205
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Relationship between hypnosis and personality trait in participants with high or low hypnotic susceptibility

, , , , , , , & show all
Pages 1007-1012 | Published online: 03 Apr 2017

Abstract

Background

The relationship between normal personality and hypnotic susceptibility is important for understanding mental processing and mental disorders, but it is less consistent in normal people or in patients with a psychiatric disorder. We have hypothesized that the correlation exists but varies in individuals with different levels of hypnotizability.

Participants and methods

We invited 72 individuals with high (HIGH group) and 47 individuals with low (LOW group) hypnotic susceptibilities to undertake tests of NEO-PI-R and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C (SHSSC).

Results

The HIGH group scored significantly higher than the LOW group did on openness to experience and its facet openness to feelings. In the LOW group, SHSSC total was positively predicted by openness to ideas; age regression was positively predicted by openness to experience and negatively predicted by extraversion; anosmia to ammonia was negatively predicted by agreeableness; and negative visual hallucination was positively predicted by openness to experience. In the HIGH group, hallucinated voice was positively predicted by openness to experience and negatively predicted by agreeableness, and posthypnotic amnesia was positively predicted by extraversion and negatively predicted by openness to experience.

Conclusion

The associations between normal personality traits and hypnotic susceptibility items were weak and different in the two groups, which imply that managing mental or somatoform disorders might be through adjusting hypnotizability and mobilizing personality functions.

Introduction

Hypnotic susceptibility is an ability of responsiveness to suggestions for changes in subjective experience and for alterations in perception, sensation, emotion, thought, or behavior.Citation1Citation3 The ability shows hereditary inclination and remains stable during the lifetime of an individual.Citation4Citation7 As a significant index reflecting the degree of hypnotic suggestions and inducement, it is purported to correlate with personality traits. In patients with a personality disorder, for example, the hypnotic features such as taste hallucination and anosmia to ammonia were correlated with the borderline personality disorder functioning style, and posthypnotic amnesia was correlated positively with the schizoid and negatively with the narcissistic styles.Citation8

Some normal personality traits were linked with hypnotic susceptibility, but their relationships were not always stable in healthy people.Citation9Citation11 The discrepancy might be resulted from various measurements of normal personality and hypnotic susceptibility. For instance, with the application of Intelligence Structure Test 2000 RCitation12 and the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A (HGSHSA),Citation13 gender factor was reported as a moderator in the relationship between hypnotic susceptibility and intelligence.Citation14

However, even with the same personality measurement, results are not consistent with each other. For example, using the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory,Citation15 one study indicated that people with high hypnotic susceptibility scored higher in depression, masculinity–femininity, and schizophrenia scales than people with low hypnotic susceptibility did,Citation16 but another study failed to replicate the results.Citation17 Nevertheless, more recent studies kept showing new evidence that persistence,Citation18 emotional contagion,Citation19 and unselfish/self-sacrificing,Citation20 were linked with hypnotic susceptibility.

In addition, using HGSHSA and the NEO Personality Inventory’s openness to experience,Citation21 Glisky et alCitation22 have found that hypnotizability was weakly correlated with openness to experience domain and with its facets openness to fantasy, openness to esthetics, and openness to feelings. Similarly, Nordenstrom et al,Citation23 using Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion ScaleCitation24 and Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form CCitation25 in two phases, reported weak correlations between openness to experience and hypnotic suggestibility. By contrast, Green,Citation26 using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R), HGSHSA, and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C (SHSSC),Citation27 obtained significant zero-order correlations in extraversion and agreeableness scores but failed to detect any association with openness to experience. Malinoski and LynnCitation28 also failed to do so with similar methods.

The inconformity of previous studies on the relationships between personality traits and hypnotic susceptibility might be due to insufficient numbers of participants, or that their participants were not highly hypnotizable. Indeed, healthy people displaying high suggestibility are grouped into a dissocial subtype with deficits in executive functioning and a predisposition to psychopathology and a subtype with superior imaginary and no observable deficits in functioning.Citation29 Furthermore, we believe that the unclear relationship between hypnosis and personality might be resulted from using different instruments to measure the hypnotic susceptibility and personality traits, or from not administering the hypnotic measures in participants with high or low hypnotic susceptibility extremes. These earlier inconsistent results prompted us to further explore the detailed personality traits in high and low hypnotizable people. Thus, this study was designed to explore the relationships between personality traits and hypnotic susceptibility in participants with either high hypnotizability or low hypnotizability. We used the NEO-PI-R because it is an extensively used measure for normal personality traits and correlates with personality disorder functioning styles,Citation30Citation32 and we used SHSSC because it is one of the widely used measures of the hypnotic susceptibility.Citation33

