69
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Patients with persistent pain after breast cancer surgery show both delayed and enhanced cortical stimulus processing

, , , , &
Pages 139-150 | Published online: 13 Jun 2012

Abstract

Background

Women who undergo breast cancer surgery have a high risk of developing persistent pain. We investigated brain processing of painful stimuli using electroencephalograms (EEG) to identify event-related potentials (ERPs) in patients with persistent pain after breast cancer treatment.

Methods

Nineteen patients (eight women with persistent pain, eleven without persistent pain), who were surgically treated more than 1 year previously for breast cancer (mastectomy, lumpectomy, and axillary lymph node dissection) and/or had chemoradiotherapy, were recruited and compared with eleven healthy female volunteers. A block of 20 painful electrical stimuli was applied to the calf, somatopically remote from the initially injured or painful area. Simultaneously an EEG was recorded, and a visual analog scale (VAS) pain rating obtained.

Results

In comparison with healthy volunteers, breast cancer treatment without persistent pain is associated with accelerated stimulus processing (reduced P260 latency) and shows a tendency to be less intense (lower P260 amplitude). In comparison to patients without persistent pain, persistent pain after breast cancer treatment is associated with stimulus processing that is both delayed (ie, increased latency of the ERP positivity between 250–310 ms [P260]), and enhanced (ie, enhanced P260 amplitude).

Conclusion

These results show that treatment and persistent pain have opposite effects on cortical responsiveness.

Introduction

In recent years interest has grown in the alterations in brain processing present in patients with persistent pain. Brain imaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) have been used to investigate brain function by measuring the evoked response to applied somatosensory stimuli.Citation1,Citation2 The results regarding altered pain processing by the brain in the context of persistent pain are highly incongruent, perhaps due to large variability between the patients regarding pain history, pain etiology, pain distribution, and psychological characteristics.Citation1,Citation2

Use of a postoperative model may help overcome some of these problems, because it permits study of a homogenous patient population regarding pain etiology, pain distribution, and treatment. Furthermore, this model makes it possible to differentiate between the effect of treatment and the effect of pain because a comparative patient group (same treatment, but no pain) can be included for comparison.

It has been shown that women who undergo surgery for breast cancer have a high risk of developing persistent postsurgical pain.Citation3Citation6 This pain persistence is difficult to treat and is accompanied by a significantly diminished quality of life.Citation5,Citation7

The often used generic term “postmastectomy pain syndrome” in cases of persistent pain after breast cancer treatment might suggest a homogeneous disease category. But this is debatable.Citation8 In fact, different types of pain have been observed after breast cancer treatment, like phantom breast pain,Citation3,Citation9 scar pain,Citation10 neuropathic pain,Citation6 complex regional pain syndrome,Citation11 pain arising from the axillary web syndrome,Citation12 and the more recently prospectively investigated myofascial pain syndrome, which is typically observed during the first year after breast surgery including axillary lymph node dissection (ALND).Citation13

The etiology of persistent pain after breast cancer treatment is probably multifactorial.Citation8 This is because breast cancer treatment includes different types of surgical interventions (eg, mastectomy, lumpectomy, sentinel lymph node biopsy, and ALND), and adjuvant therapies like chemotherapy, radiation and endocrine therapies. All these interventions may contribute to the development of persistent pain, and could have their own characteristics. However, nerve damage and radiotherapy appear to be significant risk factors.Citation8

A frequently observed phenomenon in persistent postsurgical pain conditions, and also in patients after breast cancer surgery, is a change in the sensitivity of tactile and pain processing. This change consists of a combination of sensory loss, particularly in the skin innervated by possibly damaged nerves, and hypersensitivity.Citation4,Citation6,Citation8,Citation14,Citation15

To our knowledge, studies investigating the evoked brain response using electroencephalography (EEG) in the context of persistent postsurgical pain are scarce.Citation2,Citation16 In contrast to fMRI and PET, EEG directly measures neuronal activity; furthermore, it makes it possible to study the sequential activation of different brain structures in time. The aim of this study is to investigate brain processing of painful stimuli using EEG (or more specifically, event-related potentials [ERPs]) in patients with persistent pain after breast cancer surgery. To investigate possible changes in ERPs as a result of the presence of pain, these results (ie, from patients with pain) are compared with those in women without persistent pain after breast cancer surgery. In addition, we aim to investigate possible ERP changes as a result of breast cancer treatment by comparing the results of the patients without pain with healthy female volunteers. Our main hypothesis is that persistent pain is associated with an enhanced brain response to painful stimuli.

