241
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Potential role of pre- and postnatal testosterone levels in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: is there a sex difference?

, , , , , , , & show all
Pages 1331-1339 | Published online: 16 May 2017

Abstract

Objective

Both prenatal testosterone (T) exposure and postnatal T levels have been associated with developing neural circuitry and behavioral systems. This study examined the potential correlation between pre- and postnatal T levels and behavioral and neurocognitive profiles of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Methods

Two hundred ADHD patients with a mean age of 8.7±2.0 years (158 boys and 42 girls) were recruited. The ratio of the length of the right index finger (2D) to that of the right ring finger (4D) (2D/4D ratio) served as a surrogate of prenatal T exposure, and postnatal T was determined using salivary T concentration. Behavioral symptoms were evaluated using the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham – Version IV Scale for ADHD (SNAP-IV). Neurocognitive function was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) and Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT).

Results

Lower 2D/4D ratios were associated with comorbid disruptive behavior disorders (t=2.15, P=0.033) in all participants. Among the boys with ADHD, neither 2D/4D ratios nor salivary T levels were associated with behavioral symptoms or neurocognitive function. Among the girls with ADHD, the salivary T level was positively correlated with the Perceptual Reasoning Index of the WISC-IV (r=0.48, P=0.001) and the Confidence Index (r=0.37, P=0.017) and Omission Errors of the CPT (r=0.62, P<0.001).

Conclusion

Findings suggest that a higher prenatal T exposure is associated with a greater risk of developing disruptive behavior disorders, and T may exert differential neurocognitive effects between boys and girls with ADHD. However, the neurobiological mechanisms of T involved in the pathogenesis of ADHD warrant further investigation.

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which is characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, is one of the most prevalent neuropsychiatry disorders among children. ADHD is estimated to affect 3%–10% of school-age children worldwide.Citation1 The prevalence of ADHD is substantially different between the sexes: reported ratios of boys to girls range from 4:1 to 9:1.Citation2 In addition to prevalence rates, sex differences in the behavioral features and neuropsychological profiles of patients with ADHD have been widely debated.Citation3Citation5 On the basis of observations of sex differences, many researchers have raised questions with respect to the potential roles of the neuroendocrine system in the etiology of ADHD.Citation6Citation8 Testosterone (T) is a main sex hormone produced by the testicles and the adrenal glands and is important for the proper development of male sexual characteristics.Citation9 Both prenatal T exposure and postnatal T levels have been associated with developing neural circuitry and behavioral systems.Citation10,Citation11

High levels of prenatal T may affect the dopaminergic neural circuitry and decelerate neural development globally.Citation12 A commonly used surrogate index of prenatal T exposure is the ratio of the length of the right index finger (2D) to that of the right ring finger (4D) (2D/4D ratio).Citation13,Citation14 A lower 2D/4D ratio usually represents a higher level of intrauterine T exposure (relative to estrogen).Citation15 Several studies suggested that individuals with a lower, more male-typical, 2D/4D ratio tend to exhibit more prominent ADHD features.Citation16Citation19 These studies support the hypothesis that prenatal T exposure contributes to the development of ADHD in children. However, some researchers have not found associations between 2D/4D ratios and ADHD features or externalizing behavior problems in children.Citation20Citation22 In sum, whether prenatal T exposure plays a role in the etiology of ADHD is still open to debate.

Postnatal T activation from childhood to puberty potentially influences behavioral and physiological change, as well as neurodevelopment.Citation23,Citation24 Postnatal T levels are usually determined by analyzing the current (activational) T concentration in saliva or peripheral blood.Citation25 Several studies have investigated the relationship between current T level and ADHD. For example, Herguner et alCitation26 reported that women with polycystic ovary syndrome (an endocrine disorder that manifests hyperandrogenism) had more ADHD symptoms than did control subjects. Furthermore, higher T levels in saliva or in plasma have been observed in children with aggressive tendencies compared with those without aggressive tendencies.Citation27Citation29 In contrast, Dorn et alCitation30 suggested that no significant association is found between salivary T levels and children’s disruptive behavior disorders (DBD). To date, evidence about the connection of current T levels to the pathogenesis of ADHD remains scarce and inconsistent.

Peripheral T levels are similar among boys and girls during early childhood,Citation31 but T levels notably increase in prepubertal boys.Citation9 During this period, the activation of preformed brain structures leads to sexually dimorphic, physical, behavioral, and cognitive alterations.Citation10 Therefore, we proposed that T may be involved in the pathogenesis of sex-biased neuropsychiatric disorders, such as ADHD. However, few studies have simultaneously investigated the potential roles of prenatal and postnatal T exposure on the clinical features of ADHD. In addition, how T influences the neurocognitive function in patients with ADHD and whether there is a sex difference in the relationship between T and ADHD presentations remains poorly understood. The purpose of this study was to examine the potential correlations between T levels, both pre- and postnatal and behavioral and neurocognitive profiles of patients with ADHD. We also explored whether a sex difference in such a correlation is also present.

