216
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Comparing the ability of cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescent onset schizophrenia

, , , , , , , , & show all
Pages 937-945 | Published online: 27 Mar 2017

Abstract

Background

Evidence in the literature suggests that there is an impairment of social cognition in schizophrenia. Theory of Mind (ToM) is defined as one’s ability to understand others’ wishes, beliefs, intentions, and other psychological states and thereby to judge others’ behavior, as an essential component of social cognition. However, there have been limited studies on social cognition, especially ToM in adolescent onset schizophrenia (AOS). The current study aims to investigate ToM abilities in adolescent schizophrenia according to various ToM subcomponents (cognitive ToM and affective ToM) and various ToM orders (first order and second order).

Methods

This study examines ToM in 35 adolescent schizophrenic patients and 35 healthy adolescents using the “Yoni task” and “Faux Pas Recognition test” to assess their affective and cognitive ToM abilities.

Results

In the Yoni task, patients with AOS showed differences in ToM abilities either on a different order or under different conditions. The Faux Pas Recognition task results revealed that AOS patients were not always able to recognize a faux pas or understand complicated emotions under the faux pas scenario. Furthermore, as indicated by the correlation analysis, neither cognitive ToM nor affective ToM was related to the patients’ symptoms, disease duration, dose of medication, or intelligence quotient (IQ).

Conclusion

Our findings showed AOS impairment in the performance of ToM tasks. It seemed that impairment in second-order-ToM is more serious. Moreover, these deficits are largely independent of symptom clusters, disease duration, dose of medication, and IQ. It can be speculated that ToM dysfunction may be a hallmark of adolescent schizophrenia.

Introduction

During Kraepelin’s time, people began to realize that schizophrenia could be found in children.Citation1 Schizophrenia can be divided into the following categories by the age of onset: adult schizophrenia (AS: onset aged >18 years), adolescent onset schizophrenia (AOS: onset aged 13–18 years), and childhood onset schizophrenia (onset aged <13 years).Citation2Citation4 Compared with AS, the following characteristics are observed in AOS: more neurodevelopmental impairments,Citation5 more negative symptoms,Citation6 and greater frequency of cerebral abnormalities and a longer treatment requirement.Citation7,Citation8 The development of AOS often diminishes social networks and results in difficulties in social functioning.Citation9 Although impaired social behavior in schizophrenia has been repeatedly described,Citation9 the scientific literature on the social cognition of AOS and childhood onset schizophrenia is limited.

Theory of Mind (ToM) damage in AOS may be related to poor social functioning.Citation7,Citation8 ToM is defined as an individual’s ability to understand others’ wishes, beliefs, intentions, and other psychological states and to judge others’ behavior accordingly, which is an essential component of social cognition.Citation9 ToM was divided into cognitive ToM (Cog ToM) and affective ToM (Aff ToM) in the different cognitive processes,Citation10 and another important classification is first and second order in the ToM complexity. Cog ToM is used to infer one’s own and others’ beliefs, intentions, and desires; whereas, affective ToM is used to infer one’s own and others’ emotional states and feelings. Shamay-Tsoory et al proposed a model in which Cog ToM was a precondition for Aff ToM and Aff ToM processes required both Cog ToM and empathy.Citation11 Whereas first-order-ToM is defined as the ability of an individual to understand another person’s beliefs, the ability to realize that someone else thinks that a third person believes something is called the “second order” ToM.Citation12 Several tasks have been developed to assess the above ToM subcomponents, such as the false belief tasks,Citation13 the Yoni task,Citation11 the Faux Pas Recognition (FPR) test,Citation14 the eye recognition task, and the perspective-taking task.Citation15

Adolescence is characterized as a critical period in the development of ToM.Citation16 However, initial behavioral evidence showed that Cog ToM and Aff ToM had different developmental trajectories.Citation17,Citation18 Cog ToM always develops earlier than Aff ToM.Citation18 Previous research showed that children could pass some simple Cog ToM tasks (eg, false belief) from the age of 4 to 6 years and gained an understanding of the more complex cognitive tasks at a later age (eg, second belief) between the age of 7 and 9 years.Citation13 Developmental findings on Aff ToM processing observed a later developmental trajectory in which Aff ToM began to develop between 7 and 9 years of age and that children between 9 and 11 years could infer complex affective expression (eg, FPR task).Citation17

