202
Views
30
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

More than meets the eye: visual attention biases in individuals reporting chronic pain

&
Pages 557-570 | Published online: 19 Sep 2014

Abstract

The present study used eye-tracking technology to assess whether individuals who report chronic pain direct more attention to sensory pain-related words than do pain-free individuals. A total of 113 participants (51 with chronic pain, 62 pain-free) were recruited. Participants completed a dot-probe task, viewing neutral and sensory pain-related words while their reaction time and eye movements were recorded. Eye-tracking data were analyzed by mixed-design analysis of variance with group (chronic pain versus pain-free) as the between-subjects factor, and word type (sensory pain versus neutral) as the within-subjects factor. Results showed a significant main effect for word type: all participants attended to pain-related words more than neutral words on several eye-tracking parameters. The group main effect was significant for number of fixations, which was greater in the chronic pain group. Finally, the group by word type interaction effect was significant for average visit duration, number of fixations, and total late-phase duration, all greater for sensory pain versus neutral words in the chronic pain group. As well, participants with chronic pain fixated significantly more frequently on pain words than did pain-free participants. In contrast, none of the effects for reaction time were significant. The results support the hypothesis that individuals with chronic pain display specific attentional biases toward pain-related stimuli and demonstrate the value of eye-tracking technology in measuring differences in visual attention variables.

Introduction

Selective attention to pain-related information is implicated as contributing to the cause and maintenance of chronic pain.Citation1,Citation2 This “attentional bias” involves an increase in the allocation of attentional resources to the threatening object, including the orientation,Citation3 maintenance,Citation4 and disengagement of attention.Citation5 However, evidence for attentional biases toward pain-related information in individuals with chronic pain is inconsistentCitation6Citation9 and may be related to the methodological limitations in its measurement.

Attentional biases have traditionally been inferred using paradigms such as the dot-probe task that use reaction time as the primary measure. The dot-probe task typically involves the simultaneous presentation of a threat word and a neutral word on either side of a computer screen.Citation10 The two words remain on the screen for a short duration, after which a dot is presented in the location of one of the words. The participant is to indicate the side of the screen on which the dot appears as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing one of two keys. The presence of an attentional bias toward threat-related stimuli is inferred by a shorter mean reaction time to the dot when it is presented in the location of the threat word than when the dot is presented in the location of the neutral word.Citation10

Despite its frequent use, the dot-probe task has limitations. Most prominently, the primary outcome measure of the task is reaction time, an indirect indicator of attention; it assumes that faster reaction times accurately reflect selective visual attention toward the threatening stimulus. A more accurate index of visual attention would be a measure of the eye movements of participants. To date, only one study that we are aware of has investigated visual attention patterns in the dot-probe task using eye-tracking technology in individuals with chronic pain.Citation11 The authors tracked the eye movements of 24 adults with and without chronic pain who were either high or low in fear of pain during a modified dot-probe task consisting of pain–neutral, health catastrophe–neutral, and neutral–neutral word pairs. The results did not show significant differences in reaction time according to pain status, fear of pain, or word type. However, compared with individuals who did not have chronic pain, those with chronic pain had shorter first fixation durations to health catastrophe words. These results suggest that eye-tracking may be more sensitive at identifying attentional biases than the traditional reaction time measure used for the dot-probe task.Citation12

Traditional dot-probe tasks are also limited in their ability to capture attentional biases across phases of visual attention.Citation12 Visual attention phases represent periods of attentional processing toward a stimulus that are qualitatively and temporally distinct, including initial orientation and engagement,Citation13,Citation14 maintenance and disengagement,Citation2,Citation15,Citation16 avoidance,Citation17 and reengagement.Citation18 It is possible that visual attention biases differ across visual attention phases, which have been assessed with varying exposure durations of word pairs in the classical dot-probe task. As a continuous measure of visual attention, eye-tracking permits the capture of attention at different visual attention phases within the same trial, although this has yet to be investigated.

A thorough understanding of how attentional biases relate to chronic pain must consider the biopsychosocial context of the individual.Citation19 In particular, psychological variables are linked to the development and maintenance of chronic pain.Citation19 Constructs related to anxiety are well-established, linking levels of pain catastrophizing,Citation20 fear of pain,Citation21 sensitivity to anxiety or illness,Citation22 pain anxiety,Citation23 state anxiety,Citation24 symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder,Citation25 pain vigilance,Citation26 and acceptance of chronic painCitation27 to the experience of pain. Evaluating the relationship between psychological constructs related to pain and outcome measures, such as reaction time and eye-tracking parameters, will permit a more thorough understanding of attentional biases in the context of the individual.

The present study was designed to track participants’ eye movements during a dot-probe task in order to evaluate attentional biases to sensory pain-related words in individuals reporting chronic pain. On the basis of the literature reviewed above, five primary hypotheses were tested in the present study. In comparison to pain-free participants, individuals with chronic pain will: 1) exhibit a faster reaction time to sensory pain-related words in the dot-probe task; 2) attend to sensory pain-related words more frequently; 3) exhibit a different pattern of sustained attention to sensory pain-related and neutral words; 4) show an attentional bias toward sensory pain-related words at different stages of visual attentional processing; and 5) all participants will show significant, positive correlations between reaction time scores, visual attention measures, and self-reported pain-related psychological factors.

Methods

The study was approved by the York University Research Ethics Board (Human Participants Review Subcommittee). Written informed consent was obtained from participants before beginning the study.

Recruitment

Participants were recruited through York University’s Undergraduate Research Participant Pool. Before participants enroll in studies, they complete a prescreening survey composed of questions submitted by faculty from various research laboratories. Responses to questions determine the studies that are visible to the student. For the present study, the question “do you experience physical pain on a regular basis?” was included to determine the approximate prevalence of chronic pain among undergraduate students completing the prescreen questionnaire; we defined chronic pain as the presence of ongoing pain that had persisted for 3 months or longer.Citation28 Of the 3,163 students who responded, 655 (20.71%) positively endorsed the statement, 2,508 (79.29%) did not, and 26 declined to respond. Two study postings were created to recruit participants: one was visible to students who positively endorsed the prescreen question related to pain, and the other was visible only to students who did not. Participants received one course credit for completing the study. With this method, a total of 113 participants were recruited to participate and included in the final analysis.