Materials and methods

Participants

Nineteen patients (eight women with pain and eleven without pain) who had been treated for breast cancer were recruited from a clinical database of the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre. Approval for the study was obtained from the Medical and Ethical Review Board Committee Region Arnhem-Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (NL 30189.091.09). All subjects signed an informed consent form. At the moment of inclusion none had evidence of metastases or disease recurrences. All patients (with and without pain) had been operated more than 1 year ago at the time of participating. Patients had all undergone a mastectomy or lumpectomy and ALND but no breast reconstruction. The rationale for investigating this population of patients is the high incidence of persistent pain after this type of surgery (mastectomy or lumpectomy + ALND).Citation3,Citation4 Only patients who had unilateral breast cancer were included. Persistent pain was defined as pain persisting continuously or intermittently for more than 3 months after surgery.Citation17

Besides patients, eleven healthy female volunteers were also recruited from the Nijmegen area. Patients as well as healthy volunteers were excluded from the study if they:

  1. had undergone breast reconstruction,

  2. had a psychiatric or neurological condition (for patients, neurological signs as a result of the treatment were excepted),

  3. used pain medication or other medication that potentially affects brain processing, such as antidepressants, antipsychotics, antiepileptics, and benzodiazepines (hormone therapy as adjuvant therapy used by the patients excepted),

  4. suffered from any pre-existing pain or pain syndrome.

Subjects were instructed not to consume caffeine-containing beverages for twelve hours before the recording session. This was to avoid a caffeine-induced theta decrease in EEG.Citation18

Variables measured

Demographic and clinical characteristics

The composition of the two breast cancer surgery groups (with and without pain) was based on a standardized question (obtained via an interview by telephone) whether the patient experienced ongoing pain (yes or no) as a result of the breast cancer treatment. For confirmation, the same question was asked again on the day of measurement, together with an additional standardized question (only if the patient experienced pain) regarding pain intensity as a measure of past experienced pain load (“What is the averaged intensity of the breast treatment-related pain during the last three months on a numeric 0–10 rating scale (NRS)?”).

Other demographic and clinical characteristics obtained were age, menopausal status, surgical treatment, and chemotherapy, radiation, and/or hormone therapy.

Patients who undergo ALND during breast cancer treatment are at risk for developing lymphedema.Citation19 Hypothetically, this could contribute to the persistence of pain. Therefore we measured limb volume differences (unaffected compared to affected limb) as an indirect reflection of the possible presence of lymphedema. To do so, we measured the limb volume of both sides (arms) via water displacement.Citation20 Subjects were instructed to lower their arm slowly into a fully filled volume meter and asked to stop when the top of the volume meter came in contact with the axilla.Citation20 The amount of spilt water was collected in a measuring cup (mL). The volume of the opposite (control) arm was also measured. The difference in volume of spilt water between the two sides (affected and control) was calculated. This test was also performed with the healthy volunteers to test if there are normally differences in volume between the two sides.

Data about the type of pain and pain-related sensory signs in the patients with pain were collected using the Douleur Neuropathique 4 (DN4) questionnaire.Citation21,Citation22 This questionnaire includes pain descriptors as well as three clinical tests reflecting altered somatosensory processing. The tests were performed by a physical therapist. For measuring hypoesthesia to touch, a brush (SENSElab, brush 05; Somedic, Horby, Sweden) was applied on different skin sites in the location of the pain. For measuring hypoesthesia to pinprick, a Semmes–Weinstein monofilament (nr. 5.07, 10.0 g) was applied to different skin areas in the location of the pain. For measuring brush evoked or increased pain within the location of pain, the same brush as for hypoesthesia was used. The effects of stimulation of the first two clinical tests (hypoesthesia to touch and pinprick) were quantified by comparing the skin sites in the location of pain to a control site on the contralateral body site.