Whether ToM deficits represent a distinctive trait marker or state variables for schizophrenia is debatable.Citation19 Several studies found that ToM was related to symptoms, especially in patients in remission who performed significantly better on ToM tests than those in symptomatic periods,Citation20 but impairment across ToM domains has been well documented in chronic and first-episode patients, suggesting that ToM impairment is a trait marker in schizophrenia patients.Citation21 Additionally, one study reported that schizophrenia showed ToM damage in remission,Citation22 first-degree relatives of patients, and high-risk patients and thus might qualify as a stable endophenotype of schizophrenia.Citation21 The current study explores whether ToM impairment is a trait phenomenon in the perspective of AOS.

The present study examines the performance of two ToM components (Cog ToM and Aff ToM) in AOS using the Yoni task and FPR task. The Yoni task is a classic measure to verify the cognitive vs affective aspects of the ToM and to classify first vs second order; its simplicity makes it accessible to children and adolescents. The FPR task is a widely used ToM task to analyze higher-level ToM ability and to investigate Aff and Cog ToM. No studies, to date, have utilized these two tasks in AOS; therefore, we use these two paradigms to explore the defining characteristics of Aff and Cog ToM in AOS.

Considering the effects of the mental disease and the specificity of the age of onset in AOS, the current study aims to explore the following questions: 1) whether ToM deficits exist in late AOS compared with healthy adolescent controls and 2) whether the deficits are related to symptoms and disease duration. We hypothesized that patients with late AOS would perform less well in ToM performance, compared with health controls. We also hypothesized that there was no association between ToM performance and the illness course and symptoms.

Methods

Subjects

Thirty-five adolescents with schizophrenia and 35 demographically similar healthy volunteers participated in this study. All of the subjects were Chinese patients aged 13–18 years and were consecutively recruited between February 2013 and March 2015 at the Mental Health Center of Anhui Province. The patients were recruited from outpatient clinics for confirmatory diagnostic evaluations by two psychiatrists, and they met the 10th edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. All of the patients were stable in their symptoms and had received treatment for at least 4 months. All patients were stable on fixed doses of medication (). In most cases, the psychological and psychiatric assessments were evaluated in the same day. The psychological assessments were completed by a graduate student in clinical psychology after training, and psychiatric assessments were assessed by experienced psychiatrists. Healthy adolescents were recruited from local junior and senior high schools and were age-, gender-, and intelligence-matched. The exclusion criteria for all subjects were a history of drug and alcohol abuse, head trauma, seizure disorder, other major neurological disorder, mental retardation or a score on the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) of >7.Citation23 All participants were right-handed.

Table 1 Differences between the groups in terms of demographics, clinical, and neuropsychological performance and medication

All of the subjects and their parents provided assent/consent to participate. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University.

Measurements

Clinical and neuropsychological background assessments

AOS was evaluated with the 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HARS),Citation24 the 17-item HDRS, and the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) by a trained psychiatrist.Citation23,Citation25 The PANSS was used to evaluate anxiety, depression, and the presence/absence or severity of the positive, negative, and general psychopathology of schizophrenia. The HARS and HDRS were used to assess the anxiety and depression of the patients. All patients received one type of atypical antipsychotics (Aripiprazole, Clozapine, Risperidone, Quetiapine), and the medication dosages have been transformed into chlorpromazine equivalents.Citation26 Additionally, the subjects’ intelligence quotients (IQs) were assessed using the Standard Progressive Matrices of Raven. The interference task of the Stroop Color–Word Test (SCWT) was used to assess the subjects’ response inhibition and interference susceptibility. The score included three parts as follows: part A (word condition), part B (color condition), and part C (word-color interference condition). Part C mainly assesses executive function. In the Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) they had to separately name six classes of words (animals, vegetables, appliances, fruit, words beginning with water, words beginning with self), as many as possible, within 1 min, then the total number of the subject responses was calculated.Citation27 VFT assessed frontal lobe function, Digit Span Test assessed working memory and short-term memory, which has forward and backward parts. The total score was recorded.