Materials

Hardware

Eye movements were tracked using a Tobii T60 XL eye tracker with a 24-inch widescreen monitor and a 60 Hz data rate (Tobii Technology, Falls Church, VA, USA). The eye tracker is integrated into the monitor, allowing participants to move their heads freely throughout the study. The Tobii T60 XL has an accuracy rate of 0.5° and <0.03° drift, reducing differences between what the participant is looking at and what the equipment is recording. Two Dell Precision T3400 Intel® Core™ 2 Quad CPUs with 4 GB of random-access memory (Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA) were interfaced to facilitate data collection from two software programs, E-Prime 2.0 Professional (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA) and Tobii Studio Professional 2.0 (Tobii Technology). The display resolution for the dot-probe protocol was 800×600 pixels to optimize video capture and data transfer. The study took place in a windowless room in order to standardize the lighting for all participants.

Software

E-Prime was used to design, display, collect, and prepare data for the dot-probe task.Citation29 E-Prime provides millisecond precision in the capture of reaction times. The protocol was designed in E-Studio, data was combined with E-Merge, and data was prepared for export with E-DataAid. Tobii Studio Professional 2.0 captured participant eye movements with programmed time-sensitive markers. E-Prime and Tobii Studio software programs were interfaced with E-Prime 2.0 extensions for Tobii (Psychology Software Tools). Data were analyzed with SPSS version 20.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA).

Verbal stimuli

Words were selected from previous research using the dot-probe task to test attentional biases in chronic pain populations.Citation30Citation34 Words related to the sensory experience of pain were selected over words related to other aspects of the pain experience (eg, affective experience of pain, antecedents of pain, or consequences of pain), as sensory pain words were related to larger effects in previous research.Citation8 Sensory pain words were originally drawn from the McGill Pain Questionnaire, a detailed questionnaire designed to describe diverse aspects of pain.Citation35 Sensory pain and neutral words were matched for word length and frequency.Citation36 See for a complete list of word pairs.

Table 1 Word pairs

Behavioral measures

During the dot-probe task, various behavioral measures were assessed. Reaction time (in milliseconds) was recorded to capture the speed of participant responses to the location of the dot probe. In addition, measures of visual attention were collected to determine the duration, frequency, and patterns of the participants’ gaze.

Reaction time measures

For each participant, a congruency index, incongruency index, and neutral comparison index was computed. These indexes are based on calculations by Koster et alCitation15 and Roelofs et al.Citation37 A trial is considered to be congruent when the dot probe appears in the location on the screen where the target word (ie, the sensory pain word) was presented, and incongruent when the dot probe appears in the location of the neutral word (). Each condition considers the location of the word and of the dot probe to balance individual differences in preference to look at one side of the computer screen more than the other, regardless of the location or type of word. The indexes are based on four mean reaction time (RT) scores, RT¯tldr, RT¯trdr, RT¯trdl, and RT¯tldl, which are computed by taking the mean of the 15 trials in each condition. In each formula, “t” is the target (sensory pain word), “d” is the dot probe, “l” is the left position on the screen, and “r” is the right position on the screen.

Figure 1 Visual depiction of counter-balanced trials of words presented during the dot-probe task.

Notes: Sensory pain word (target, t) on left (l) side of screen followed by the dot (d) on the left side of the screen (left, congruent or “tldl”), sensory pain word on left side of screen followed by the dot on the right (r) side of the screen (left, incongruent or “tldr”), sensory pain word on right side of screen followed by the dot on the right side of the screen (right, congruent or “trdr”), and sensory pain word on right side of screen followed by the dot on the left side of the screen (right, incongruent or “trdl”).
Figure 1 Visual depiction of counter-balanced trials of words presented during the dot-probe task.

The congruency index is calculated by taking the grand mean of the mean RTs of congruent trials that are presented on the right and left sides of the computer screen: RT¯congruent=RT¯tldl+RT¯trdr/2. Similarly, the incongruent index is calculated by taking the grand mean of the mean RTs of the responses of incongruent trials that are presented on the right and left sides of the computer screen: RT¯incongruent=RT¯trdl+RT¯tldr/2. The neutral comparison index is calculated from the neutral–neutral pairs and to permit comparison with the congruent and incongruent indexes. It is calculated by taking the grand mean of the mean reaction times for the neutral word pairs: RT¯neutral=RT¯tldr+RT¯trdr+RT¯trdl+RT¯tldl/4

Visual attention measures

Tobii Velocity-Threshold Identification (I-VT) fixation filter settings were used to classify fixations, as they provide validated and robust function parameter values that are easily replicated for future research.Citation38 As such, maximum gap interpolation (ie, merging) of fixations was set to 75 ms, the maximum angle between fixations was 0.5°, and fixations had to be greater than 60 ms.Citation39 These settings provide accurate fixation classifications for most research.Citation39 Additionally, data was screened for validity and only used if the data collected was associated with the correct eye.

Gaze patterns were recorded for the duration of the dot-probe task. Currently, there are no accepted standard dependent variables associated with eye-tracking within a dot-probe task due to the novelty of its use. Therefore, we based our dependent variables in part on a recent studyCitation21 and in part on theoretical predictions. The following dependent variables were based on eye movements recorded within a region on the left and right sides of the screen defined by the experimenter as an “area of interest” (AOI). An AOI, 250 pixels in length by 107 pixels in height, was demarcated at mirror image regions on the (center and midline of the) left and right sides of the screen within which word stimuli were presented.

Two variables were calculated based on visual fixations for each word type (sensory pain and neutral): “number of fixations” was the number of times the participant’s eyes focused on a sensory pain or neutral word within its respective AOI, and “average fixation duration” was the mean time associated with all fixations.

A “visit” was defined by one or more contiguous eye-movements within an AOI (ie, a visit ended when the eyes moved outside the AOI). Two variables were calculated on the basis of visits to the sensory pain word AOI or neutral word AOI. The “number of visits” was the total number of visits to the sensory pain word AOI or neutral word AOI, and “average visit duration” was the mean time spent looking during all visits to the sensory pain word AOI or neutral word AOI.

To investigate whether gaze patterns varied during different stages of visual processing/attention, we calculated the total fixation duration for three different periods of stimulus presentation: “early-phase total fixation duration” (0–500 ms); “middle-phase total fixation duration” (500–1,000 ms); and “late-phase total fixation duration” (1,000–2,000 ms). Each index was calculated by summing the total duration of each trial for each period.

Self-report measures

Demographics and pain-history questionnaire

Participants were asked basic demographic questions and questions about their pain history and current pain complaints, including the duration, location, frequency, and cause of any pain. They were also asked to describe the pain intensity associated with several common types of pain (tooth ache, paper cut, stubbed toe, biting your tongue, sunburn, and ear ache) on a scale from 0–10, where 0= no pain and 10= the highest pain intensity.

