740
Views
33
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

“I am active”: effects of a program to promote active aging

&
Pages 829-837 | Published online: 05 May 2015

Abstract

Background

Active aging involves a general lifestyle strategy that allows preservation of both physical and mental health during the aging process. “I am Active” is a program designed to promote active aging by increased physical activity, healthy nutritional habits, and cognitive functioning. The purpose of this study was to assess the effectiveness of this program.

Methods

Sixty-four healthy adults aged 60 years or older were recruited from senior centers and randomly allocated to an experimental group (n=31) or a control group (n=33). Baseline, post-test, and 6-month follow-up assessments were performed after the theoretical–practical intervention. Effect sizes were calculated.

Results

At the conclusion of the program, the experimental group showed significant improvement compared with the control group in the following domains: physical activity (falls risk, balance, flexibility, self-efficacy), nutrition (self-efficacy and nutritional status), cognitive performance (processing speed and self-efficacy), and quality of life (general, health and functionality, social and economic status). Although some declines were reported, improvements at follow-up remained in self-efficacy for physical activity, self-efficacy for nutrition, and processing speed, and participants had better nutritional status and quality of life overall.

Conclusion

Our findings show that this program promotes improvements in domains of active aging, mainly in self-efficacy beliefs as well as in quality of life in healthy elders.

Introduction

Population aging is taking place worldwide, and has major social, economic, and health consequences. While people are living longer lives almost everywhere, the prevalence of non-communicable diseases and disability is increasing as populations experience aging.Citation1

The World Health Organization (WHO)Citation2 considers that “active aging” is a key concept allowing people to realize their own potential, living their own aging as a positive experience free of disability, with continuing opportunities for health, participation, and security, especially in aging societies like ours. The theoretical WHO model of active aging involves several determinants related to health and social services, economics, and the social and physical environment, as well as personal and behavioral factors embedded in cultural and sex contexts.

The concept of active aging has been developed both at the political and the individual level. Politically speaking, it has been proposed as a strategy that connects key policy issues (employment, retirement, health, and citizenship) with health, and suggests that active aging involves a general lifestyle strategy to preserve both physical and mental health during the aging process.Citation3 As a political strategy, there are population-based initiatives focused on promotion of active aging. Outstanding examples are “Active Ageing Australia”,Citation4 “Active for Life”,Citation5 the “Building Healthy Communities for Active Aging National Recognition Program”,Citation6,Citation7 and the “International Council on Active Aging”;Citation8 in the European Union, 2012 was named as the “European Year for Active Ageing and Solidarity between Generations”.Citation9

However, aging is not only a population phenomenon, but an experience and an individual reality,Citation10 and even though active aging is well considered as a political strategy, at the individual level there is a lack of agreement in academic spheres regarding its concept and definition.Citation11,Citation12 Currently, the most widely used terms are: “successful aging” based on Rowe and Kahn’s model,Citation13,Citation14 defined as free of disease and disability, high cognitive and physical functioning, and social engagement, and “healthy” or “optimal” aging. This conceptual disagreement becomes important when analyzing practical interventions.

Moreover, there are a number of intervention programs designed as strategies to promote active aging which are implemented in groups of individuals. These programs address different dimensions and strategies to promote active aging.

For example, “vital aging” is an individual active aging promotion program developed on the basis of four domains as determinant factors of active aging, ie, health, cognitive and physical functioning, affect and control, and social participation. It is implemented through three different modalities, ie, life, multimedia, and e-learning, and has been implemented in several editions, particularly in Spain. In general, the results show improving physical exercise, diet, improved memory, improved emotional balance, and better social relationships, and participants enjoyed more cultural, intellectual, affective, and social activities than before. It should be said that this program had different results among program versions.Citation15 Other interventions, also based on the WHO model, focused on health-promoting behaviors in the elderly, such as physical activity, nutrition, stress management, spiritual growth, interpersonal relationships, and health, dimensions considered to implement an educational program based on a successful aging approach. After 12 months, results for 464 elderly people showed that participants in the experimental group improved their awareness of facts about aging and better health behaviors.Citation16