Yoni task

The Chinese version of this task (programmed using E-prime) consisted of 96 trials based on a task as previously described. Each trial had a cartoon outline of a face (named Xiao Ming) and four colored pictures, each of which was located in one corner of the computer screen. The pictures were of objects (eg, fruit or chairs) or faces. Based on the cues available (such as Xiao Ming’s eye gaze and facial expression) at the top of the screen, the subject’s task was to point to the correct answer (according to the cartoon to which Xiao Ming was referring). After a participant found the correct answer, he or she was asked to respond (click) quickly. These trials fall into the following three classes: cognitive, affective and physical. The “affective” trials investigate “hot” ToM, the “cognitive” trials inspect “cold” ToM, and the physical trials act as the control condition to explore whether the subject understood the questions. All of the trials were further subdivided into first and second order inferences. The addition would have no influence on the difficulty and framework of the task, which were basically the same as the original. The duration of the task was approximately 12 minutes, and E-prime recorded the participant’s correct numbers and reaction times. The individual received one point for each correct answer. The task consisted of three parts:

  • Cognitive condition

    The first-order-cognitive-condition, 12 trials (on a scale from 0 to 12; translated from Chinese)

     Cog1: Xiao Ming is thinking of ______?

    The second order cognitive condition, 12 trials (on a scale from 0 to 12; translated from Chinese)

     Cog2: Xiao Ming is thinking of the toy that _____ wants?

  • Affective condition

    The first-order-affective-condition, 12 trials (on a scale from 0 to 12; translated from Chinese)

     Aff1: Xiao Ming likes_____?

    The second order affective condition, 42 trials (on a scale from 0 to 42; translated from Chinese)

     Aff2: Xiao Ming likes animals that______likes?

  • Physical condition

    The first-order-physical-condition, 8 trials (on a scale from 0 to 8; translated from Chinese)

     Phy1: Xiao Ming is near to______?

    The second-order-physical-condition, 12 trials (on a scale from 0 to 12; translated from Chinese)

     Phy2: Xiao Ming has the same fruit that____has?

In our view, the second order Aff ToM is a complex item and thus the 16 questions may not fully reflect subjects’ abilities in the second order Aff ToM. Therefore, we added 30 questions to the second order Aff ToM condition (eg, original question: Xiao Ming likes animals that______likes?; added question: Xiao Ming likes toys that______likes?).

FPR test

The Baron-Cohen’s FPR test was applied in the Chinese context. The Chinese version of the FPR test has good test-retest reliability and interrater reliability.Citation28 A faux pas is considered to occur if a person says something unintentionally that should not have been said and the remark makes people feel sad and embarrassed. The following is an example of a social faux pas story in this task:

Liu yan’s mother was having a surprise party for Liu yan’s birthday. She invited Li mei and said, “Don’t tell anyone, especially Liu yan”. The day before the party, Li mei and Liu yan were playing together and Li mei ripped her new dress. “Oh!” said Li mei, “I was going to wear this to your party.” “What party?” said Liu yan. “Come on”, said Li mei, “let’s go and see if my mum can mend the tear.”

If a subject discovered that there was a faux pas, he or she would realize that the speaker was going to make the listener uncomfortable and predict that those who heard the words would be sad. There were 10 faux pas stories in our study. After each story, five questions were answered: • Question 1 (Cog ToM: detecting the faux pas): Did someone say something he (or she) should not have said?

  • Question 2 (Aff ToM: detecting the faux pas): Who said something he (or she) should not have said?

  • Question 3 (Aff ToM: understanding the listener’s mental state): Why should he (or she) not have said it?

  • Question 4 (Aff ToM: understanding the speaker’s mental state): Why did he (or she) say it?

  • Question 5 (control condition): Who was the surprise party for?