It is important to mention that, in this study, the DN4 questionnaire is not used as a screening or diagnostic instrument for neuropathic pain, because at present it is not validated for this purpose in this population of surgical patients. Thus we used the DN4 exclusively to collect data regarding the clinical qualitative characteristics of the pain syndrome.

Patients of both groups (with and without persistent pain) were asked if they had experienced tactile hypoesthesia or numbness since their treatment. If they did, they were asked to draw on a map the size and anatomical area of hypoesthesia.

Electrophysiological measures

A multichannel EEG (BrainVision; Brain Products GmbH, Waldkirch, Germany) was recorded during the experiment (band-pass 0.1–100 Hz, sample frequency 2000 Hz) with 64 active electrodes mounted in an elastic electrode cap. The electrodes were arranged according to the international 10–20 system and electrode CPz was used as common reference. Eye movements were detected by horizontal and vertical electrooculogram (EOG) recordings. Horizontal EOG was measured from the outer canthus of the left eye, and vertical EOG supra orbital to the left eye. Impedance was kept under 20 kΩ for all leads.

Painful stimulation

Subjects received painful stimulation on the calf, between the medial and lateral head of the gastrocnemius, using a concentric electrode (CE).Citation23 Because of its concentric design and small anode–cathode distance, this stimulating electrode produces a high current density at relatively low current intensities. In this way, depolarization is more limited to the superficial layer of the dermis (where nociceptive [Aδ] fibers are present) with less recruitment of deeper lying nonnociceptive fibers. Stimulation with this electrode produces a pinprick-like painful sensation. The stimulated site was balanced across patients with regard to the affected side. In healthy subjects, balancing was according to lateral dominance.

The stimulation protocol consisted of 20 double pulses (monopolar square wave; duration 0.5 ms and double-pulse interval 5 ms) with a random interpair interval ranging from 7 to 10 seconds. The double pulses were delivered through the CE using a constant current stimulator (Twister®; Dr Langer Medical GmbH, Waldkirch, Germany) and with an intensity of 150% of the individual pain threshold. This individual pain threshold was determined by an ascending sequence of increasing current intensities starting from 0 mA and in steps of 0.5 mA. This procedure stopped when the pain threshold was achieved, as verbally reported by the subjects. This threshold determination protocol was performed twice and the mean was used in the experiment to set intensity of stimulation.

During stimulation, subjects were comfortably seated in a chair and were instructed to passively perceive the stimuli with eyes closed (as this condition is less prone to artifacts), without making any movements. A computer display was placed in front of the subject (0.5 m) together with a computer mouse. The display was used to show the visual analog scale (VAS) (see Behavioral measure), preceded by a tone (65 dB). Participants were instructed to open their eyes after the tone and use the mouse to mark the VAS, after which they closed their eyes again.

Behavioral measure

In order to quantify the amount of pain as a result of the painful stimulation, subjects were asked to rate, at random times within a train of five double pulses, the amount of pain caused by the last received stimulus on a VAS. The VAS ranged from 0 cm = “no pain” to 10 cm = “unbearable pain” and was rated by the subject by moving the mouse pointer (vertical line) on a horizontal bar.

Procedure

At the beginning of the experiment, demographic and clinical characteristics were collected. Next, the individual pinprick-like pain thresholds for the double pulse stimulation were determined. Finally, subjects received the experimental painful stimulation with simultaneous recording of the EEG.

Signal analysis

Event-related potentials

The EEG was analyzed offline using Brain Vision Analyzer software (v. 2.0; Brain Products GmbH, Gliching, Germany) and MatLab (2011a; MathWorks, Natick, MA). As a first step, the continuous EEG was referenced to a common average (ie, all electrodes). Next, the EEG signal (2500 Hz) was high-pass filtered at 1 Hz and low-pass filtered at 30 Hz. Based on the onset of the stimulus, the EEG was segmented into epochs from −100 ms prestimulus to 1000 ms poststimulus with a total period of 1100 ms. Bad segments containing ocular artifacts were corrected using the Gratton–Coles method.Citation24 Segments were also inspected for other artifacts like muscle or jaw and line noise activity, and were removed if necessary. As a last step, baseline correction (−100–0 ms) was applied to all segments.