Pain disability

The Pain Disability Index (PDI)Citation40 is a seven-item scale that assesses the degree of daily disability due to pain. Items are scored on an eleven-point Likert scale with a total scale score range of 0–70, where higher scores reflect a greater degree of pain disability. The PDI has good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha =0.86), good test–retest reliability (r=0.44 over a 2-month period), and good concurrent validity with measures of psychological distress, pain severity, and other items measuring pain-related disability.Citation41 The internal consistency of the PDI in the present study was good (Cronbach alpha =0.89).

Pain catastrophizing

The Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS)Citation42 is a 13-item scale that measures the degree to which individuals catastrophize, ie, ruminate/worry about, magnify, and feel helpless in the face of painful experiences. Items are scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–52. Higher scores reflect more pain catastrophizing. Internal consistency is high (Cronbach alpha =0.87) and test–retest reliability is good (r=0.75 over a 6-week period).Citation42 The internal consistency of the PCS in the present study was excellent (Cronbach alpha =0.94).

Pain vigilance

The Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire (PVAQ)Citation43 is a 16-item scale that assesses an individual’s daily preoccupation with pain. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–64. Higher scores indicate greater awareness, consciousness, vigilance, and observation of pain. The PVAQ has good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha =0.86), good test–retest reliability (r=0.80 when retaken an average of 4 days later), and good convergent validity with private body consciousness (r=0.58) and a negative correlation to the ignoring pain subscale of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire (r=−0.24).Citation43 The internal consistency of the PVAQ in the present study was good (Cronbach alpha =0.87).

Pain anxiety

The Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale – Short Form (PASS-SF)Citation23 is a 20-item scale that assesses anxiety regarding pain and pain sensations. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–100. Higher scores reflect a tendency to be anxious about the experience of pain. The short form correlates highly with the original 40-item scale (r=0.97). The PASS-SF has excellent internal consistency (Cronbach alpha =0.91), reliability, and validity (Cronbach alpha =0.81).Citation23 The internal consistency of the PASS-SF in the present study was excellent (Cronbach alpha =0.93).

Chronic pain acceptance

The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ)Citation44 is a 20-item scale that measures the tendency to accept the experience of pain and to engage in daily activities despite feeling pain. All items are scored on a seven-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–120. Greater acceptance of chronic pain is reflected in higher scores. It has good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha =0.85), adequate reliability, and good convergent validity with related psychosocial distress and physical functioning questionnaires.Citation45 The internal consistency of the CPAQ in the present study was acceptable (Cronbach alpha =0.74).

State anxiety

The state version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S)Citation46 is a 20-item scale that assesses current feelings of anxiety. Each item is rated on a four-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 20–80. High scores are reflective of higher levels of state anxiety. Internal consistency is high (Cronbach alpha ranges from 0.86–0.95). Test–retest reliability is fairly low (r ranges from 0.16–0.62) because state levels of anxiety are expected to change with time.Citation47 The STAI-S has well-established construct and concurrent validity.Citation48 The internal consistency of the STAI-S in the present study was excellent (Cronbach alpha =0.94).

Anxiety sensitivity

The Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3)Citation49 is an 18-item questionnaire designed to assess beliefs and fear associated with anxiety-related sensations (specifically, that the symptoms of anxiety have harmful consequences). Items are scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–72. Higher scores reflect a greater tendency to be fearful of the experience of anxiety symptoms. The internal consistency for the subscales ranges from good to excellent (Cronbach alpha ranges from 0.80–0.90). The ASI-3 also has good reliability (Cronbach alpha =0.93).Citation50 The internal consistency of the ASI-3 in the present study was good (Cronbach alpha =0.89).

Illness sensitivity

The Illness Sensitivity Index – Short Version (ISI-SF)Citation51 is an eleven-item scale that assesses the fear of experiencing illness. Items are scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–44. Higher scores indicate a higher level of fear. The ISI has good internal consistency (Cronbach alpha >0.80)Citation51 and total scale reliability (r=0.89). The internal consistency of the ISI-SF in the present study was excellent (Cronbach alpha =0.91).

Sensitivity to pain traumatization

The Sensitivity to Pain Traumatization Scale (SPTS)Citation25 is a 20-item measure of anxiety-related cognitive, emotional, and behavioral reactions to pain that resemble symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Items are scored on a five-point Likert scale, and the total score range is 0–80. The scale was developed with an exploratory factor analysis on the items of the PASS-SF, PCS, and ASI-3. The SPTS has good psychometric properties: the concurrent validity was established by comparing people with chronic pain and without chronic pain and it shows good convergent validity with related measures.Citation25 The internal consistency of the SPTS in the present study was excellent (Cronbach alpha =0.91).

Procedure

After arriving at the lab and providing informed consent, participants completed a computer-administered survey consisting of the ten questionnaires described above. The demographic and pain history questions appeared at the beginning of the survey for all participants. The order of the remaining questionnaires was randomized for each participant using an online survey manager (Sona Systems, Bethesda, MD, USA). Participants were led to a windowless room to complete the dot-probe task. They were positioned approximately 60 cm in front of the screen with eyes level with the center of the screen. Five-point eye calibration was performed with Tobii Studio software to customize the eye-tracking specifications to each participant. The computer input was adjusted to run E-Prime from a second computer, whereupon a second eye calibration was performed. Next, the dot-probe task was introduced and described. Participants were told that they were to fix their gaze at the central fixation cross, that a pair of words would appear briefly on the screen and that a dot would be presented in the location of one of the words. Their task was to identify the location of the dot as quickly and accurately as possible. Participants then completed 12 practice trials to familiarize themselves with the task.

The protocol for each trial consisted of three parts: 1) a fixation cross (“+”) appeared at the center of the screen for 500 ms; 2) two words in bold, Arial font in size 24 (sensory pain–neutral or neutral–neutral) were presented simultaneously, one on the left side and the other on the right side of the screen for 2,000 ms; and 3) immediately after the offset of the words, a dot probe (“•”) appeared on the left or right side of the screen (in the prior location of the word) for 2,000 ms or until the participant responded by pressing one of two keyboard keys. Participants were told to press the “P” with their right index finger if the dot probe was on the right side of the screen and the “Q” with their left index finger if the dot probe was on the left. All text and symbols appeared in black on a white background. Word pairs were counter-balanced, controlling for word type location and dot probe location, so that each word pair was presented four times (). In total, participants completed 120 trials (four trials each of the 15 sensory pain–neutral word pairs and four trials each of the 15 neutral–neutral word pairs). The presentation order of trials was randomly generated by E-Prime (ie, the order selection was set to “random”) for each participant.