Participants and methods

Participants

The first three SHSSC items, ie, the hand lowering, moving hands apart, and mosquito hallucination, were used as screening for participants; those who either had passed two items (high susceptibility) or had passed only one item or failed to pass any item (low susceptibility) would be enrolled into the current study. Altogether, 340 participants from university or community were invited, but only 121 participants succeeded to be enrolled; 74 (42 women; mean age, 21.09 years with 1.63 SD; age range, 18–26 years) were classified into the low susceptibility (LOW) group, and 47 (28 women; mean age, 20.70±1.69; age range, 18–26 years) into the high susceptibility (HIGH) group. There was no age (F[1, 119]=0.24, P=0.63, mean square effect [MSE] =0.31) or gender (χ2[1, 119]=0.094, P=0.76) difference between the two groups. All participants were free from somatic or psychiatric illnesses and were requested to refrain from consuming any drugs or alcohol for at least 72 hours prior to the test. The study was approved by the ethics committee of Zhejiang University College of Medicine, and all participants gave their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Measures

The participants were asked to undergo the SHSSC test and to complete the NEO-PI-R in two quiet rooms.

SHSSC

The Chinese version of the SHSSC was translated from (and back-translated to) the original English version by a professor and two PhD candidates majoring in Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, and it has been used in a previous study.Citation8 The SHSSC consists of 12 items including direct ideomotor (eg, arm heaviness), challenge motor (eg, arm immobilization), and cognitive-perceptual (eg, auditory hallucination) suggestions, with scores ranging from 0 to 12. The internal alpha (reliability) of SHSSC was 0.72 in this study.

If a participant got one point on an item, we referred to this as “s/he has passed the item”, otherwise as “s/he has failed to pass the item”. We defined “passing rate” of each item as the percentage of the participants who had passed the item.

NEO-PI-R

The NEO-PI-R is a 240-item questionnaire measuring normal adult personality across the following five basic domains: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. Six facet scales comprise each of the five domain scales and represent important constructs within each domain. The translation and psychometric properties of the Chinese language version are described elsewhere.Citation34 The internal alphas (reliabilities) of the five scales were 0.91, 0.87, 0.77, 0.80, and 0.88 in this study.

Statistical analyses

Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and the post hoc independent Student’s t-test were applied to the five NEO-PI-R scales in the LOW and HIGH groups. If two groups scored significantly on a domain, further analyses would be conducted on its facets. Chi-square test was used to evaluate the differences of SHSSC item passing rates in two groups. The multiple linear regression analysis (step-wise method) was used to explore the relationships between SHSSC and NEO-PI-R scales in a given group.

Results

The mean NEO-PI-R scale scores were significantly different between the two groups (group effect, F[1, 119]=10.32, P=0.00, MSE =2,791.43; scale effect, F[4, 116]=52.84, P=0.00, MSE =16,716.77; group × scale interaction effect, F[4, 116]=0.61, P=0.66, MSE =192.06). Participants in the HIGH group scored significantly higher than those in the LOW group on openness to experience (P=0.007, 95% confidence interval [CI] =[−12.08, −1.99]) and its facet openness to feelings (P=0.001, 95% CI =[−3.11, −0.79]) (). The total SHSSC score was 9.49 (with 0.66 SD) in the HIGH group and 2.08 (1.00) in the LOW group; obviously, comparing the passing rates of SHSSC items, the HIGH group passed significantly more often than did the LOW group ().

Table 1 NEO-PI-R scale scores (mean ± SD) in two groups of participants

Table 2 Distribution of participants who passed the hypnotic susceptibility tests in two groups of participants

In the LOW group, the total SHSSC score was positively predicted by openness to ideas (adjusted R2=0.157, β=0.086, standard error [SE] =0.031, P=0.008, 95% CI =[0.02, 0.15]). When referring to individual SHSSC item, age regression score was positively predicted by openness to experience (β=0.062, odds ratio [OR] =1.064, SE =0.024, P=0.018, 95% CI =[1.02, 1.12]) and negatively predicted by extraversion (β=−0.036, OR =0.97, SE =0.024, P=0.045, 95% CI =[0.93, 1.00]). Anosmia to ammonia was negatively predicted by agreeableness (β=−0.074, OR =0.93, SE =0.036, P=0.038, 95% CI =[0.87, 1.00]). Negative visual hallucination score was positively predicted by openness to experience (β=0.138, OR =1.15, SE =0.065, P=0.033, 95% CI =[1.01, 1.30]).