For each subject separately, all segments were averaged to obtain an averaged subject-specific event-related potential waveform. ERP components were defined in terms of their latency and topographic distribution. Subsequently the grand average global field power (GFP) of all subjects was calculated.Citation25,Citation26 Next, we calculated the topographic voltage distribution corresponding to the ERP latencies identified in the GFP plot. Then we identified the electrode in the topographic plot which showed the maximal activity, and used this electrode for subsequent analysis. To insure accurate identification of point of maximal activity we also inspected the grand average ERPs (of all electrodes) for all subjects.

Individual ERP latencies were determined in the individual GFP plot corresponding to the windows of the grand average GFP latencies.Citation26 The mean amplitude of each ERP component was calculated at the individual GFP latency ± 5 ms at the electrode of maximal activity.Citation26 The rationale for using the mean activity instead of the more commonly used maximal peak value (baseline-to-peak) is that, the fewer trials included in the subject-specific average, the more residual noise is superimposed on the maximal peak, and thus the more the maximal peak of the subject-specific average will be determined by residual noise rather than by the peak of interest. Therefore, we calculated the mean amplitude instead of the maximal peak amplitude, because the former value is more stable and representative of evoked activity.Citation27

Statistical analysis

The software package GraphPad Prism 5 (GraphPad, San Diego, CA) was used for statistical analysis. Because of the small sample sizes and non-Gaussian distributions, nonparametric test statistics were used for between-group comparisons. A Kruskall–Wallis test statistic (H) was used for ratio variables. In the present study only two pairs of posthoc comparisons were tested; healthy volunteers compared to patients without pain (effect of treatment) and patients without pain compared to patients with pain (effect of pain). The Dunn’s multiple comparisons test, which corrects for the number of statistical tests, was used as a post-hoc test. The effect size r was calculated as the Z-score divided by the square root of the total number of observations. Categorical variables were tested using the chi-squared (χ2) test statistic (P < 0.05).

Results

Clinical and demographical characteristics

Clinical and demographical characteristics are shown in and .

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patients (A) with pain, (B) without pain and (C) healthy volunteers

Table 2 Results of the Douleur Neuropathique 4 questionnaire

No statistically significant differences were observed between the three groups with respect to age and limb volume differences. Median age and limb volume differences scores (and interquartile ranges) are shown in .

A significant association (χ2 (2) = 7.972, p = .019) was observed between condition (healthy volunteers and patients) and menopausal status (pre- and post-). As can be seen in , all patients (with and without pain) are postmenopausal, whereas 44% of healthy volunteers are premenopausal.

No significant associations were observed between the two patient groups (with and without pain) regarding the type of surgical intervention (mastectomy + ALND or lumpectomy + ALND) and incidences of adjuvant therapies (chemotherapy, radiation, or hormone therapy). See also for incidences. The results obtained from the DN4 questionnaire are shown in . shows the topography of hypoesthesia (numbness) drawn by the patients (with and without pain).

Figure 1 Area of tactile hypoesthesia (numbness).

Notes: This figure shows the topographical map of areas of tactile hypoesthesia (numbness) drawn by the patients without pain and with pain. The scale of percentages shown in the legend represents the number of patients (converted to percentages) who marked that area as hypoesthetic.
Figure 1 Area of tactile hypoesthesia (numbness).

Stimulation intensity

No statistically significant differences were observed between the three groups regarding the applied stimulation intensities for noxious stimulation for ERPs. Median (and interquartile ranges) stimulation intensities were: healthy volunteers 3.0 (2.7–4.2) mA, patients without pain 3.3 (3.0–3.7) mA, patients with pain 3.9 (2.7–4.7) mA.

Behavioral tests

No statistically significant differences were observed between the three groups regarding the VAS scores obtained during the noxious stimulation. Median VAS scores (and interquartile ranges) were: healthy volunteers 4.2 (2.5–4.7) cm, patients without pain 3.0 (2.4–5.9) cm, patients with pain 2.5 (1.6–4.2) cm.