For scoring, each correct response was given 1 point. If the subject answered “No” to the first question, questions 2 to 4 would be skipped and he (or she) would directly answer question 5. In this a case, questions 1 to 4 were given zero points. The total of “detecting faux pas” (Cog ToM scores) was the sum of the scores for each question 1 of the 10 stories on a scale from 0 to 10. The total of “understanding (Aff ToM scores)” was the sum of the scores for questions 2 to 4 of the 10 stories on a scale from 0 to 30. The total score for questions 1 to 4 was regarded as the faux pas total. Question 5 was a control condition to examine whether the subjects understood the story and was scored on a scale from 0 to 10.

Statistical analysis

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 19.0 was used for the statistical analysis. Independent sample t-tests were used to examine the differences in independent variables (age, education, duration of illness, subcomponent scores, and clinical and neuropsychological background assessments). To assess performance in the ToM tasks, a repeated-measures ANOVA was respectively performed on the first and second-order-ToM, ToM components (three components: cognitive, affective and control subcomponents) acted as within-subject factors and the groups (AOS and healthy controls [HC] group) acted as between-subject factors. The data were used to detect the significance of the main effects (groups and ToM components) and the interaction effect between the groups and ToM components. Then, multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed on the group difference (AOS and HC group) for each component (Cog ToM, Aff ToM, and physical condition). MANOVA was conducted on Faux Pas Recognition Test, which was used to examine the group differences in the components. We also used Pearson’s correlation analysis to examine the correlation between the severity of symptoms, duration of illness, SCWT performance, and ToM subcomponents. The threshold of statistical significance was set at P<0.05.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics and neuropsychological results

The background and neuropsychological data of AOS vs the healthy controls are shown in . The results of the independent-sample t-test indicated that there was no significant difference in terms of age (P=0.59), years of education (P=0.10), HARS (P=0.78) and HDRS scores (P=0.43), and IQ-Raven (P=0.72) between the groups. A significant difference was observed between the two groups in the SCWT test (SCWT test A: P<0.001; SCWT test B: P<0.001; and SCWT test C: P=0.01). The two groups showed no significant difference in the scores of the VFT (P=0.94), digit span forward (P=0.16) and Digital span backward (P=0.14) tests ().

Yoni task

First-order-ToM

A repeated measures ANOVA was performed on the first-order-ToM using each of the ToM subcomponents (three subcomponents: cognitive, affective, and physical) as the within-subjects factor and the group (EOS group and HC group) as the between-subjects factor. The results demonstrated a primary effect for the ToM condition (F[1.68] =70.103, P<0.001, η2=0.508), and the group-by-condition interaction (F[1,68] =6.408, P=0.014, η2=0.086) reached statistical significance. A multivariate analysis with group as a fixed factor and first physical condition as a covariate was performed to examine the relationship between first cognitive condition and first affective condition. The EOS group had significantly lower scores in first order Cog ToM (F[1,68] =4.031, P=0.049, η2=0.057) and first order Aff ToM (F[1,68] =6.809, P=0.011, η2=0.092) ().

Figure 1 The three parts of z scores in Yoni task.

Notes: Mean score of Cog ToM, Aff ToM and Phy Con for subjects with AOS and HC in Yoni task. The error bars: SD; *P<0.05 for differences between the two groups. ***P<0.001.
Abbreviations: Aff, affective; AOS, adolescent onset schizophrenia; Cog, cognitive; HCs, healthy controls; Phy Con, physical condition; ToM, Theory of Mind.
Figure 1 The three parts of z scores in Yoni task.

Second-order-ToM

The repeated measures ANOVA on the second-order-ToM was performed for the within-subjects and between-subjects factors of the second-order-ToM as described earlier. There were significant main effects for both the ToM conditions (F[1,68] =5.222, P=0.025, η2=0.071) and the group (F[1,68] =14.772, P<0.001, η2=0.178). There was also a significant effect for the condition by group interaction (F[1,68] =16.285, P<0.001, η2=0.193). A multivariate analysis with the group as a fixed factor and the second physical ToM condition as a covariate was performed to examine the relationship between the second Cog ToM condition and second Aff ToM condition. The EOS group had significantly lower scores in the second order Cog ToM (F[1,68] =16.737, P<0.001, η2=0.200) and second order Aff ToM (F[1,68] =13.631, P<0.001, η2=0.169) ().