Methods

Study participants

The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Chang Gung Hospital in Taiwan (IRB No 101-4835A3). All procedures performed in this study involving participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the Helsinki Declaration. We obtained written informed consent from the parents or guardians of all the participating children.

Eligible patients with ADHD treated in the outpatient Department of Child Psychiatry at a medical center in Taiwan were recruited for this cross-sectional study from August 2013 to December 2015. The inclusion criteria were 1) clinical diagnosis of ADHD by a senior child psychiatrist based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) after structured interviews based on the Chinese version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children, epidemiologic version (K-SADS-E);Citation32 2) age between 6 and 16 years; 3) being Han Chinese; and 4) new diagnosis of ADHD in a drug-naïve patient or one who had an existing diagnosis but had not used an ADHD medication in the previous 6 months or longer. Patients were excluded if they had a history of comorbid pervasive developmental disorder, intellectual disability, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, psychosis, epilepsy, or brain injury. In total, 203 patients with ADHD were recruited into this study.

Assessments of prenatal T exposure and salivary T levels

Finger-length ratios are often used as a surrogate measure of prenatal hormone levels.Citation13 Lengths of fingers of all participants were measured by a research assistant. Lengths of fingers were measured from the connection to the palm to the tip of the finger. The 2D/4D ratios were computed in this study because they are the best established and best replicated with respect to human behavioral sex differences.Citation14

Saliva samples of ADHD patients were collected at 8:00 am in the outpatient department using the passive drool method; patients were instructed to avoid excessive levels of physical activity during the preceding 24-hour and to maintain an overnight fast before saliva collection. Saliva samples were collected in collecting tubes, immediately placed on ice, and then stored at −80°C until further analysis. Salivary T levels were quantified using a chemiluminescence immunoassay (IB57403; IBL-America, Minneapolis, MN, USA). The sensitivity of detection of this method was 1.0 pg/mL. The intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were 2.9%–7.0% at 21–557 pg/mL and 1.5%–3.2% at 38–541 pg/mL, respectively.

Clinical measurements

Each ADHD patient was interviewed by a senior psychiatrist who used the K-SADS-E diagnostic tool. An experienced child psychologist conducted the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) and Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT) with individual patients in a room dedicated to reducing variability in testing conditions. The Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham – Version IV Scale (SNAP-IV) parent form and SNAP-IV teacher form were completed by the patient’s parents and a teacher, respectively.

The K-SADS-E is a semistructured diagnostic interview that is designed to assess current and past episodes of psychopathology in children and adolescents according to DSM-III-Revised (DSM-III-R) and DSM-IV criteria.Citation32 The K-SADS-E is administered by interviewing the parent(s) and the child and finally achieving summary ratings that include all sources of information. The validity and reliability of the Chinese version of K-SADS-E have been established in Taiwan.Citation33

The WISC-IV Chinese version is an individually administered and norm-referenced instrument designed to measure the intelligence of children aged from 6 to 16 years.Citation34 The WISC-IV contains 10 core and 5 supplemental subtests. The core subtests are used to form 4 factor indexes, including the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), the Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), the Working Memory Index (WMI), and the Processing Speed Index (PSI). The Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) is also formed from the 10 core subtests. The factor indexes and FSIQ each has a population mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.Citation35

The SNAP-IV is a 26-item questionnaire that is used to evaluate ADHD symptoms and severity and is completed by parents or teachers.Citation36 The 26 items include 18 for ADHD symptoms (9 for inattentive and 9 for hyperactive/impulsive) and 8 for oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) symptoms as defined in DSM-IV. Each item is scored on a 0–3 Likert scale. The Chinese version of the SNAP-IV parent formCitation37 and the SNAP-IV teacher formCitation38 were reported to have satisfactory levels of reliability and concurrent validity.

The CPT is a 14-minute computerized test that primarily assesses attention and impulse control.Citation39 Briefly, participants are required to respond to the stimuli on a computer screen by pressing a space bar for every letter except the letter “X.” Among the multiple dependent measures that may be used, omissions, commissions, hit reaction time, and detectability are the most commonly used indices. The T-score of the CPT, in which a lower T-score indicates better performance, is commonly used in research analyses. The Confidence Index (percentile) showed the integration of all the CPT data obtained from test administration to provide a chance out of 100 that a significant attention problem existed.Citation40

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using the statistical software package SPSS, version 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Variables are presented as either the mean (standard deviation) or frequency. In 3 patients, the salivary T levels were undetermined, and they were excluded from the analysis. Categorical variables among male and female patients were compared using either the chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test, depending on case numbers, and an independent t-test was used to compare continuous variables between groups. Pearson correlation was performed to analyze the relationships between 2D/4D ratios, salivary T levels, clinical symptoms, and neuropsychological functions measured by the CPT and WISC-IV, respectively. Two-tailed P-values of <0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