Event-related potentials

Based on the grand average GFP and corresponding topographic representations of all subjects (N = 30) shown in , we defined four distinctive ERP components:

Figure 2 Grand average global field power (GFP) and corresponding topographic representations. (A) Grand average GFP (N = 30). The dotted lines indicate peak latency of the different event-related potential (ERP) components. Four different components can be identified: a negative voltage between 110–180 ms, maximal at FCz, labeled as N150, a positive voltage between 190–230 ms, maximal at Cz, labeled as P200, a positive voltage between 250–310 ms, maximal at FCz, labeled as P260, and a positive voltage between 310–380 ms, maximal at Cz and labeled as P350. (B) Topographic representations of the ERP components at the ERP latencies (N = 30). To best illustrate the maximal activity in each representation, we adjusted the scale to its maximal absolute values (for increases and decreases in voltages). As a result the scale differs between the different representations and is therefore left out.

Figure 2 Grand average global field power (GFP) and corresponding topographic representations. (A) Grand average GFP (N = 30). The dotted lines indicate peak latency of the different event-related potential (ERP) components. Four different components can be identified: a negative voltage between 110–180 ms, maximal at FCz, labeled as N150, a positive voltage between 190–230 ms, maximal at Cz, labeled as P200, a positive voltage between 250–310 ms, maximal at FCz, labeled as P260, and a positive voltage between 310–380 ms, maximal at Cz and labeled as P350. (B) Topographic representations of the ERP components at the ERP latencies (N = 30). To best illustrate the maximal activity in each representation, we adjusted the scale to its maximal absolute values (for increases and decreases in voltages). As a result the scale differs between the different representations and is therefore left out.

  1. A negative voltage between 110–180 ms, maximal at electrode FCz, which we label as N150,

  2. A positive voltage between 190–230 ms, maximal at Cz, which we label as P200,

  3. A positive voltage between 250–310 ms, maximal at FCz, which we label as P260,

  4. A positive voltage between 310–380 ms, maximal at Cz, which we label as P350.

shows the topographic representations of the ERP components for each group at the ERP latencies.

Figure 3 Group-specific topographic representations. Shown are the topographic representations of the different event-related potential (ERP) components at the different ERP latencies ().

Notes: To best illustrate the maximal activity in each representation, we adjusted the scale to its maximal absolute values (for increases and decreases in voltages). As a result the scale differs between the different representations and is therefore left out.
Figure 3 Group-specific topographic representations. Shown are the topographic representations of the different event-related potential (ERP) components at the different ERP latencies (Figure 2).

ERP amplitude

There were no statistically significant differences regarding N150, P200, and P350 between groups. Median and interquartile ranges are shown in . A statistical difference was observed for the P260 between the three groups (H (2) = 6.490, P = 0.039). Dunn’s post-hoc tests revealed a statistically significant difference between patients with pain compared to patients without pain (P < 0.05; effect size r = −0.49). Grand average ERPs of P260 are shown in .

Figure 4 Event-related potential (ERP) waveforms. Grand average ERPs observed from FCz showing the P260 differences (A) effect of treatment, (B) effect of pain.

Notes: Upward deflection is positive charge and downward is negative charge. Representations of ERPs are with respect to common reference.
Figure 4 Event-related potential (ERP) waveforms. Grand average ERPs observed from FCz showing the P260 differences (A) effect of treatment, (B) effect of pain.

Table 3 Event-related potential (ERP) amplitude and latencies

ERP latency

A statistically significant difference was observed between the three groups (H (2) = 9.367, P = 0.009) regarding P260 latency. Dunn’s post-hoc tests revealed a statistically significant difference between patients without pain and healthy volunteers (P < 0.05; effect size r = 0.58) but also between patients with pain compared to patients without pain (P < 0.05; effect size r = −0.56). Median and interquartile ranges are shown in .

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate cortical processing by means of EEG and with this kind of stimuli in this group of patients. In comparison to patients without persistent pain, persistent pain after breast cancer treatment is associated with delayed and enhanced stimulus processing as reflected in an increased latency and enhanced amplitude of the ERP positivity between 250–310 ms (P260). Moreover, in comparison to healthy volunteers, breast cancer treatment is associated with acceleration of (reduced P260 latency) and a tendency towards less intense (smaller P260 amplitude) stimulus processing. These results suggest that the two conditions, ie, treatment and pain persistence, have opposite effects regarding cortical responsiveness.