FPR task

Almost all participants answered the control questions of the Faux Pas Recognition Test correctly (F[1,68] =3.058, P=0.085, η2=0.043). The MANOVA found significant group differences in recognition of faux pas (F[1,68] =4.395, P=0.041, η2=0.060) and understanding of faux pas (F[1,68] =7.929, P=0.006, η2=0.104) (, ).

Figure 2 The sub-tests of z scores in FPR.

Notes: Mean score of Cog ToM, Aff ToM and Phy Con for subjects with AOS and HC in FPR. The error bars: SD; ***P<0.001.
Abbreviations: Aff, affective; AOS, adolescent onset schizophrenia; Cog, cognitive; FPR, Faux Pas Recognition; HCs, healthy controls; Phy Con, physical condition; SD, standard deviation; ToM, Theory of Mind.
Figure 2 The sub-tests of z scores in FPR.

Table 2 Performances of AOS, control group in the Yoni task and Faux Pas Recognition Test

Correlation coefficients among multi-factors or patient groups

presents the correlation of the illness course, symptoms, executive function, and chlorpromazine equivalents with the scores of the Cog and Aff ToMs. There was no significant association between the symptom clusters, illness duration, chlorpromazine equivalents, or ToM scores. There was a significant association between SCWT-C performance and second-order-ToM score ().

Table 3 Correlation coefficients between symptom, illness duration, EF, CPZ equivalents and results of ToM for AOS

Discussion

This study investigated the ToM performance in AOS using the Yoni task and FRP test compared with HC group, and examined whether the ToM scores were related to the course of the disease and the PANSS scores. Results indicated that patients with AOS demonstrated impairment to different extents of the Cog and Aff ToM compared with age-, gender, and intelligence-matched healthy controls. In the Yoni task, patients with AOS showed differences in ToM abilities either on a different order or under different conditions. It seemed that impairment in second-order-ToM is more serious. The FPR task results revealed that AOS patients were not always able to recognize a faux pas or understand complicated emotions under the faux pas scenario. Furthermore, as indicated by the correlation analysis, neither Cog ToM nor Aff ToM was related to the patients’ symptoms, disease duration, dose of medication, and IQ.

With regard to previous ToM studies in schizophrenia patients, our results are in concordance with the meta-analysis by Bora et al that found that individuals with schizophrenia seemed to perform poorly on both Cog and Aff ToM tasks.Citation19 However, Shamay-Tsoory et al used the same Yoni task and found the Aff ToM component was damaged but that the Cog ToM was intact in AS patients.Citation29 Thus, Shamay-Tsoory et al proposed that Aff ToM impairment instead of complete ToM impairment in schizophrenia was a core characteristic. Based on the results in our study, it may not be plausible to conclude that Aff ToM impairment is a core characteristic of schizophrenia, at least not for late AOS.

The present study found a lower significance level in first order Cog ToM impairment (P=0.049) but a higher significance level in second order Cog ToM impairment between AOS and controls, demonstrating that second order Cog ToM showed more severe damage in AOS patients. Some previous studies found AS patients with relatively intact Cog ToM, but the current results showed AOS with higher order Cog ToM damaged, implying that earlier onset schizophrenia was associated with more severe ToM damage. This finding may be a major reason why AOS patients have poor outcomes.Citation30

The meta-analysis data supported the hypothesis that ToM was subserved by a network of regions that included the MPFC, temporo-parietal junction, precuneus, and superior temporal sulcus.Citation31 These regions played distinct roles during ToM. The vmPFC is considered to be closely related to Aff ToM, whereas the dlPFC is believed to be more related to Cog ToM.Citation32,Citation33 Importantly, an elaborated ToM continues beyond the beginning of puberty until early adulthood.Citation34 For instance, the MPFC develops during late childhood and early adolescence. More developmental changes in neural activities were observed in the vmPFC in adolescents.Citation35 The non-linear changes in vmPFC activities that occur between 10 and 12 and 14 and 16 years of age suggest that the vmPFC develops unsteadily in adolescents around the onset of puberty.Citation36 The adolescent brain is still undergoing several maturational processes, which makes it more vulnerable to some of the effects of the disease. Therefore, schizophrenia onset during this period may lead to a disruption in neurodevelopment, which may lead to a worse effect in adolescents.