A total of 200 ADHD patients (mean age 8.7±2.0 years) were recruited into this study. Of these patients, 158 (79%) were boys and 42 (21%) were girls; 60 of them (30%) were classified as inattentive type, and 140 (70%) were classified as hyperactive-impulsive type or combined type. In terms of psychiatric comorbidities, 35 (17.5%) patients had comorbid ODD, 2 (1%) had conduct disorder, 18 (9%) had tic disorders, 4 (2%) had sleep disorders, and 9 (4.5%) had anxiety disorders. Compared to the girls (), boys were taller (t=3.08, P=0.002), heavier (t=3.07, P=0.002), have higher body mass index (BMI) (t=2.34, P=0.023), more likely to be ADHD hyperactive/impulsive or combined type (χ2=4.19, P=0.041), have higher VCI scores on the WISC-IV (t=2.31, P=0.022), and have higher teacher-rated hyperactivity/impulsivity scores (t=4.37, P<0.001) and oppositional scores on the SNAP-IV (t=3.33, P=0.001).

Table 1 Characteristics of patients with ADHD and comparison of male and female patients

Of the total 200 patients with ADHD, the mean 2D/4D ratios and salivary T levels were 96.1%±4.6% and 50.1±79.4 pg/mL, respectively. Patients’ salivary T levels were not significantly correlated with their 2D/4D ratios (r=0.09, P=0.229). lists the relationships between 2D/4D ratios and salivary T levels between sexes, ADHD subtypes, and comorbidities. Patients with comorbid DBD (ODD or conduct disorder) had lower 2D/4D ratios than did their counterparts without a comorbidity (t=2.15, P=0.033). The salivary T levels of ADHD boys were significantly higher than those of ADHD girls (t=2.04, P=0.043).

Table 2 Ratios of 2D/4D and salivary testosterone levels between sex and ADHD subtypes and comorbidities among ADHD patients

summarizes the correlations between 2D/4D ratios, salivary T levels, patients’ characteristics, and neuropsychological function among ADHD patients, stratified by sex. In both sexes, the 2D/4D ratios were not correlated with any clinical measure or neuropsychological performance. Among the boys with ADHD, salivary T levels were positively correlated with age (r=0.22, P=0.005), height (r=0.22, P=0.005), body weight (r=0.24, P=0.003), and BMI (r=0.17, P=0.036), but they did not correlate with clinical symptoms and neurocognitive performance. Among girls with ADHD, salivary T levels positively correlated with the PRI scores on the WISC-IV (r=0.48, P=0.001), Confidence Index (r=0.37, P=0.017), and Omission Errors of the CPT (r=0.62, P<0.001).

Table 3 Correlations among 2D/4D ratios, salivary testosterone levels, patients’ characteristics, and neuropsychological functions among ADHD patients

Discussion

The findings in this study indicate that prenatal T exposure, using the 2D/4D ratio as a surrogate index, is not associated with ADHD clinical symptoms or neuropsychological performance. We did find an association between prenatal T exposure and comorbid DBD (ODD or conduct disorder) in our sample overall, but there was no evidence for an association between 2D/4D ratio and DBD scores for either sex. Among girls with ADHD, a higher level of salivary T was associated with a better performance on the PRI in the WISC-IV and a poorer performance on the Confidence Index and Omissions Errors of the CPT. In contrast, no association between salivary T levels and clinical assessments was observed among boys with ADHD.

2D/4D ratios and ADHD

We did not find a sexual difference in 2D/4D ratios among school-age children with ADHD in our cohort. Evidence generally indicates that 2D/4D ratio of the right hand is lower in males compared with females among general population.Citation41 However, the overall patterns of 2D/4D ratio may vary across age, sex, and hand.Citation42 As study sample contained ADHD patients and the sample size of girls was also small, the current result may not be comparable to previous literature. In both sexes, the 2D/4D ratios were not correlated with any clinical measure or neuropsychological performance. Several researchers have reported that the 2D/4D ratio correlates inversely with ADHD symptoms in both healthy and clinical populations.Citation16Citation19 However, some studies have failed to replicate such a correlation and challenge the hypothesis that fetal T exposure increases the risk of early ADHD.Citation20Citation22 The inconsistency between studies might be caused by sample diversity (eg, clinical or community sample, age distribution, and specific sex). Our study sample consisted of pure ADHD patients with a mean age of 8.7±2.0 years, and these sample characteristics were similar to those of the sample enrolled by Lemiere et al; in fact, the findings in our study are generally in line with those of Lemiere et al.Citation21 This phenomenon may reflect that subjects with ADHD genuinely differ from typically developed children in terms of the sex effect on this measure. In sum, we suggest that there was no significant association between 2D/4D ratio and ADHD severity among our cohort of school-age children with ADHD.