Other studies have implemented intervention programs at the individual level as strategies to promote “active”, “successful”, or “optimal” aging based on only one dimension, such as physical activity, emotional well-being, intergenerational relationships, or promotion of leisure activities. “The California Active Aging Community Grant Program” is a choice-based, telephone-assisted intervention model in different community settings designed to promote active aging through a training intervention to increase physical activity in older people. Data from 447 participants revealed favorable levels of program satisfaction and significant increases in physical activity.Citation17 Similarly, the “Program for Active Aging and Community Engagement” is also focused on promoting physical activity among the elderly. Two interventions, based on educational sessions or a prosocial behavior physical activity, demonstrated not only increased physical activity but also improvements in physical function and health-related quality of life after 12 months.Citation18

On the other hand, some active aging programs have been designed based specifically on enhancing emotional well-being. Training strategies were based on life review techniques of specific positive events in non-depressed elderly. Results show that the experimental group had decreased depressive symptomatology, improved life satisfaction, and increased autobiographical memory.Citation19 According to other authors, successful aging strategies are reflected in interest in outdoor adventure activities (such as mountain biking and backpacking). These kinds of activities are considered as positive leisure experiences that include challenging physical activity, social engagement, and the natural environment. This community-based program was assessed using qualitative and quantitative data, and outcomes showed psychological and physical benefits through improvements in health and well-being.Citation20 Meanwhile “The Optimal Aging Program” emphasizes the importance of intergenerational relationships to promote active aging. This is a longitudinal mentoring program that pairs college students with older adults who are considered to be aging successfully. Its main goals are to provide students with an opportunity to develop a relationship with an older adult who continues to be active in her/his community and expand their concept of aging to include the reality of healthy, active older adulthood.Citation21

The intervention program “I am Active” was designed in this study. It takes into account the theoretical model of the WHO, considering active aging as an outcome of different determinant factors that place people in profiles, that are more at risk or, on the other hand, are more favorable to age actively.Citation22 This program was an intervention on behavioral and personal dimensions of active aging. The strategies used were focused on promoting physical activity, better nutrition, and better cognitive function. “I am Active” considers that one of the main risks for aging is disability, which can be preventable by adequate intervention strategiesCitation23 and healthy nutrition.Citation24 It also includes cognitive function, based on the evidence that learning ability remains across old age,Citation25 improved cognitive function prevents cognitive impairmentCitation26 and its evident progression to dementia,Citation27,Citation28 considered to be one of the most disabling and expensive diseases of old age.Citation29

The purpose of this research was to determine the impact on active aging of the intervention program “I am Active”, which promotes physical activity, improved nutrition and cognitive function, as well as quality of life in people aged 60 years and older.

Materials and methods

Participants

Sixty-four older persons participated in this randomized controlled trial, which included baseline, post-test, and 6-month follow-up assessments. Participants were recruited from senior centers based on the following inclusion criteria: age 60 years or older, availability to attend sessions at least twice a week, willingness to participate in the program, and being literate. Exclusion criteria were: depressive symptomatology measured by the Spanish version of the Geriatric Depression ScaleCitation30 and cognitive impairment determined by the Mini-Mental State Examination,Citation31 translated and validated in Mexico by age, sex, and education.Citation32

The experimental group included 31 participants (attrition 13.9%) and 33 control participants (attrition 9.1%). The exclusion of both the study and analysis of those who did not complete the study did not alter the similarity between groups.

The intervention

“I am Active” is a program designed to promote active aging in persons aged 60 years and older by stimulating and improving physical activity, nutrition, and cognitive functioning, and seeks to promote better quality of life. Its specific objectives are to stimulate and improve physical activity, encourage and promote healthy eating behaviors, and improve working memory and processing speed.

The participants in the intervention group received a “user manual” specifically designed for this purpose. The program lasted 2 months, and consisted of 2-hour group sessions, held twice a week (ie, 16 sessions in total). The participants in the control group remained on a wait list and participated in the program once the study was completed, participating in the meantime in weekly social activities organized by the senior center.

All sessions were both theoretical and practical and had similarities in their structure. Eight lessons were focused on nutritional topics and eight sessions on cognitive functioning and were presented alternately ().