Pearson correlations of the ToM (FPR and Yoni tasks) subscores with PANSS and the course of disease, the Stroop–Color Word Test (SWCT)-C performance, IQ, and dosage of drugs in AOS group are reported in . ToM deficits are independent of the course and severity of disease, and the results were in agreement with the study by Bora et al, which showed mental impairment in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and their unaffected siblings.Citation37 Another study demonstrated that deficits in social cognition existed in the prodromal stage of schizophrenia and that ToM deficits in schizophrenia might precede the initial stage of psychotic breakdown, thereby accelerating disease progression.Citation38 Therefore, the present results seemed to be more plausible to infer that ToM damage in schizophrenia may be a trait phenomenon, but not a state one, together with insignificance of the relativity between ToM performance and dosage of drugs, our results added the evidence that ToM damage may be an intermediate phenotype of schizophrenia.Citation38 Although the AOS and HC groups were matched for age, gender, IQ, and education level, the AOS group had significantly poorer executive performance (eg, SWCT). These behavioral findings are corroborated by emerging evidence that high-order Cog ToM involves executive functions.Citation39 Our results on the significant relativity between SCWT-C performance and second-order-ToM were indeed in accordance with the above findings, whereas ToM is not simply a more general executive function.Citation39 Indeed, ToM performance requires more processes than executive functions.

Limitations

The limitations of the study should be considered. Our subjects were young and newly exposed to this chronic disease, and AOS had a shorter disease course (approximately 2–3 years). The relationship between disease progression and the severity of the ToM injury is uncertain. More AOS patients with longer courses should be recruited in future studies. Although our study strictly controlled for the level of intelligence, gender, education, background and type and dose of drugs, we should not ignore the influence of medications. Medications may have variable effects on shaping brain activities and performance, and various doses may also affect basic social cognitive functions. Such small sample sizes of the AOS may not be representative of AOS as a whole. In addition, the current work does not directly compare the ability of ToM in AOS with AS so as to identify the special property in AOS. Adolescence can be divided into three stages (the early, middle, and late stages), with the brain maturation processes continuing throughout the three stages. In our study, we chose only subjects in late adolescence. We will continue our study of the disease impacts on the ToM in the two other stages of adolescence.