In the HIGH group, there was no association between the SHSSC total and NEO-PI-R scale scores. By contrast, the SHSSC item hallucinated voice score was positively predicted by openness to experience (β=0.112, OR =1.12, SE =0.044, P=0.011, 95% CI =[1.03, 1.22]) and negatively predicted by agreeableness (β=−0.086, OR =0.92, SE =0.037, P=0.021, 95% CI =[0.85, 0.99]). Posthypnotic amnesia score of SHSSC was positively predicted by extraversion (β=0.008, OR =1.13, SE =0.045, P=0.008, 95% CI =[1.03, 1.23]), and negatively predicted by openness to experience (β=−0.138, OR =0.87, SE =0.052, P=0.008, 95% CI =[0.79, 0.96]).

Discussion

Compared to the LOW group, the HIGH group scored higher on NEO-PI-R openness to experience and its facet openness to feelings. The total SHSSC score was obviously lower in the LOW group, but it was positively associated with a trait facet openness to ideas in the group, which was roughly comparable with the previous reports.Citation22,Citation23 The traits agreeableness, extraversion, and openness to experience were associated with SHSSC items, but the associations were weak and their patterns were different between the two groups.

The HIGH group scored higher on openness to experience, which also accords with previous results.Citation22,Citation23 The trait of openness to experience covers fantasy,Citation35 and highly suggestible individuals were fantasy-prone respondents as demonstrated previously.Citation36 The high suggestibility enables a participant to reduce their awareness of exogenous stimuli including the auditory information,Citation37 and the abovementioned fantasy-prone and the less filtered auditory intrusion help to explain the association between hallucinated voice and openness to experience in this group. Negative visual hallucination is similar to illusion, and hallucinations of several sensory modalities are believed to be illusions of reality in normal people.Citation38 This notion helps to explain the association between negative visual hallucination and openness to experience found in the LOW group.

Openness to feelings implies receptivity to one’s own inner feelings and emotions and the evaluation of the emotion as an important part of life.Citation39 High hypnotic susceptibility individuals, who tend to experience more hypnotic pain or emotional disturbances,Citation40,Citation41 might be more sensitive and acceptable to their inner hypnotic feelings. Another studyCitation42 has demonstrated that the openness to feelings which shared a common conceptual ground with empathy, a state of unconscious emotional influence of the mind upon body, was also correlated with the hypnotizability.

It has been shown that the posthypnotic amnesia works when memory is consciously suppressed and dissociated from awareness.Citation43 People with high openness to experience are willing to entertain novel ideas.Citation39 The hypnotic experience seems fresh and novel for them; therefore, these people might pay more attention to the verbal or behavioral suggestions given by the experimenter during the hypnotic process. Moreover, when asked to recall the details of the experiment, highly hypnotizable people are prone to do better. These narrations in part help to explain the negative association of openness to experience with posthypnotic amnesia in the HIGH group and positive association with age regression in the LOW group. By contrast to openness to experience, extraversion was positively associated with posthypnotic amnesia in the HIGH group but negatively associated with age regression in the LOW group. Evidence has shown that the extraverts, being characterized as sociable, assertive, active, and talkative,Citation39 are less able to focus attention on a specific task and more easily distracted by external stimuli.Citation44 Hence extraverts incline to pay less attention to the details of the hypnotic process and cannot concentrate on the whole course of the hypnotic experiment. In contrast, hypnosis requires intensive concentration on the elaborate hypnotic suggestions, so that people would be enrolled into a hypnotic trance state. Thus, extraverts would not bear particularly detailed matters of the hypnotic experiment in mind and get higher passing rates on posthypnotic amnesia. These descriptions, also in part, help to explain why extraversion was positively associated with posthypnotic amnesia in the HIGH group and negatively associated with age regression in the LOW group. However, at present, there is no plausible explanation why openness to experience and extraversion functioned differently to the two different hypnotic aspects in different groups. Nevertheless, previous results have shown that personality disorder patients passed significantly higher on posthypnotic amnesia, and the schizoid personality disorder functioning style was positively associated with the posthypnotic amnesia and the narcissistic was negatively associated with the posthypnotic amnesia.Citation8 Other studies have shown that the schizoid personality disorder is negatively correlated with openness to experienceCitation45 and the narcissistic is negatively correlated with extraversion.Citation30