Table 1 Contents of the “I am Active” program

Each session started with reality orientation techniques, where participants were asked about orientating information, such as time (date, day of the week, time, season, current events), place (address, streets around the site, city, state, cardinal points), and person (name, age, who in the group were absent or present). This initial activity was called “daily news”. The next 30 minutes of each session were focused on physical activity, where physical exercises were performed to work on muscular strength, balance, and mobility, and included a discussion of basic concepts concerning fact and fiction related to physical exercise, and benefits, types, and levels of intensity during exercises, preventive measures, and self-monitoring.

After the physical activity was finished, the trainer gave a general presentation of the session’s content, presenting supporting evidence on each issue. In sessions related to nutrition, general concepts were reviewed, along with truth and fiction about nutritional facts, healthy proportions of each of the principle food groups and types of nutrients, and good eating behaviors. Later, practical exercises were undertaken, including how to plan healthier dishes on a budget, both on a daily basis and when choosing from a restaurant menu. In the sessions related to cognitive function, the trainer explained the theory of the changes that occur in cognitive aging, and memory and practical activities were developed based on information organization, visualization, or association strategies to improve or activate both memory and attention as well as speed processing tasks like pairing images in the least possible time.

Both topics and practical exercises were included based on literature reviews, and chosen according to their effectiveness. At the end of each session, the trainer made some concluding remarks and homework was explained. As reinforcement, at the end of the intervention, each participant designed his or her own plan and set personal goals. Follow-up group sessions were held monthly during the next 6 months to enhance adherence efficacy.

Outcome measures of active aging

The experimental group and the control group were evaluated at baseline, post-test, and after 6 months of follow-up. Participants were assessed at home by a previously trained team comprising a psychologist and a dietitian. The following outcomes were assessed for all participants.

Physical activity

Balance, gait, and risk of falling were assessed using the Tinetti scale. A goniometer was used to measure the range of motion (in degrees) of the arms when moving sideways and forward, which is regarded as a measure of flexibility. Maximal grip strength (in kg) was measured in both hands using a hand-held dynamometer. Self-efficacy for physical activity was measured by estimating the strength of one’s belief in the ability to perform regular physical activity on a scale from 0 (“I cannot do it”) to 6 (“sure, I can do it”). Citation33

Nutrition

Nutritional status was measured using the Mini-Nutritional Assessment.Citation34 Body mass index was calculated based on WHO parametersCitation35 using a digital bioimpedance scale (Tanita Inner Scan BC-558) for body composition. Height was measured using an anthropometric tape. Self-efficacy for improving nutrition was measured by estimating the strength of beliefs in the ability to do so, using a scale designed according to Bandura parameters.Citation33

Cognitive function

Working memory was assessed by the Digit Span Backward SubtestCitation36 and processing speed by the Digit Symbol Subtest.Citation36 Self-efficacy to improve memory was determined using a scale designed especially for this purpose, according to Bandura parameters.Citation33

Quality of life

A Spanish version of the Quality of Life IndexCitation37 was used to assess general quality of life as well as its specific dimensions, ie, health and functioning, psychological/spiritual, socioeconomic, and family.

Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 18 software (Chicago, IL, USA). The data were processed to obtain proportions, means, and their standard deviations. Changes in active aging domains and quality of life in the experimental and control groups were compared to evaluate the effectiveness of the “I am Active” program. Comparisons were performed by chi-squared test, the Student’s t-test, and repeated measures analysis of variance. Accordingly, when reporting and interpreting intervention studies, a P-value can inform whether an effect exists but not reveal the size of it. The magnitude of the difference between groups, or effect size, was calculated as a main finding of the quantitative study, thus both the substantive significance (effect size) and statistical significance (P-value) are considered essential results to be reported.Citation38 In this study, Cohen’s d was used to determine the effect size, which could be classified as small (d=0.2), medium (d=0.5), or large (d ≥ 0.8).Citation39

Results

The sociodemographic characteristics of participants in the experimental and control groups are shown in . The groups were comparable for age, sex, marital status, and disease status. There was a trend of higher schooling in the experimental (5.55 years) group when compared with control group (3.97 years). The results of the intervention on the dimensions of active aging, ie, physical activity, cognitive functioning, and nutrition, are shown in .