Acknowledgments

This study is sponsored by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (nos 31571149, 91432301, 81300944, 81301176, and 91232717) and Natural Science Foundation of Anhui Province (1308085QH148). We are deeply grateful to all the patients and healthy controls who participated in this research.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • EisenbergLThe course of childhood schizophreniaAMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry1957781698313434528
  • ArmenterosJLDaviesMAntipsychotics in early onset schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysisEur Child Adolesc Psychiatry200615314114816470340
  • YoungCMFindlingRLPharmacologic treatment of adolescent and child schizophreniaExpert Rev Neurother200441536015853615
  • SchulzSCFindlingRLWiseAFriedmanLKennyJChild and adolescent schizophreniaPsychiatr Clin North Am199821143569551490
  • HansSLAuerbachJGAsarnowJRStyrBMarcusJSocial adjustment of adolescents at risk for schizophrenia: the Jerusalem Infant Development StudyJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry200039111406141411068896
  • BassoMRNasrallahHAOlsonSCBornsteinRACognitive deficits distinguish patients with adolescent- and adult-onset schizophreniaNeuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol19971021071129150511
  • RohdeGKleinWArinirUAssociation of the ASP299GLY TLR4 polymorphism with COPDRespir Med2006100589289616219455
  • Ping-ChiaLI-JuLYu-ChingLLi-ChingCWen-ChungCSubstance P scavenger enhances antioxidant defenses and prevents prothrombotic effects on the rat lung after acute exposure to oil smokeJ Biomed Sci2009165819575822
  • FettAKViechtbauerWDominguezMDPennDLvan OsJKrabbendamLThe relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysisNeurosci Biobehav Rev201135357358820620163
  • BrothersLRingBA neuroethological framework for the representation of mindsJ Cogn Neurosci19924210711823967887
  • Shamay-TsoorySGHarariHAharon-PeretzJLevkovitzYThe role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendenciesCortex201046566867719501818
  • HoKKLuiSSHungKSTheory of mind impairments in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and their unaffected siblingsSchizophr Res20151661–31826049215
  • PernerJWimmerH“John thinks that Mary thinks that …” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old childrenJ Exp Child Psychol1985393437471
  • Baron-CohenSO’RiordanMStoneVJonesRPlaistedKRecognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autismJ Autism Dev Disord199929540741810587887
  • VogeleyKBussfeldPNewenAMind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspectiveNeuroimage2001141 Pt 117018111525326
  • SteinbergLCognitive and affective development in adolescenceTrends Cogn Sci200592697415668099
  • RuffmanTKeenanTRThe belief-based emotion of surprise: the case for a lag in understanding relative to false beliefDev Psychol19963214049
  • RieffeCTerwogtMMCowanRChildren’s understanding of mental states as causes of emotionsInfant Child Dev2005143259272
  • BoraEYucelMPantelisCTheory of mind impairment: a distinct trait-marker for schizophrenia spectrum disorders and bipolar disorder?Acta Psychiatr Scand2009120425326419489747
  • CorcoranRMercerGFrithCDSchizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophreniaSchizophr Res19951715138541250
  • GreenMFBeardenCECannonTDSocial cognition in schizophrenia, Part 1: performance across phase of illnessSchizophr Bull201238485486421345917
  • GilDFernandez-ModamioMBengocheaRArrietaMAdaptation of the Hinting Task theory of the mind test to SpanishRev Psiquiatr Salud Ment201252798822854578
  • HamiltonMA rating scale for depressionJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry196023566214399272
  • HamiltonMThe assessment of anxiety states by ratingBr J Med Psychol1959321505513638508
  • KaySRFiszbeinAOplerLAThe Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophreniaSchizophr Bull19871322612763616518
  • GardnerDMMurphyALO’DonnellHCentorrinoFBaldessariniRJInternational consensus study of antipsychotic dosingAm J Psychiatry2010167668669320360319
  • TroyerAKMoscovitchMWinocurGClustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adultsNeuropsychology19971111381469055277
  • ZhuCYLeeTMLiXSJingSCWangYGWangKImpairments of social cues recognition and social functioning in Chinese people with schizophreniaPsychiatry Clin Neurosci200761214915817362432
  • Shamay-TsoorySGAharon-PeretzJPerryDTwo systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesionsBrain2009132Pt 361762718971202
  • BruneM“Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: a review of the literatureSchizophr Bull2005311214215888423
  • Dodell-FederDKoster-HaleJBednyMSaxeRfMRI item analysis in a theory of mind taskNeuroimage201155270571221182967
  • Shamay-TsoorySGTibi-ElhananyYAharon-PeretzJThe ventromedial prefrontal cortex is involved in understanding affective but not cognitive theory of mind storiesSoc Neurosci200613–414916618633784
  • BurnettSSebastianCCohen KadoshKBlakemoreSJThe social brain in adolescence: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studiesNeurosci Biobehav Rev20113581654166421036192
  • VetterNCLeipoldKKliegelMPhillipsLHAltgassenMOngoing development of social cognition in adolescenceChild Neuropsychol201319661562922934659
  • VetterNCWeigeltSDohnelKSmolkaMNKliegelMOngoing neural development of affective theory of mind in adolescenceSoc Cogn Affect Neurosci2014971022102923716712
  • MoorBGMacksZAGurogluBRomboutsSAMolenMWCroneEANeurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyesSoc Cogn Affect Neurosci201271445221515640
  • BoraEYucelMPantelisCTheory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysisSchizophr Res20091091–31919195844
  • ChungYSKangDHShinNYYooSYKwonJSDeficit of theory of mind in individuals at ultra-high-risk for schizophreniaSchizophr Res2008991–311111818096371
  • Fernandez-GonzaloSPousaEJodarMTuronMDunoRPalaoDInfluence of the neuropsychological functions in theory of mind in schizophrenia: the false belief/deception paradigmJ Nerv Ment Dis2013201760961323787482