Statistical analysis

Comparisons of nominal data categories, such as participants in the chronic pain group in comparison to pain-free participants, were made with chi-square tests of independence. The reaction time indexes were analyzed using a mixed design 2×3 analysis of variance (ANOVA) using group (chronic pain, pain-free) as the independent-samples factor and index type (congruent, incongruent, and neutral comparison indexes) as the within-subjects factor. Number of fixations, average fixation duration, number of visits, average visit duration, early-phase total fixation duration, middle-phase total fixation duration, and late-phase total fixation duration were analyzed by a series of 2×2 mixed-design ANOVAs using group (chronic pain, pain-free) as the between-subjects factor and word type (sensory pain, neutral) as the within-subjects factor. Significant interactions were proceeded with simple main effects analyses with the alpha criterion adjusted to P=0.01 to account for the number of comparisons.Citation52 Correlation analyses were used to examine the strength of the linear relationships between total questionnaire scores and group, reaction time, and visual attention.

Results

Demographic information

As noted in the Recruitment section, the study was advertised to the 655 students who endorsed the pre-screen statement “Do you experience pain on a regular basis?”. Of these, 113 signed up and participated. We do not have data on why the remaining eligible participants did not sign up for the study. The age range of the sample of 113 participants was 18–44 years, mean (M)age= 21.32, standard deviation (SD)age =4.35. Eighty-four participants were female (74.30%) and 29 were male (25.70%). The sample was ethnically diverse, with participants self-identifying as Caucasian (32.54%), South Asian (28.57%), African (10.32%), East Asian (9.53%), Middle Eastern (8.73%), Hispanic/Latino (4.76%), Caribbean (3.97%), Aboriginal (0.79%), or undisclosed (0.79%). Forty-nine participants wore vision-correction aids during testing, with 16 (14.2%) using contact lenses and 33 (29.2%) using glasses.

For the purpose of the present study, chronic pain was defined as the presence of ongoing pain that had persisted for 3 months or longer.Citation28 According to this criterion, 51 participants (45.13%) reported experiencing chronic pain. Of these, seven (13.70%) reported experiencing pain for 3–6 months, five (9.80%) for 6–12 months, and 39 (76.50%) for 12 months or longer. Participants reported pain in one to five body locations (Mlocation=2.16, SDlocation =1.08): 30 reported neck and/or back pain, 22 reported headache/migraine pain, 21 reported ankle and/or knee pain, 15 reported shoulder pain, 12 reported stomach pain, five reported hip(s) pain, two reported arm pain, one reported eye pain, one reported jaw pain, and one declined to respond to this question. Most participants reported that the pain commenced after an injury (47.1%) or that they were unsure how the pain started (35.3%), while 7.8% of participants stated that it started due to overuse and/or stress, 5.9% due to an illness, and one (0.79%) due to a hereditary predisposition. Frequency of pain was reported as daily (64.7%), weekly (33.3%), or other (2.0%) with the average intensity as mild (15.7%), moderate (74.5%), or severe (9.8%). When completing the questionnaires, 30 participants reported their present pain intensity as mild (73.3%) or moderate (26.6%) pain. Approximately half (52.9%) of chronic pain participants used painkillers on a regular basis. Of those participants, 63.0% used nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, 59.3% used acetaminophen, 25.9% used aspirin, 7.4% used opioids, and 29.6% reported using other types of painkiller.

Participants reporting pain (n=51) did not differ significantly from those not reporting pain (n=62) on age, t(111)=0.71, P=0.705, sex, χ2 (2, n=113)=0.001, P=0.969, use of contact lenses, χ2 (1, n=113)=0.014, P=0.905, or glasses, χ2 (1, n=113)=0.77, P=0.381. Pearson chi-square 2×4 analysis using pain status (chronic pain, pain-free) and ethnicity (African descent, Asian descent, Caucasian, other) identified a significant difference according to ethnicity, χ2 (3, n=113)=18.93, P<0.001. Post-hoc comparisons showed a significantly greater proportion of participants self-identifying as Caucasian among those reporting pain.

Data preparation

Reaction time measures

On the dot-probe task, incorrect trials or trials that had reaction times >2,000 ms were excluded from the calculation of the mean reaction time scores.Citation15 Reaction times for these trials do not indicate a bias toward the measured variables and therefore may artificially skew the data. According to these criteria, 1.02% of trials were excluded from the calculation of the mean reaction time scores. Missing reaction time scores were prorated if 80% or more of trials were available.Citation52,Citation53 One participant with insufficient data was excluded from the analysis of the reaction time data.

Visual attention measures

Eye-tracking recordings were screened for the quality of visual-gaze capture. Of the 135 participants recruited, only those whose gaze capture exceeded 75% were included, consistent with previous eye-tracking research.Citation4 Lower gaze capture may be related to compromised attention, consistently lowered/closed eyelids, or reflections off of glasses that obscure the visual recording.Citation55 This led to the exclusion of 20 participants with poor capture (Mcapture=60.90%, SDcapture =12.52%) and two participants who were unable to be calibrated. The mean eye capture for included participants (n=113) was 88.44%, SD =5.34%.

Participants included in the final analysis did not differ significantly from those with insufficient gaze data (n=22) on age, t(133)=0.71, P=0.481, use of contact lenses, χ2 (1, n=135)=5.50, P=0.064, glasses, χ2 (1, n=135)=0.45, P=0.504, or presence of chronic pain, χ2 (1, n=135)=0.58, P=0.448. There was a significantly greater proportion of males, χ2 (1, n=135)=11.84, P=0.003. Pearson chi-square 2×4 analysis using pain status (chronic pain, pain-free) and ethnicity (African descent, Asian descent, Caucasian, other) identified a significant difference according to ethnicity, χ2 (3, n=135)=7.749, P=0.051. Post-hoc comparisons showed a significantly greater proportion of participants self-identifying as Caucasian among those with sufficient gaze data.

Questionnaires

Missing questionnaire items were prorated if 80% or more of trials were available.Citation53,Citation54 One participant responded to fewer than 80% of the questions on the ASI-3, STAI-S, and SPTS and was consequently excluded from the analysis of these questionnaires.

Normality

There was no evidence that assumptions of normality were violated in the present data set: sphericity was not violated according to Box’s Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices and Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances did not show evidence that homogeneity of variance was violated. Shapiro-Wilk Test of Normality and an examination of skewness and kurtosis according to a test statistic adapted from CramerCitation56 (sample skewness divided by the standard error of skewness), did not suggest violations in normality. Exceptions for any tests are discussed in conjunction with the specific analysis below.