Table 2 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants

Table 3 Outcome measures of active aging dimensions, ie, physical activity, cognitive functioning, and nutrition (experimental n=31, control n=33)

The groups were similar for physical activity dimensions at baseline (all P<0.05), but by the end of the study, the intervention group had a lower risk of falls than the control group (P<0.05, d=034, indicating a small to medium effect size. However, at follow-up, a decline was observed and performance returned almost to their initial values.

The same pattern was seen in measures of balance, with participants in the intervention group improving in the post-test (P<0.05) compared with the control group (effect size d=0.41 considered as medium) but decreasing at follow-up to the baseline value. Similarly, the intervention group showed improved arm flexibility at post-test (P<0.05; effect size d=0.65, considered as medium to large); however, decreased performance was noted at follow-up. No change in risk of falls, balance, or flexibility was found in the control group. No effects of the program were seen in measures of gait and grip strength in either group.

Regarding self-efficacy for physical activity, the experimental group improved in the post-test (P<0.01) with a large effect size (d=0.77), and this improvement was maintained at follow-up, when the effect size was increased (d=0.86) compared with baseline. The control group showed a significant change in a negative direction at the post-test.

Regarding the cognitive functioning dimension, the experimental group showed significantly better performance on processing speed at the post-test (P<0.001), with a medium effect size (d=0.50), and this improvement was maintained at follow-up (P<0.001), with an effect size of d=0.44. The control group showed no significant changes. No significant changes in working memory performance were found in either group. Self-efficacy for improving memory showed a significant improvement in the pre-test/post-test comparison (P<0.001) in the experimental group, with a large effect size of d=0.89, but declined during follow-up. The control group showed no significant changes.

Improvements were observed for the nutritional dimension of active aging in the experimental group, where participants showed greater self-efficacy for nutrition after intervention (P<0.01), with a medium to large effect size (d=0.62), and although there was a small decline at follow-up, the improvement was maintained (P>0.05), with an effect size of d=033. No significant changes were noted in the control group.

Both groups were similar for nutritional status at baseline, with 54.8% of the experimental group and 57.6% in the control group having normal nutritional status and 45.2% and 42.4%, respectively, being at risk of malnutrition (). After the intervention groups were statistically different, in the experimental group decreased the proportion of participants at risk of malnutrition (29%) while increased those with normal nutritional status (71 %) and maintained at follow-up. The proportion of controls with normal nutritional status decreased to 42.4% (P<0.05) in the post-test but increased back to baseline values at follow-up. However, differences between the experimental and control groups were statistically significant (P<0.05) at the post-test and follow-up.

Table 4 Nutritional variables of active aging (experimental n=31, control n=33)

There was a statistically significant difference in body mass index between the two groups at baseline (P=0.03), with a higher proportion of overweight participants in the control group, but no changes were observed at the post-test or follow-up assessments.

As shown in , the experimental group reported improvements in overall quality of life at the post-test assessment (P<0.01), with a medium to large effect size (d=0.63), which was maintained at follow-up (P<0.01, d=0.63). Specifically, it was found that participants in the program showed improvements after the intervention (post-test) in quality of life dimensions, health, and functionality (P<0.01, d=0.55) and social and economic status (P<0.05, d=0.59), with medium effect sizes of d=0.55 and d=0.59, respectively, which declined at follow-up to small effect sizes (d=0.38 and d=0.27). No significant changes in psychological/spiritual or family-specific quality of life dimensions were found in the experimental group. The control group showed no significant changes in any measure of quality of life.

Table 5 Quality of life, outcomes in general and specific domains (experimental n=31, control n=33)

Discussion

The findings show in general that the “I am Active” program promotes improvements in the dimensions of active aging (physical activity, nutrition, and cognitive function) and quality of life in healthy older adults. Specifically, greater effects were observed immediately after the intervention, but some of these decreased at follow-up.