Breast cancer treatment and cortical processing

The comparison between the patients without pain and the healthy volunteers reveals the effect of treatment on cortical processing. This comparison revealed an acceleration of stimulus processing (reduced P260 latency) in the patients without pain compared to the healthy volunteers. Moreover, there is a smaller late ERP amplitude (P260) in the patients without pain compared with the healthy volunteers, although this is not statistically significant according to the Dunn’s post hoc test. This is probably due to the small sample sizes and the fact the P value has to be corrected for multiple comparisons. Indeed, the effect size is r = −0.45.

Kreukels et alCitation29 also observed a lower ERP amplitude in disease- free breast cancer survivors who were treated for breast cancer (including surgery and radiotherapy). All patients underwent surgery and radiotherapy. In this study, the authors investigated the effect of different chemotherapy regimens on ERP activity in response to auditory stimuli (by using an oddball paradigm). Overall, they observed significantly reduced late ERP (ie, P3) amplitude between patients that received chemotherapy as compared to matched control patients who had not received chemotherapy. Moreover, a shorter P3 latency was observed after chemotherapy. The authors did not find any changes in midlatency N1 ERP amplitude or latency between the two groups (with and without chemotherapy), a finding in agreement with the present study.

Are there alternative factors that can explain the reduced brain activity? Regarding hormone therapy, Kreukels et alCitation29 performed an additional subanalysis on their data in which they compared the ERP P3 amplitude between current, past, and never users of tamoxifen. They found no significant difference in P3 amplitude between the three groups, suggesting that tamoxifen (and perhaps also other hormone therapy regimens) cannot explain the observed ERP reduction.

An as yet undefined pathophysiological process subsequent to amputation, eg, deafferentation, might also change EEG activity.Citation30 This argument is based on the study by Karl et al.Citation30 Although not statistically significantly different, a lower P3 amplitude was observed in the amputees without pain compared to the healthy controls.

When we look at the clinical and demographic characteristics (), the proportion of premenopausal status among the healthy women compared to the patients without pain is different. Theoretically, this could be a further factor explaining the differences in P260 amplitude between the two groups.

Persistent pain and cortical processing

The comparison between the patients with and without pain reveals the effect of the presence of persistent postsurgical pain on cortical processing. Based on the results mentioned in the previous section, we suggest that breast cancer treatment (ie, chemotherapy) affects late ERP activity, ie, lower ERP amplitude and shorter latency. The larger ERP amplitude (and increased latency) seen in the patients with pain compared to the patients without pain is likely the result of the presence of pain additional to the effect of breast cancer treatment. Therefore, we conclude that persistent pain after breast cancer treatment is associated with delayed (increased P260 latency) and enhanced (larger P260 amplitude) stimulus processing.

Interestingly, Karl et al,Citation30 using an oddball paradigm, compared visual P3 amplitude between upper limb amputees with and without persistent pain and healthy volunteers. Patients with pain showed significantly higher P3 amplitudes than patients without pain, but neither group were statistically different from the healthy volunteers. The latter result could be due to the small sample sizes (patients with pain, N = 5; patients without pain, N = 5; and healthy volunteers, N = 10). However, the ERP findings observed in the study of Karl et al appear to involve later ERP activity (between 300–500 ms) than in our study (between 250–310 ms). Possible explanations for the different ERP activities affected in the two studies are the type of stimulus and paradigm used.

Methodological considerations

Defining (late) ERP components

The positivity around 260 ms (ie, P260) shares the same time course and topographic distribution as the previously described SP5 component (233–277 ms) evoked after painful electrical stimulation.Citation31 This ERP component seems to overlap with the greater later positivity SP6 or pain-related P2.

The positivity around 350 ms, labeled as P350, might be the pain-related P2 evoked after painful electrical stimulation.Citation31,Citation32 By comparing laser stimulation with electrical sural nerve stimulation, Dowman showed that this P2, evoked after painful stimulation, has similar properties to the commonly described P2, associated with selective Aδ fiber activation, evoked after painful laser stimulation.Citation33Citation36

However, Mouraux et alCitation35 recently compared electrical intraepidermal, electrical nonnociceptive transcutaneous and laser stimulation for their selectivity in generating Aδ fiber-associated evoked brain responses. They showed that only laser and low intensity electrical intraepidermal stimulation are able to evoke Aδ-associated evoked brain responses. Additionally, they showed that intraepidermal stimulation loses its selectivity with increasing stimulus intensity, something that occurred above intensities of 2.5 mA.Citation35 In the present study we used transcutaneous electrical stimulation with stimulation intensities around 3.0 mA, which indicates against the possibility that we selectively evoked Aδ-associated brain responses.