Hypothesis 1: dot-probe task reaction time measures

shows the mean reaction times for the congruency, incongruency, and neutral indexes according to chronic pain and pain-free participants. A mixed-design ANOVA evaluated the relationship between group and index type (congruency, incongruency, and neutral comparison index). Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity was significant, χ2(2)=9.30, P=0.01 which necessitated a Huynh-Feldt correction.Citation57 Significant effects were not found for group, F(1, 110)=0.59, P=0.451, ηp2=0.012, index type, F(1.90, 208.57)=2.27, P=0.113, ηp2=0.021, or the group by index type interaction, F(1.90, 208.57)=0.70, P=0.490, ηp2=0.006.

Table 2 Hypothesis 1: reaction time index scores (in milliseconds) for the chronic pain group (n=51) and the pain-free group (n=62)

Hypothesis 2: frequency measures of visual attention using eye-tracking

shows the means and standard deviations for the visual attention measures by group. Mixed-design ANOVAs evaluated differences between group (chronic pain versus pain free) and word type (sensory pain versus neutral) for all visual attention measures. For number of fixations, the main effect of group, F(1, 111)=4.55, P=0.035, ηp2=0.039, word type, F(1, 111)=66.03, P<0.001, ηp2=0.373, and the group by word type interaction were significant, F(1, 111)=6.06, P=0.015, ηp2=0.052. Simple main effects showed that the number of fixations on threat words was greater for participants with chronic pain than for pain-free participants, F(1,111)=6.46, P=0.012, ηp2=0.055, and the number of fixations on sensory pain words was significantly greater than that for neutral words among pain-free participants, F(1, 111)=17.77, P<0.001, ηp2=0.138, and for chronic pain participants, F(1, 111)=51.07, P<0.001, ηp2=0.315 ().

Figure 2 Frequency of fixations for each group.

Notes: Error bars display the standard error. **P<0.01.
Figure 2 Frequency of fixations for each group.

Table 3 Hypotheses 2–4: frequency and duration of eye-tracking dependent variables for the chronic pain group (n=51) and the pain-free group (n=62)

For number of visits, only the main effect of word type was significant, F(1, 111)=34.37, P<0.001, ηp2=0.236 [group: F(1, 111)=1.64, P=0.204, ηp2=0.015, group by word type interaction: F(1, 111)=2.21, P=0.140, ηp2=0.020].

Hypothesis 3: measures of sustained visual attention using eye-tracking

For average fixation duration, none of the effects was significant [group: F(1, 111)=2.93, P=0.090, ηp2=0.026, word type: F(1, 111)=0.41, P=0.523, ηp2=0.004, group by word type interaction: F(1, 111)=1.16, P=0.283, ηp2=0.010].

For average visit duration, the main effect of word type, F(1, 111)=22.34, P<0.001, ηp2=0.168, and the group by word type interaction were significant, F(1, 111)=4.15, P=0.044, ηp2=0.036 [group: F(1, 111)=0.03, P=0.876, ηp2=0.000]. Simple main effects showed that the average visit duration for sensory pain words was significantly longer than that for neutral words among participants with chronic pain, F(1,111)=20.839, P<0.001, ηp2=0.158 ().

Figure 3 Average visit duration for each group.

Notes: Error bars display the standard error. **P<0.01.
Figure 3 Average visit duration for each group.

Hypothesis 4: measures of visual attention according to presentation phase

The pattern of results was the same for the early-phase and middle-phase total fixation duration. For early-phase total fixation duration, only the main effect of word type was significant, F(1, 111)=8.24, P=0.005, ηp2=0.069 [group: F(1, 111)=3.42, P=0.067, ηp2=0.030, group by word type interaction: F(1, 111)=0.28, P=0.599, ηp2=0.002]. Similarly, for middle-phase total fixation duration only the main effect of word type was significant, F(1, 111)=44.10, P<0.001, ηp2=0.284 [group: F(1, 111)=0.13, P=0.721, ηp2=0.001, group by word type interaction: F(1, 111)=1.58, P=0.211, ηp2=0.014].

In contrast, for late-phase total fixation duration, the main effect of word type, F(1, 111)=12.59, P=0.001, ηp2=0.102, and the group by word type interaction were significant, F(1, 111)=5.594, P=0.020, ηp2=0.048 [group: F(1, 111)=0.436, P=0.510, ηp2=0.004]. Simple main effects showed that the total late-phase fixation duration for sensory pain words was significantly longer than that for neutral words among participants with chronic pain, F(1,111)=15.94, P<0.001, ηp2=0.126 ().

Figure 4 Late-phase total fixation duration for each group.

Notes: Error bars display the standard error. **P<0.01.
Figure 4 Late-phase total fixation duration for each group.

Hypothesis 5: relationship between questionnaires and behavioral measures

Group effects

shows the means and SDs for the questionnaire total scores for participants with and without chronic pain. Groups were compared with Student’s t-tests, and the Type I error rate was adjusted to P=0.006 using a Bonferroni correction to adjust for alpha error rate inflation.Citation52 Chronic pain participants reported significantly higher levels of pain disability and chronic pain acceptance.

Table 4 Hypothesis 5: comparisons between the chronic pain group (n=51) and pain-free group (n=62) according to questionnaire data

Correlations among reaction time and visual attention measures

Pearson correlations evaluated the relationship between questionnaires and behavioral measures. Only correlations with sensory pain words were examined. The Type I error rate was adjusted to P=0.003 for reaction time measures and to P=0.001 for visual attention measures with a Bonferroni correction to control error associated with the large number of comparisons.Citation52 One-tailed tests were used, consistent with predicted outcomes. Five correlations were significant for participants with chronic pain: PCS total score and incongruency index, r(51)=0.38, P=0.003, CPAQ total score and congruency index, r(51)=−0.39, P=0.002, CPAQ total score and incongruency index, r(51)=−0.38, P=0.003, and ISI-SF total score and late-phase total fixation duration, r(51)=0.482, P<0.001. Only the correlation the STAI-S total score and early-phase total fixation duration was significant for pain-free participants, r(61)=−0.38, P=0.001.

Discussion

The present study used eye-tracking technology to compare attentional biases to threat versus neutral words in individuals with and without chronic pain using a dot-probe task. Significant differences were not found for reaction time. In contrast, significant group by word type interaction effects were found for several eye-tracking measures: number of fixations, average visit duration, and late-phase total fixation duration. These findings indicate that individuals with chronic pain, but not those who are pain-free, display a bias toward sensory pain words ( and ) and that individuals with chronic pain differ from those who are pain-free in terms of how frequently they attend to sensory pain words (). Taken together, these findings suggest that eye-tracking technology provides more sensitive measures of a bias than does the dot-probe task.