Several studies have focused on identifying factors that contribute to successful lifestyle change. Planning changes in complex behaviors such as diet and physical activity involves a complex and elaborate process of specific decision-making.Citation40 Studies have also shown that goals are easier to reach if they are specific and not too broad or general, and there should be sufficient material resources to develop them. In this study, at the end of the intervention, the goals proposed in the physical activity dimension were self–imposed, meaning that they were probably not well planned, which caused that the participants did not followed their own goals. This should be considered in future interventions to achieve better results related to physical activity. Perhaps these types of activity should be performed with other people and not alone. Generally speaking, it has been found from different theoretical models that changes in health behaviors that build up a lifestyle is a process, not an event, and most people relapse at the some point in time.Citation41

Regarding cognitive functioning, it is important to note that participants in our program generally had a low level of education (less than primary school), so did not pursue intellectual activities like reading or other cognitively stimulating activities throughout life. The change that the program “I am Active” proposes requires a greater effort for such people and the results might not be seen immediately.

The study program had a larger effect on self-efficacy indicators, which could be explained on the basis of theory. People’s beliefs about their efficacy can be instilled and strengthened in four ways, ie, mastery experience, vicarious experience, social persuasion, and judgment of their own psychological state.Citation42,Citation43

Through the intervention, the participants experienced personally and directly that they were capable to attain physical activity, good nutritional habits and improved memory, this mastery experience constitutes the most powerful source of self-efficacy. Moreover, the group modality used in the program allowed the participants to observe their classmates making an effort and being successful in their activities (vicarious experience), and this is considered to be the second most active way of developing self-efficacy. Further, the participants in this program were verbally persuaded to aim for active aging (social persuasion); this is the third way of developing self-efficacy but requires a greater effort to develop it. Finally, promotion of positive mood during the program (judgments of their psychological states) probably influenced the participants’ judgment about their personal efficacy, and this represents the fourth way of strengthening self-efficacy beliefs.

The program design considered all four sources of self-efficacy when attempting to influence the development of these beliefs in the experimental group. Meanwhile, talking about the control group, it cannot be discarded that they were mainly exposed to the social persuasion or the promotion of positive moods related to their capabilities as part of senior center activities. However, it is difficult to communicate high efficacy beliefs exclusively through these channels, because unrealistic encouragement is rapidly offset by self-doubt and disappointing results of one’s efforts when is not followed by a performance accomplishment (a mastery experience), which the participants in the control group did not have access to.Citation43

Perceived self-efficacy plays a critical role in human functioning. Beliefs concerning efficacy influence people’s thoughts, the course of action they choose to pursue, their challenges and goals, their commitment to the action and the amount of effort invested, the results they hope to achieve for their efforts, the magnitude of their perseverance while facing barriers, resistance to adversity, the level of stress and depression they experience when facing a demanding environment, and the achievements reached.Citation42

In this sense, the finding that older adults who participated in the “I am Active” program considered themselves to be more capable of physical activity, to develop good nutrition habits, and improve their memory, is an important predictor of their ability to do so, due to the sense of self-efficacy it can even be considered precedent of behavior.Citation44 Although self-efficacy for improving memory performance declined during follow-up.

With regard to cognitive functioning, the program had a large impact on processing speed that remained during the follow-up measure, also considered as a mechanism that explains cognitive decline in aging,Citation45 and slowing of processing speed is generally acknowledged as a hallmark of aging.Citation46 Nevertheless, cognitive decline is not an inevitable outcome of aging, and efforts to improve processing speed through targeted interventions should be implemented due to its relationship not only with cognitive impairmentCitation46 but also with physical functioning.Citation47

An important effect of the program that was still present at follow-up was an improvement in overall quality of life. This component involves greater satisfaction in the areas considered important to people’s lives. Quality of life is considered a key goal in both individual and social welfare, especially in the elderly.Citation48 The “I am Active” program assists older adults to achieve this goal.

“I am Active” had a positive impact on health, psychological components, and cognitive performance; these have been found to be the top three factors explaining active aging, as well as social relationships, biobehavioral components, and personality, and could be used to guide specific community-based and individually-based interventions.Citation22

Active aging is still an issue in development, not only conceptually but also related to the assessment tools and designing interventions. In this sense, it is important to develop valid and reliable scales for assessing active aging in older adults. These tools are needed not only in the community but also in clinical practice settings.Citation49

The limitations of this study include use of self-reported measures, with their well-known bias in terms of social desirability and acquiescence. However, this does not mean that self-reported data are invalid, just that these potential limitations should be taken into account when analyzing and interpreting the data.Citation50 Sample size might be also a limitation. The generalization of our results may be limited by the inclusion criteria and the characteristics of the participants; specifically, our participants were literate and attending senior centers, and comprised mainly women.