Alternatively, the P350 could be a P3a-like component.Citation31,Citation37,Citation38 This hypothesis can be supported by the facts that:

  1. A “single stimulus” paradigm as used in the present study, in which only target but no standard stimuli are delivered with long, variable and random interstimulus intervals, is able to evoke a P3a-like component,Citation39,Citation40 also after painful electrical stimulation,Citation31 and

  2. this positivity shares the same generators in the brain as the classic P3a, as is demonstrated via intracranially-recorded cortical responses evoked after painful electrical stimulation. These generators include the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortices, temporal-parietal junction and posterior hippocampus.Citation37

Area of stimulation

In the present study, the painful stimuli were applied to a body part somatopically remote from the initially injured or painful area. We choose to do this because we wished to investigate cortical changes in pain processing (which one would expect to be generalized). For this, we need to stimulate in an area remote from the spinal segment undergoing nociceptive input due to breast cancer treatment. Our study therefore reflects only generalized but not localized effects of surgery or radiation therapy.

Sample size

An important methodological limitation of this study is the small sample size. This was the result of our opting for more strict inclusion and exclusion criteria (to avoid confounding factors), but has the advantage that the resulting patient groups are very homogenous. Nevertheless, the ERP effects observed in the present study should be confirmed in a new future study with larger sample sizes.