As noted above, dot-probe reaction times did not show evidence of an attentional bias, contrary to the expectations of our first hypothesis (). There are several possible explanations for the absence of an effect. The most likely one is the limitation associated with using reaction time as the sole measure of attention.Citation12,Citation58 Reaction time is an indirect indicator of visual attention, where faster congruent reaction times suggest that the participant’s gaze is on the threat word before the dot appears on the screen,Citation10 creating a substantial margin of error. Another possibility is that the exposure duration used in the present study was longer than that in other studies.Citation2 To our knowledge, only one other study has used an exposure duration of 2,000 ms in a sample of people with pain, and the results also failed to show significant differences.Citation11 However, this explanation does not appear to account for the lack of differences in reaction time, since a recent meta-analysisCitation8 of dot-probe studies showed larger effect sizes associated with exposure durations longer than 1,000 ms in comparison to exposure durations between 0–500 ms and 500–1,000 ms. Another possibility may be the nature of the sample used in the present study; namely, students who self-reported experiencing chronic pain. Attentional biases have been more consistently identified with reaction times using the dot-probe task in individuals recruited from clinical settings.Citation37,Citation59,Citation60 Individuals seeking treatment for pain likely experience greater distress and preoccupation with painful sensations, leading to greater attentional biases that then become detectable with the dot-probe task. This suggests that only substantial attentional biases can be identified with reaction time as the outcome measure, whereas smaller biases can be identified with eye-tracking, as in the present study.

In contrast to the lack of findings using reaction time as the bias measure, number of fixations, average visit duration, and late-phase total fixation duration showed evidence for a bias toward pain-related stimuli in individuals with chronic pain, supporting hypotheses 2–4. Of particular interest is the finding that a bias to sensory pain words, as measured by fixation duration, was only evident in the later stage of attentional processing and not the early or middle stages (). Because later stages of attentional processing are associated with more top-down, conscious processes, such as avoidance and re-engagement,Citation17,Citation18 this supports the proposition that increased attention toward pain-related stimuli reflects more of an explicit, cognitive bias rather than an automatic, preattentive bias.Citation2,Citation8 That is, the bias becomes evident only once the threat has been identified and its meaning and relevance have become apparent. The late-phase findings are consistent with the work of Pincus and MorleyCitation6 who proposed the presence of an explicit cognitive-processing bias in chronic pain.

In contrast, number of fixations and average visit duration ( and ) were calculated across the course of each trial (ie, 0–2,000 ms) so that the significant interactions may reflect a bias in any one or more of the following phases of attention: initial orientation and engagement, maintenance, or re-engagement.Citation2,Citation13Citation16,Citation18 Given the simplicity of the dot-probe task, it may be that the associated attentional processing required by participants was not sufficiently intentional and conscious to be considered controlled, but the present results do not permit a conclusion regarding the stage(s) of processing that underlies the present effects. Nevertheless, the results clearly show that participants with chronic pain fixated more frequently on pain words than did pain-free participants () and that the average visit duration for sensory pain words was significantly longer than that for neutral words among participants with chronic pain (). Future research might consider a finer-grained analysis to determine the extent to which these significant effects are guided by early (eg, bottom-up, preattentive) or later (top-down, cognitive) processes.

Eye-tracking measures also detected significant word type and group by word type interaction effects () independent of pain status. Word type effects showed that all participants looked more frequently at and for longer durations on sensory pain words than neutral words. This supports the idea that humans are evolutionarily predisposed to attend to threats in the environment: automatic processing provides information regarding the threat to prepare for a behavioral response, such as escape or confrontation.Citation60Citation62 Because sensory pain words have a higher threat value than neutral words, this may have led to increased attention by all participants.

We expected to find significant correlations among reaction time scores, visual attention measures, and self-reported pain-related psychological factors. For participants with chronic pain, only five correlations were significant, providing provisional support for hypothesis 5. We found that higher pain catastrophizing scores were related to higher incongruency scores. As high incongruency scores reflect longer response times to trials in which the dot appears behind the neutral words, this suggests that participants high in catastrophizing have a bias away from neutral words. This is consistent with the increased level of vigilance proposed in individuals that catastrophize.Citation42,Citation64 Both the congruency and incongruency indexes were negatively related to chronic pain acceptance scores, suggesting that participants higher in chronic pain acceptance respond faster on all trials, regardless of the location of the dot. Since pain has been linked to diminished performance on cognitive tasksCitation65,Citation66 as a form of interruption,Citation1 it is possible that greater levels of acceptance of the pain experience increased the level of performance in the dot-probe task. The finding that participants with higher illness sensitivity scores spent more time looking at threat words during the late-phase for total fixation duration shows that the more fearful a participant is of experiencing illness, the more likely they are to attend to pain-related stimuli. Overall, there is some evidence to support the hypothesis of a relationship between pain-related psychological factors and attentional patterns among participants with chronic pain.

The present study has important clinical implications regarding the development and course of chronic pain. According to the Fear-Avoidance Model, increased attention toward pain sensations can exacerbate symptoms, leading to avoidance of activities that produce pain, physical deconditioning, and consequently more pain.Citation67 In the Misdirected Problem-Solving Model, increasing attention to pain-related stimuli disrupts routine cognitive functioning and promotes worry, motivating the individual with pain to engage in problem-solving behavior (eg, taking medication, seeing the doctor).Citation68 However, if the pain persists after problem-solving behavior, the attentional activation continues to contribute to the cycle of worry, hypervigilance, and pain interruption, thereby worsening outcomes. This model suggests that acceptance of chronic pain, therefore, can reduce psychological symptoms such as distress, anxiety, and depression, as well as physical symptoms such as pain and disability.Citation69 This is supported by the finding that increased levels of chronic pain acceptance are associated with a reduced attentional bias toward pain-related information.Citation70 Therefore, if attentional biases causally contribute to the development and maintenance of chronic pain as has been proposed, then eye-tracking technology can be used to monitor progress and efficacy of pain-management interventions.