Active aging at a political level must involve other strategies focused on individuals in order to successfully overcome the challenges faced by our aging population. In this sense, “I am Active” is the first approach to propose a concrete individual strategy to promote active aging according to the WHO conceptual framework, and should be seen as an opportunity to reinforce the program its self, addressing its weaknesses and strengths in future implementations.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population DivisionWorld Population Ageing 2013New York, NY, USAUnited Nations2013
  • World Health PopulationActive AgeingGeneva, SwitzerlandWorld Health Organization2002
  • WalkerAA strategy of active ageingInt Soc Secur Rev200255121139
  • Active Ageing Australia [home page on the Internet]Adelaide, AustraliaGovernment of South Australia [updated 2014 March]. Available from: http://www.activeageingaustralia.com.auAccessed October 25, 2014
  • WilcoxSDowdaMLevitonLCActive for Life. Final results from translation of two physical activity programsAm J Prev Med20083534035118779028
  • Building Healthy Communities for Active Aging National Recognition Program [home page on the Internet]Durham, NC, USAUnited States Environmental Protection Agency [updated 2013 November]. Available from: http://www.epa.gov/research/aging/bhc/index.htmAccessed October 20, 2014
  • OryMLilesCLawerKBuilding healthy communities for active aging: a national recognition programGenerations2009338284
  • International Council on Active Aging. [home page on the Internet]Vancouver, BC, USAInternational Council on Active Aging2014 Available from: http://www.icaa.cc/index.aspAccessed October 22, 2014
  • European Year for Active Ageing and Solidarity between Generations 2012. [home page on the Internet]Brussels, BelgiumEuropean Union2012 Available from: http://www.age-platform.eu/age-policy-work/solidarity-between-generations/lastest-news/1231-2012-european-year-on-active-ageing-and-intergenerational-solidarityAccessed October 25, 2014
  • Fernández BallesterosRRobineMAWalkerAKalacheAActive aging: a global goalCurr Gerontol Geriatr Res2013201329801223476642
  • Fernández-BallesterosRMendoza-RuvalcabaNMToward a definition of successful agingKruseALife in Old AgeHeidelberg, GermanyAkademische Verlagsgesellschaft AKA2010 German
  • DeepCAJasteDVDefinitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studiesAm J Geriatr Psychiatry20061462016407577
  • RoweJWKahnRLHuman aging: usual and successfulScience19872371431493299702
  • RoweJWKahnRLSuccessful AgingNew York, NY, USAPantheon1998
  • CapraraMGMolinaMASchettiniRActive aging promotion: results from the Vital Aging ProgramCurr Gerontol Geriatr Res2013201381781323476644
  • EstebsariFTaghdisiMHForoushaniARArdebiliEAShojaeizadehDAn educational program based on the successful aging approach on health-promotion behaviors in the elderly: a clinical trial studyIran Red Crescent Med J20141616314
  • HookerSPSeaveyWWeidmerCEThe California active aging community grant program: translating science into practice to promote physical activity in older adultsAnn Behav Med20052915516515946109
  • FoyCGVitolinsMZCaseLIncorporating prosocial behavior to promote physical activity in older adults: rationale design of the Program for Active Aging and Community Engagement (PACE)Contemp Clin Trials20133628429723876672
  • LatorreJMSerranoJPRicarteJBoneteBRosBSitgesELife review based on remembering specific positive events in active agingJ Aging Health20152714015725005172
  • BoyesMOutdoor adventure and successful ageingAgeing Soc201333644665
  • SikoraSAThe University of Arizona College of Medicine optimal aging program: stepping in the shadows of successful agingGerontol Geriatr Educ200627596717023384
  • PaúlCRibeiroOTeixeiraLActive ageing: an empirical approach to the WHO modelCurr Gerontol Geriatr Res2012201238297223193396
  • NelsonMERejeskyWJBlairSNPhysical activity and public health in older adults: Recomendation from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart AssociationMed Aci Sports Exerc20073914351445