The agreeableness characterizes individuals as fundamentally altruistic, sympathetic, and helpful to others,Citation39 and it is negatively associated with paranoid personality disorder.Citation46 It is then easier to understand that agreeableness was negatively associated with hallucinated voice in the HIGH group. The agreeableness also ascribes individuals as amiable and peaceful to aversive situations,Citation39 and the trait might help to elevate the threshold of ammonia provoking, thus assigning its negative association with anosmia to ammonia in the LOW group. Once more, at present, there is no plausible explanation why agreeableness was associated with two different hypnotic aspects in the two groups; however, our study demonstrated that this trait consistently helped to annihilate some hypnotic experiences.

Several limitations of the study design should also be noted. First, the age span of our participants was narrow and whether the current results can be replicated in more diverse age groups remains unclear. Second, the SHSSC items that we used for the participant screening were aimed to detect somatoform dissociation and whether they had resulted in an enrollment bias remains unknown. However, with five-factor model of normal personality, we have demonstrated that the hypnotic susceptibility was associated with openness to experience, extraversion, and agreeableness, and we have shown that these associations were weak and their patterns were different in participants with high or low hypnotizability. Moreover, our study implies that adjusting hypnotizability through mobilizing the three personality traits help to manage some mental or somatoform disorders.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that the associations between normal personality traits and hypnotic susceptibility items were weak and different in the two groups, which imply that managing mental or somatoform disorders might be through adjusting hypnotizability and mobilizing personality functions. Future studies might be designed to see the detailed correlation patterns in high and low hypnotizable people.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by a grant from the Natural Science Foundation of China (grant no 81571336) to Dr W Wang.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • HilgardERThe domain of hypnosis: with some comments on alternative paradigmsAm Psychol197328119729824584217
  • GreenJPBarabaszAFBarrettDMontgomeryGHForging ahead: the 2003 APA division 30 definition of hypnosisInt J Clin Exp Hypn200553325926416076663
  • SilvaCBridgesKRMetzgerMPersonality, expectancy, and hypnotizabilityPers Individ Dif2005391131142
  • MorganAHThe heritability of hypnotic susceptibility in twinsJ Abnorm Psychol197382155614738327
  • LichtenbergPBachner-MelmanRGritsenkoIEbsteinRPExploratory association between cathechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) high/low enzyme activity polymorphism and hypnotizabilityAm J Med Genet200096677177411121178
  • MorganAHJohnsonDLHilgardERThe stability of hypnotic susceptibility: a longitudinal studyInt J Clin Exp Hypn19742232492574847205
  • PiccioneCHilgardERZimbardoPGOn the degree of stability of measured hypnotizability over a 25-year periodJ Pers Soc Psychol19895622892952926631
  • WangFChenWHuangJPreliminary study of relationships between hypnotic susceptibility and personality disorder functioning styles in healthy volunteers and personality disorder patientsBMC Psychiatry201111112121801440
  • ShorROrneMO’connellDPsychological correlates of plateau hypnotizability in a special volunteer sampleJ Pers Soc Psychol19663180955902080
  • LynnSJRhueJWThe fantasy-prone person: hypnosis, imagination, and creativityJ Pers Soc Psychol19865124044083746620
  • RocheSMMcConkeyKMAbsorption: nature, assessment, and correlatesJ Pers Soc Psychol199059191101
  • AmthauerRBrockeBLiepmannDBeauducelAIntelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 RGöttingenHogrefe2001
  • ShorROrneECHarvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form APalo Alto, CAConsulting Psychologists1962
  • GeigerEPeterBPradeTPiesbergenCIntelligence and hypnotizability: is there a connection?Int J Clin Exp Hypn201462331032924837062
  • HathawaySRMckinleyJCMinnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; Manual (Revised)San Antonio, TXPsychological Corporation1951