The present study had several limitations. First, due to programming limitations, we were unable to collect data related to the direction of first fixation and first fixation duration for each trial, an index used in previous research.Citation11,Citation21 This would have captured early attention more effectively.Citation4 Second, the present study used words related to the sensory aspects of pain to detect attentional biases. Words reflect a semantic understanding of threat and may not adequately capture automatic processing responses that people with chronic pain experience.Citation1,Citation7 Future studies should consider using pictures related to pain (eg, images of others experiencing pain, injuries, or objects that may cause pain) to increase the ecological validity of the visual stimuli. Third, the chronic pain group was heterogeneous: pain was experienced in a wide variety of body locations (most notably the neck/back and headache/migraine pain) and the frequency and severity of pain was varied. It is possible that the presence and direction of attentional biases may differ according to these qualities. This is consistent with the finding that attentional biases, at least as measured by the traditional dot-probe task, are not found in populations that experience procedural pain, acute pain, and experimental pain.Citation8 Fourth, this study did not include a comprehensive assessment of the participants’ medical treatment or psychiatric history. Since attentional biases have been more consistently identified in clinical-pain populationsCitation37,Citation59,Citation60 and in individuals diagnosed with anxiety disorders,Citation71 these variables may have contributed to the present findings. Relatedly, pain and psychopharmacological medication may also contribute to reaction time and gaze-duration variables: if the experience of pain was less salient during study testing due to medication, this may have led to smaller attentional biases. Future studies should perform a comprehensive assessment of the medical and psychiatric history of participants and control for medication use.

A final relevant issue is related to the processing of threat-related material. Most studies using the dot-probe investigate attentional biases toward pain-related information. However, it is possible that some people display a bias away from pain-related stimuli, as predicted by the fear-avoidance model of chronic pain.Citation72 If this were the case, it is possible that the absence of an overall effect of an attentional bias reported in many studies actually reflects a bias toward pain-related stimuli in some individuals and a bias away from pain-related stimuli in others. This pattern of results would mask the effects of the two biases that operate in opposite directions and would produce an overall nonsignificant finding. Future studies should evaluate whether, in whom, and under what circumstances biases are evident toward and away from pain-related stimuli.

In summary, the present study used eye-tracking methodology to investigate attentional biases toward sensory pain words in individuals with and without chronic pain. The results showed that all participants attended to stimuli related to pain more than to neutral stimuli and that this effect was more pronounced among participants with chronic pain as measured by number of fixations, average visit duration, and late-phase total fixation duration. As well, a larger effect was present in later stages of attentional processing, suggesting the presence of a top-down cognitive bias toward sensory pain words. In contrast, the traditional dot-probe reaction time measures did not show evidence for an attentional bias. Taken together, these findings suggest that eye-tracking technology provides a more accurate and sensitive measure of selective visual attentional biases than reaction time.