  • AhmedTHaboubiNAssessment and management of nutrition in older people and its importance to healthClin Interv Aging2010520721620711440
  • Fernández-BallesterosRBotellaJZamarrónMDCognitive plasticity in normal and pathological agingClin Interv Aging20127152522291469
  • KimEYKimKWA theoretical framework for cognitive and non-cognitive interventions for older adults: stimulation versus compensationAging Ment Health20141830431524354740
  • RitchieKArteroSTouchtonJClassification criteria for mild-cognitive impairment: a population-based validation studyNeurology200156374211148233
  • FlickerCFerrisSHReisbergBMild cognitive impairment in the elderly: predictors of dementiaNeurology199141100610092067629
  • LeichtHKönigHHStuhldreherNPredictors of costs in dementia in a longitudinal perspectivePLoS One20138110
  • YesavageJABrinkTLRoseTDevelopment and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary reportJ Psychiatr Res19831737497183759
  • FolsteinMFolsteinSMcHughP“Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinicianJ Psychiatr Res1975121891981202204
  • Arias-MerinoEDOrtizGGMendoza-RuvalcabaNMEl Mini-mental State Examination el efecto de la edad, género y nivel educativo en una población MexicanaGutiérrez-RobledoLMTópicos de Actualización en neurobiología envejecimiento y neurodegeneraciónGuadalajara, MéxicoUniversidad de Guadalajara2011 Spanish
  • BanduraAGuide for constructing self-efficacy scalesUrdanTPajaresFSelf-Efficacy Beliefs of AdolescentsNew York, NY, USAInformation Age Publishing2006
  • VellasBGuigozYGarryPThe Mini-Nutritional Assessment (MNA) and its use in grading the nutritional state of elderly patientsNutrition1999151161229990575
  • Global Database on Body Mass IndexBMI Classification [home page on the Internet]Geneva, SwitzerlandWorld Health Organization [updated 2014 December]. Available from: http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.htmlAccessed October 30, 2014
  • WechslerDWechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleCuauhtémoc, MéxicoManual Moderno1997
  • FerransCEPowersMPsychometric assessment of the Quality of Life IndexRes Nurs Health19921529381579648
  • SullivanGMFeinnRUsing effect size – or why the P-value is not enoughJ Grad Med Educ2012427928223997866
  • CarsonCThe effective use of effect size indices in institutional research Available from: http://rlrw.bnu.edu.cn/NewsImage/2012410114358.pdfAccessed October 25, 2014
  • SniehottaFFSchwarzerRScholzUSchüzBAction planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessmentEur J Soc Psychol200535565576
  • ArmitageCJConnerMSocial cognition models and health behavior: a structured reviewPsychol Health200015173189
  • BanduraASelf-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral changePsychol Rev199784191215847061
  • BanduraAExercise of personal and collective efficacy in changing societiesBanduraASelf-Efficacy in Changing Societies2nd edCambridge, UKCambridge University Press1999
  • BanduraASelf-efficacy mechanism in human agencyAm Psychol198237122147
  • ParkDCThe basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive functionParkDCognitive Aging: A PrimerNew York, NY, USAPsychology Press2012
  • SalthouseTAThe processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognitionPsychol Rev19961034034288759042
  • Desjaardins-CrépeauLBerrymanNVuTTPhysical functioning is associated with processing speed and executive functions in community-dwelling older adultsJ Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci20146983784424829304
  • Fernández-BallesterosRArias-MerinoEDSantacreuMMendoza-RuvalcabaNMQuality of life in Mexico and in SpainPowellJLThe Global Dynamics of AgingUKNova Science Publishers2011
  • ThanakwangKIsaramalaiSHatthakitUDevelopment and psychometric testing of the active aging scale for Thai adultsClin Interv Aging201491211122125092971
  • BarkerCPistrangNElliottRSelf-report methodsBarkerCResearch Methods in Clinical Psychology2nd edWileyOnline Library2002