJWShiotaMNEmotionBelmont, CAWadsworth Publishing2007
  • CarstensenLLThe influence of a sense of time on human developmentScience200631257821913191516809530
  • GoelevenEDe RaedtRDierckxEThe positivity effect in older adults: the role of affective interference and inhibitionAging Ment Health201014212913720336546
  • CharlesSTMatherMCarstensenLLAging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adultsJ Exp Psychol Gen2003132231032412825643
  • SullivanSJMikelsJACarstensenLLYou never lose the ages you’ve been: affective perspective taking in older adultsPsychol Aging201025122923420230142
  • BaltesPBStaudingerUMWisdom. A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellenceAm Psychol200055112213611392856
  • GrossmannIKarasawaMIzumiSAging and wisdom: culture mattersPsychol Sci201223101059106622933459
  • BaltesPBSmithJThe fascination of wisdom: its nature, ontogeny, and functionPerspectives Psychol Sci2008315664
  • JesteDVArdeltMBlazerDKraemerHCVaillantGMeeksTWExpert consensus on characteristics of wisdom: a Delphi method studyGerontologist201050566868020233730
  • BaltesPBStaudingerUMMaerckerASmithJPeople nominated as wise: a comparative study of wisdom-related knowledgePsychol Aging19951021551667662175
  • GlückJBaltesPBUsing the concept of wisdom to enhance the expression of wisdom knowledge: not the philosopher’s dream but differential effects of developmental preparednessPsychol Aging200621467969017201489
  • WoodwardKAgainst wisdom: The social politics of anger and agingCultural Critique200251186218
  • YahayaNMomtazYAOthmanMSulaimanNMatFSpiritual well-being and mental health among Malaysian adolescentsLife Science Journal201291440448
  • Abolfathi MomtazYHamidTAIbrahimRYahayaNAbdullahSSModerating effect of Islamic religiosity on the relationship between chronic medical conditions and psychological well-being among elderly MalaysPsychogeriatrics2012121435322416828
  • BechPMeasuring the dimension of psychological general well-being by the WHO-5Quality of Life Newsl2004321516
  • StacksDWPrimer of Public Relations ResearchNew YorkGuilford2002
  • PouwerFGeelhoed-DuijvestijnPHTackCJPrevalence of comorbid depression is high in out-patients with Type 1 or Type 2 diabetes mellitus. Results from three out-patient clinics in The NetherlandsDiabet Med201027221722420546267
  • RoyTLloydCEParvinMMohiuddinKGRahmanMPrevalence of co-morbid depression in out-patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus in BangladeshBMC Psychiatry20121212322909306
  • MeyersLSGamstGGuarinoAJApplied Multivariate Research: Design and InterpretationThousand Oaks, CASage Publishers2006
  • JohnsonCRFieldCAUsing fixed-effects model multivariate analysis of variance in marine biology and ecologyOceanogr Marine Biol Annu Rev199331177221
  • LeeLOKnightBGAttentional bias for threat in older adults: moderation of the positivity bias by trait anxiety and stimulus modalityPsychol Aging200924374174719739931
  • ErskineJAKvavilashviliLConwayMAMyersLThe effects of age on psychopathology, well-being and repressive copingAging Ment Health200711439440417612803
  • GrandnerMAMartinJLPatelNPAge and sleep disturbances among American men and women: data from the US Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance SystemSleep201235339540622379246
  • CarstensenLLPasupathiMMayrUNesselroadeJREmotional experience in everyday life across the adult life spanJ Pers Soc Psychol200079464465511045744
  • StoneAASchwartzJEBroderickJEDeatonAA snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United StatesProc Natl Acad Sci U S A2010107229985999020479218
  • HahnJOishiSPsychological needs and emotional well-being in older and younger Koreans and AmericansPersonality Individual Differences2006404689698
  • SmithJFleesonWGeiselmannBSetterstenRKunzmannUSources of well-being in old ageBaltesPBMayerKUThe Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100CambridgeCambridge University Press1999450471
  • CarstensenLLTuranBScheibeSEmotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience samplingPsychol Aging2011261213320973600
  • BaltesPBExtending longevity: Dignity gain – or dignity drain?Max Planck Res200331419
  • IsaacowitzDMMood Regulation in Real-Time: Age Differences in the Role of LookingCurr Dir Psychol Sci201221423724223139458
  • FengMCCourtneyCGMatherMDawsonMEDavisonGCAge-related affective modulation of the startle eyeblink response: older adults startle most when viewing positive picturesPsychol Aging201126375276021463060
  • von HippelWHenryJDMatovicDAging and social satisfaction: offsetting positive and negative effectsPsychol Aging200823243543918573016
  • IsaacowitzDMTonerKGorenDWilsonHRLooking while unhappy: mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adultsPsychol Sci200819984885318947348
  • MehrotraCMWagnerLSAging and Diversity: An Active Learning ExperienceWashington, DCTaylor and Francis1998
  • BerkLEDevelopment through the Lifespan5th edBoston, MAPearson, Allyn and Bacon2010
  • MomtazYAIbrahimRHamidTAYahayaNMediating effects of social and personal religiosity on the psychological well being of widowed elderly peopleOmega (Westport)201061214516220712141
  • MomtazYAHamidTAIbrahimRYahayaNChaiSTModerating effect of religiosity on the relationship between social isolation and psychological well-beingMental Health Religion Culture2012142141156
  • WiesmannUHannichHJA salutogenic analysis of the well-being paradox in older ageJ Happiness Stud20132014152339355
  • HansenTSlagsvoldBThe age and subjective well-being paradox revisited: a multidimensional perspectiveNorsk Epidemiol2012222187195
  • KunzmannULittleTDSmithJIs age-related stability of subjective well-being a paradox? Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from the Berlin Aging StudyPsychol Aging200015351152611014714
  • ArnoldJManaging Careers into the 21st CenturyLondonPaul Chapman1997