Acknowledgments

Samantha Fashler is supported by an Ontario Graduate Scholarship and a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Frederick Banting and Charles Best CGS Master’s Award. Joel Katz is supported by a CIHR Canada Research Chair (CRC) in Health Psychology. Funds to support the project were obtained from Dr Katz’s CRC. We are grateful to Dr. Paul Ritvo for providing partial funding for the purchase of the Tobii eye-tracker through a Federal Development Grant. This article was derived, in part, from Samantha Fashler’s master’s thesis.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • EcclestonCCrombezGPain demands attention: a cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of painPsychol Bull1999125335636610349356
  • SchothDENunesVDLiossiCAttentional bias towards pain-related information in chronic pain; a meta-analysis of visual-probe investigationsClin Psychol Rev2012321132522100743
  • LiossiCSchothDEGodwinHJLiversedgeSPUsing eye movements to investigate selective attention in chronic daily headachePain2014155350351024287436
  • VervoortTTrostZPrkachinKMMuellerSCAttentional processing of other’s facial display of pain: an eye tracking studyPain2013154683684423561271
  • Van DammeSCrombezGEcclestonCDisengagement from pain: the role of catastrophic thinking about painPain20041071–2707614715391
  • PincusTMorleySCognitive-processing bias in chronic pain: a review and integrationPsychol Bull2001127559961711548969
  • AsmundsonGJCarletonRNEkongJDot-probe evaluation of selective attentional processing of pain cues in patients with chronic headachesPain20051141–225025615733651
  • CrombezGVan RyckeghemDMEcclestonCVan DammeSAttentional bias to pain-related information: a meta-analysisPain2013154449751023333054
  • DearBFSharpeLNicholasMKRefshaugeKThe psychometric properties of the dot-probe paradigm when used in pain-related attentional bias researchJ Pain201112121247125421982721
  • MacLeodCMathewsATataPAttentional bias in emotional disordersJ Abnorm Psychol198695115203700842
  • YangZJacksonTChenHEffects of chronic pain and pain-related fear on orienting and maintenance of attention: an eye movement studyJ Pain201314101148115723850178
  • SharpeLAttentional biases in pain: more complex than originally thought?Pain2014155343944024342463
  • BradleyBPMoggKMillarNHCovert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxietyCogn Emot2000146789808
  • GambleALRapeeRMThe time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescentsJ Anxiety Disord200923784184719447004
  • KosterEHCrombezGVerschuereBDe HouwerJSelective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengageBehav Res Ther200442101183119215350857
  • KosterEHVerschuereBCrombezGVan DammeSTime-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxietyBehav Res Ther20054381087109815922291
  • HermansDVansteenwegenDEelenPEye movement registration as a continuous index of attention deployment: data from a group of spider anxious studentsCogn Emot1999134419434
  • HeerenALievensLPhilippotPHow does attention training work in social phobia: disengagement from threat or re-engagement to non-threat?J Anxiety Disord20112581108111521907539
  • GatchelRJPengYBPetersMLFuchsPNTurkDCThe biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: scientific advances and future directionsPsychol Bull2007133458162417592957
  • PicavetHSVlaeyenJWSchoutenJSPain catastrophizing and kinesiophobia: predictors of chronic low back painAm J Epidemiol2002156111028103412446259
  • YangZJacksonTGaoXChenHIdentifying selective visual attention biases related to fear of pain by tracking eye movements within a dot-probe paradigmPain201215381742174822717101
  • ZvolenskyMJGoodieJLMcNeilDWSperryJASorrellJTAnxiety sensitivity in the prediction of pain-related fear and anxiety in a heterogeneous chronic pain populationBehav Res Ther200139668369611400712
  • McCrackenLMDhingraLA short version of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20): preliminary development and validityPain Res Manag200271455016231066
  • AsmundsonGJKatzJUnderstanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: state-of-the-artDepress Anxiety2009261088890119691031
  • KleimanVClarkeHKatzJSensitivity to pain traumatization: a higher-order factor underlying pain-related anxiety, pain catastrophizing and anxiety sensitivity among patients scheduled for major surgeryPain Res Manag201116316917721766066
  • AldrichSEcclestonCCrombezGWorrying about chronic pain: vigilance to threat and misdirected problem solvingBehav Res Ther200038545747010816905
  • McCrackenLMEcclestonCA prospective study of acceptance of pain and patient functioning with chronic painPain20051181–216416916203093
  • MerskeyHBogdukNTask Force on Taxonomy of the International Association for the Study of PainClassification of Chronic Pain: Descriptions of Chronic Pain Syndromes and Definitions of Pain TermsSeattle, WAIASP Press1994
  • SchneiderWEschmanAZuccolottoAE-Prime User’s GuidePittsburgh, PAPsychology Software Tools Inc2002
  • AsmundsonGJWrightKDHadjistavropoulosHDHypervigilance and attentional fixedness in chronic musculoskeletal pain: consistency of findings across modified stroop and dot-probe tasksJ Pain20056849750616084464
  • VagoDRNakamuraYSelective attentional bias towards pain-related threat in fibromyalgia: preliminary evidence for effects of mindfulness meditation trainingCogn Ther Res2011356581594
  • KeoghEElleryDHuntCHannentISelective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli amongst pain fearful individualsPain2001911–29110011240081
  • AsmundsonGJKuperosJLNortonGRDo patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information?: Preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fearPain1997721–227329272784
  • SniderBSAsmundsonGJWieseKCAutomatic and strategic processing of threat cues in patients with chronic pain: a modified stroop evaluationClin J Pain200016214415410870727
  • MelzackRThe McGill Pain Questionnaire: major properties and scoring methodsPain1975132772991235985
  • KučeraHFrancisWNComputational Analysis of Present-Day American EnglishProvidence, RIBrown University Press1967
  • RoelofsJPetersMLFassaertTVlaeyenJWThe role of fear of movement and injury in selective attentional processing in patients with chronic low back pain: a dot-probe evaluationJ Pain20056529430015890631
  • SalvucciDDGoldbergJHIdentifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols: Proceedings of the Eye Tracking Research and Applications Symposium, Palm Beach Gardens, Florida, United States, 6–8 November 2000New York, NYACM Press2000
  • KomogortsevOVGobertDVJayarathnaSKohDHGowdaSStandardization of automated analyses of oculomotor fixation and saccadic behaviorsIEEE Trans Biomed Eng2010571126352645
  • PollardCAPreliminary validity study of the pain disability indexPercept Mot Skills19845939746240632
  • TaitRCChibnallJTKrauseSThe Pain Disability Index: psychometric propertiesPain19904021711822308763
  • SullivanMJLBishopSRPivikJThe pain catastrophizing scale: development and validationPsychol Assess199574524532
  • McCrackenLM“Attention” to pain in persons with chronic pain: a behavioral approachBehav Ther1997282271284
  • VowlesKEMcCrackenLMMcLeodCEcclestonCThe Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire: confirmatory factor analysis and identification of patient subgroupsPain2008140228429118824301
  • McCrackenLMVowlesKEEcclestonCAcceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment methodPain20041071–215916614715402
  • SpielbergerCDGorsuchRLLusheneREVaggPRJacobsGAManual for the State-Trait Anxiety InventoryPalo Alto, CAConsulting Psychologists Press1983
  • SpielbergerCDAnxiety: Current Trends in Theory and ResearchNew York, NYAcademic Press Inc1972
  • SpielbergerCDState-Trait Anxiety Inventory: A Comprehensive BibliographyPalo Alto, CAConsulting Psychologists Press1989
  • TaylorSZvolenskyMJCoxBJRobust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3Psychol Assess200719217618817563199
  • WheatonMGDeaconBJMcGrathPBBermanNCAbramowitzJSDimensions of anxiety sensitivity in the anxiety disorders: evaluation of the ASI-3J Anxiety Disord201226340140822306133
  • TaylorSThe structure of fundamental fearsJ Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry19932442892998077447
  • BonferroniCETeoria Statistica delle Classi e Calcolo delle Probabilità. [Statistical class theory and calculation of probability]Firenze, ItalyLibreria internazionale Seeber1936 Italian
  • HeckmanJJSample selection bias as a specification errorEconometrica1979471153161
  • RubinDBInference and missing dataBiometrika1976633581592
  • DuchowskiAEye Tracking Methodology: Theory and Practice2nd edLondon, UKSpringer-Verlag London Limited2007
  • CramerDBasic Statistics for Social Research: Step-by-Step Calculations and Computer Techniques using MinitabNew York, NYRoutledge1997
  • HuynhHFeldtLSEstimation of the Box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in randomized block and split-plot designsJ Educ Behav Stat1976116982
  • SchmukleSCUnreliability of the dot probe taskEur J Pers2005197595605
  • DehghaniMSharpeLNicholasMKSelective attention to pain-related information in chronic musculoskeletal pain patientsPain20031051–2374614499418
  • KhatibiADehghaniMSharpeLAsmundsonGJPouretemadHSelective attention towards painful faces among chronic pain patients: evidence from a modified version of the dot-probePain20091421–2424719201094
  • PrattoFJohnOPAutomatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social informationJ Pers Soc Psychol19916133803911941510
  • OhmanAThe role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of threatPsychoneuroendocrinology2005301095395815963650
  • ÖhmanAWiensSThe concept of an evolved fear module and cognitive theories of anxietyMansteadASRFrijdaNFischerAFeelings and Emotions: The Amsterdam SymposiumCambridge, UKCambridge University Press20045880
  • SullivanMJThornBHaythornthwaiteJATheoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and painClin J Pain2001171526411289089
  • RothRSGeisserMETheisen-GoodvichMDixonPJCognitive complaints are associated with depression, fatigue, female sex, and pain catastrophizing in patients with chronic painArch Phys Med Rehabil20058661147115415954053
  • McCrackenLMIversonGLPredicting complaints of impaired cognitive functioning in patients with chronic painJ Pain Symptom Manage200121539239611369160
  • VlaeyenJWLintonSJFear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the artPain200085331733210781906
  • EcclestonCCrombezGWorry and chronic pain: a misdirected problem solving modelPain2007132323323617961924
  • McCrackenLMEcclestonCCoping or acceptance: what to do about chronic pain?Pain20031051–219720414499436
  • VianeICrombezGEcclestonCDevulderJDe CorteWAcceptance of the unpleasant reality of chronic pain: effects upon attention to pain and engagement with daily activitiesPain2004112328228815561383
  • Bar-HaimYLamyDPergaminLBakermans-KranenburgMJvan IJzendoornMHThreat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic studyPsychol Bull2007133112417201568
  • CrombezGEcclestonCVan DammeSVlaeyenJWKarolyPFear-avoidance model of chronic pain: the next generationClin J Pain201228647548322673479