  • FawVWilcoxWWPersonality characteristics of susceptible and unsusceptible hypnotic subjectsInt J Clin Exp Hypn195868394
  • SchulmanRELondonPHypnotic susceptibilty and MMPI profilesJ Consult Psychol19632715716013987177
  • LichtenbergPBachner-MelmanREbsteinRPCrawfordHJHypnotic susceptibility: multidimensional relationships with Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire, COMT polymorphisms, absorption, and attentional characteristicsInt J Clin Exp Hypn2004521477214768969
  • CardeñaETerhuneDBLööfABurattiSHypnotic experience is related to emotional contagionInt J Clin Exp Hypn2009571334619031232
  • PeterBVogelSEPradeTGeigerEMohlJCPiesbergenCHypnotizability, personality style, and attachment: an exploratory study, part 1 – general resultsAm J Clin Hypn20155711340
  • CostaPTMcCraeRRThe NEO Personality Inventory: ManualOdessa, FLPsychological Assessment Resources1985
  • GliskyMLTatarynDJTobiasBAKihlstromJFMcConkeyKMAbsorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizabilityJ Pers Soc Psychol19916022632722016669
  • NordenstromBKCouncilJRMeierBPThe “big five” and hypnotic susceptibilityInt J Clin Exp Hypn200250327628112088333
  • SpanosNPRadtkeLHHodginsDCBertrandLDStamHJDuebreuilDLThe Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale: stability, reliability, and relationships with expectancy and “hypnotic experiences”Psychol Rep1983535555636647703
  • BowersKSWaterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C: manual and response bookletInt J Clin Exp Hypn19984632502689650438
  • GreenJPThe five factor model of personality and hypnotizability: little variance in commonContemp Hypn2004214161168
  • WeitzenhofferAMHilgardERStanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form CPalo Alto, CAConsulting Psychologists Press1965
  • MalinoskiPTLynnSJThe plasticity of early memory reports: social pressure, hypnotizability, compliance, and interrogative suggestibilityInt J Clin Exp Hypn199947432034510553313
  • TerhumeDBCardeñaELindgrenMDissocial tendencies and individual differences in high hypnotic suggestibilityCogn Neuropsychiatry201116211313520721761
  • WangWHuLMuLFunctioning styles of personality disorders and five-factor normal personality traits: a correlation study in Chinese studentsBMC Psychiatry2003311113129438
  • LivesleyWJJangKLDifferentiating normal, abnormal, and disordered personalityEur J Pers2005194257268
  • ClarkLAAssessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualizationAnnu Rev Psychol200758122725716903806
  • ChampignyCMRazATranscultural factors in hypnotizability scales: limits and prospectsAm J Clin Hypn201558217119426264541
  • YangJMcCraeRRCostaPTCross-cultural personality assessment in psychiatric populations: the NEO-PI-R in the People’s Republic of ChinaPsychol Assess1999113359368
  • WidigerTAThe DSM-5 dimensional model of personality disorder: rationale and empirical supportJ Pers Disord201125222223421466251
  • CardeñaEThe phenomenology of deep hypnosis: quiescent and physically activeInt J Clin Exp Hypn2005531375915788243
  • OkleyDAHalliganPWHypnotic suggestion and cognitive neuroscienceTrends Cogn Sci200913626427019428287
  • BentallRPThe illusion of reality: a review and integration of psychological research on hallucinationsPsychol Bull1990107182952404293
  • CostaPTMcCraeRRRevised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional ManualOdessa, FLPsychological Assessment Resources1992
  • RogersJHypnosis in the treatment of social phobiaAust J Clin Exp Hypn20083616468
  • HuberALuiFPorroCAHypnotic susceptibility modulates brain activity related to experimental placebo analgesiaPain201315491509151823664683
  • WickramasekeraIESzlykJPCould empathy be a predictor of hypnotic ability?Int J Clin Exp Hypn200351439039914594187
  • WagstaffGFParkersMHanleyJRA comparison of posthypnotic amnesia and the simulation of amnesia through brain injuryRev Int Psicol Ter Psicol201116778
  • StenburgGRosenIRisebergJAttention and personality in augmenting/reducing of visual evoked potentialsPers Individ Dif1990111212431254
  • HengartnerMPAjdacic-GrossVRodgersSMuellerMRoesslerWThe joint structure of normal and pathological personality: further evidence for a dimensional modelCompr Psychiatry201455366767424314825
  • EganVLewisMNeuroticism and agreeableness differentiate emotional and narcissistic expressions of aggressionPers Individ Dif2011506845850