Conclusions

This observational study shows that the two conditions, ie, treatment and persistent pain, have opposite effects regarding cortical responsiveness. Breast cancer treatment is associated with an acceleration of and a tendency to less intense stimulus processing. Persistent pain after breast cancer treatment is associated with delayed and enhanced stimulus processing. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate cortical processing by means of EEG and with this kind of stimuli in this group of patients.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank drs Magarethe Schlooz and drs Annelies Werner for their efforts regarding the recruitment of the patients and Jos Wittebrood for the technical support. Finally they would like to thank all the patients and healthy subjects. This study was supported by the unrestricted EFIC Grünenthal Grant 2008 (see http://www.e-g-g.info/grt-egg/EFIC_GRUENENTHAL_GRANT/Awards/2008/Winners/78700358.jsp).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • MoissetXBouhassiraDBrain imaging of neuropathic painNeuroimage200737Suppl 1S80S8817512757
  • KupersRKehletHBrain imaging of clinical pain states: a critical review and strategies for future researchLancet Neurol20065121033104417110284
  • SteegersMAWoltersBEversAWStrobbeLWilder-SmithOHThe effect of axillary lymph node dissection on prevalence and intensity of chronic and phantom pain after breast cancer surgeryJ Pain20089981382218585963
  • KehletHJensenTSWoolfCJPersistent postsurgical pain: risk factors and preventionLancet200636795221618162516698416
  • MacdonaldLBruceJScottNWSmithWCSChambersWALongterm follow-up of breast cancer survivors with post-mastectomy pain syndromeBr J Cancer200592222523015655557
  • JungBFAhrendtGMOaklanderALDworkinRHNeuropathic pain following breast cancer surgery: proposed classification and research updatePain20031041–211312855309
  • StevensPEDibbleSLMiaskowskiCPrevalance, characteristics, and impact of postmastectomy pain syndrome: an investigation of women’s experiencesPain199561161687644250
  • AndersenKGKehletHPersistent pain after breast cancer treatment: a critical review of risk factors and strategies for preventionJ Pain201112772574621435953
  • DijkstraPURietmanJSGeerzenJHBPhantom breast sensations and phantom breast pain: a 2-year prospective study and methodological analysis of literatureEur J Pain20071119910816487732
  • MacreaWAChronic pain after surgeryBr J Anaesth2001871889811460816
  • GrahamLEMcGuiganCKerrSComplex regional pain syndrome post mastectomyRheumatol Int200221416516611843174
  • LeideniusMLeppanenEKrogerusLMotion restriction and axillary web syndrome after sentinel node biopsy and axillary clearance in breast cancerAm J Surg2003185212713012559441
  • Torres LacombaMMayoral del MoralOCoperias ZazoJLGerwinRDZapico GoñiAIncidence of myofascial pain syndrome in breast cancer surgery: a prospective studyClin J Pain201026432032520393267
  • MaierCBaronRTölleTRQuantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): somatosensory abnormalities in 1236 patients with different neuropathic pain syndromesPain2010150343945020627413
  • GottrupHAndersenJArendt-NielsenLJensenTSPsychophysical examination in patients with post-mastectomy painPain200087327528410963907
  • ApkarianAVBushnellMCTreedeR-DZubietaJ-KHuman brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and diseaseEur J Pain20059446348415979027
  • Classification of chronic pain. Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Prepared by the International Association for the Study of Pain, subcommittee on taxonomyPain Suppl19863S1S2263461421
  • LandoltH-PReteyJVTonzKCaffeine attenuates waking and sleep electroencephalographic markers of sleep homeostasis in humansNeuropsychopharmacology200429101933193915257305
  • PetrekJAPressmanPISmithRALymphedema: current issues in research and managementCA Cancer J Clin200050529230711075239
  • SanderAPHajerNMHemenwayKMillerACUpper- extremity volume measurements in women with lymphedema: a comparison of measurements obtained via water displacement with geometrically determined volumePhys Ther200282121201121212444879
  • BouhassiraDAttalNAlchaarHComparison of pain syndromes associated with nervous or somatic lesions and development of a new neuropathic pain diagnostic questionnaire (DN4)Pain20051141–2293615733628
  • Van SeventerRVosCMeerdingWLinguistic validation of the DN4 for use in international studiesEur J Pain2010141586319282208
  • KatsaravaZAyzenbergISackFLimmrothVDienerH-CKaubeHA novel method of eliciting pain-related potentials by transcutaneous electrical stimulationHeadache200646101511151717115984
  • GrattonGColesMGHDonchinEA new method for off-line removal of ocular artifactElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19835544684846187540
  • SkrandiesWGlobal field power and topographic similarityBrain Topogr1990311371412094301
  • BoyleYEl-DeredyWMontesEMBentleyDEJonesAKPSelective modulation of nociceptive processing due to noise distractionPain2008138363064018423872
  • PictonTWBentinSBergPGuidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteriaPsychophysiology200037212715210731765
  • ReznikJCicchettiMGDegaspeBFitzgeraldTJAnalysis of axillary coverage during tangential radiation therapy to the breastInt J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys200561116316815629607
  • KreukelsBPCHamburgerHLde RuiterMBERP amplitude and latency in breast cancer survivors treated with adjuvant chemotherapyClin Neurophysiol2008119353354118164658
  • KarlADiersMFlorHP300-amplitudes in upper limb amputees with and without phantom limb pain in a visual oddball paradigmPain20041101–2404815275750
  • DowmanRThe pain-evoked P2 is not a P3a event-related potentialBrain Topogr200417131215669751
  • De TommasoMSantostasiRDevitofrancescoVA comparative study of cortical responses evoked by transcutaneous electrical vs CO2 laser stimulationClin Neurophysiol2011122122482248721641860
  • DowmanRTopographic analysis of painful laser and sural nerve electrical evoked potentialsBrain Topogr200416316917915162914
  • MobascherABrinkmeyerJWarbrickTLaser-evoked potential P2 single-trial amplitudes covary with the fMRI BOLD response in the medial pain system and interconnected subcortical structuresNeuroimage200945391792619166948
  • MourauxAIannettiGDPlaghkiLLow intensity intra-epidermal electrical stimulation can activate Aδ-nociceptors selectivelyPain2010150119920720510515
  • Arendt-NielsenLBjerringPSelective averaging of argon laser induced pre-pain and pain related cortical responsesJ Neurosci Methods19882421171233405008
  • DowmanRDarceyTBarkanHThadaniVRobertsDHuman intracranially-recorded cortical responses evoked by painful electrical stimulation of the sural nerveNeuroimage200734274376317097306
  • PolichJUpdating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3bClin Neurophysiol2007118102128214817573239
  • PolichJHeineMRDP300 topography and modality effects from a single stimulus paradigmPsychophysiology19963367477528961797
  • MertensRPolichJP300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: passive versus active tasks and stimulus modalityElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol199710464884979402891