2D/4D ratios and DBD

Patients with comorbid DBD (ODD or conduct disorder) had lower 2D/4D ratios than did their counterparts without the comorbidity. There was no evidence for an association between 2D/4D ratio and ODD scores for either sex. ODD and conduct disorder, which are characterized by temper tantrums, physical aggression, and other forms of defiance or resistance to authority, are the most common psychiatric comorbidities with ADHD.Citation43 Several studies have investigated the potential association between T and DBD. For example, de Bruin et alCitation44 observed that males with autistic spectrum disorders and those with ADHD or ODD had lower 2D/4D ratios than did males with an anxiety disorder. Roberts and MartelCitation19 reported that there were no significant associations between right-hand 2D/4D ratios and ODD or total symptoms of DBD. Subsequently, Martel and RobertsCitation45 demonstrated that high prenatal T exposure increases the risk of DBD symptoms in males who are susceptible to prenatal environmental stressors (eg, prenatal alcohol exposure). Prenatal T could influence the development of neural circuits that regulate affect and impulse control.Citation23 Taken together with our results, we suggest that these findings support the hypothesis that excessive prenatal T exposure may result in a high risk of developing DBD among school-age children with ADHD.

Salivary testosterone levels and ADHD

We found that salivary T levels were positively correlated with age, height, body weight, and BMI in boys, but this phenomenon was not observed in girls. The result herein is inconsistent with the findings reported by Clifton et al,Citation46 in which salivary T levels are negatively correlated with BMI among British men (ages between 18 and 74 years and BMI ≥18.5). We suggest that, different from the physiological effect in adult, peripheral T levels may be associated with physical development in male youths.Citation10 Furthermore, our results show that, among ADHD patients, current salivary T levels are not significantly correlated with their behavioral symptoms and comorbidities. Some researchers have revealed a significant positive relationship between T levels (either plasma or salivary) and the tendency toward DBD.Citation27Citation29 Dorn et alCitation30 did not find such a relationship, which is generally comparable with the findings in our study. The possible explanation is that T has been implicated in dominant behavior and muscular physiology change but does not necessarily cause aggression or externalized problem behaviors.Citation23,Citation24 The findings in this study do not support the assumption that current T levels influence ADHD behavior.

Salivary testosterone levels and neurocognitive functions

Among the boys with ADHD, salivary T levels were not significantly correlated with neurocognitive functions. In contrast, among the girls in our cohort, salivary T levels seem to be beneficial for the PRI performance in the WISC-IV and may be detrimental for attention measured by the CPT. Bjork et alCitation47 reported a positive relationship between T and impaired impulse control assessed using CPT among healthy women. In general, the CPT has been considered to be advantageous compared with other ADHD behavioral rating scales because it eliminates rater bias.Citation48 We assume that CPT may be more sensitive for detecting impaired attention than the scores of the SNAP-IV. We found that salivary T levels in girls were lower than those in boys. Girls with ADHD possibly were more susceptible to endogenous T exposure, which further influenced their neuropsychological performance.

The PRI in the WISC-IV mainly assesses nonverbal reasoning abilities and problem-solving skills. Some previous studiesCitation49Citation51 have investigated the potential influence of T on human cognitive function, but with contradictory results. Puts et alCitation49 indicated that salivary T levels do not play a role in sex differences in spatial ability among young men and women. Shangguan and ShiCitation50 suggested that the relationship between salivary T level and fluid intelligence varies based on participants’ age. In prepubertal children, a significant association with salivary T levels and IQ was observed among boys, but not among girls.Citation51 A brain imaging study revealed a positive association between T levels and activation of certain brain areas, including the inferior frontal lobe, the inferior parietal lobe, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.Citation52 These brain areas manage nonverbal reasoning and visuospatial ability, which are considered superior in males relative to females.Citation53 On the basis of these findings and our results, we suggest that the current T level may exert its neurodevelopmental effects and modify cognitive patterns, particularly among girls with ADHD.

Limitations

This study has several limitations: first, this study lacked a control group. Therefore, we could not compare the pre-or postnatal T levels among ADHD patients to those of healthy control subjects. Second, this study was performed with a cross-sectional design. The causal relationships between T exposure, DBD, and neurocognitive deficits remain unclear. In addition, this study used salivary T as a surrogate marker of postnatal T exposure. The mechanisms by which postnatal T exposure during patients’ early childhood influences their current ADHD characteristics is unknown, and peripheral salivary T levels may not necessarily represent the T exposure in the central nervous system. Third, the sample size of the girls with ADHD was relatively small compared with the sample size of boys, which reduces the statistical power to detect potential correlations of T levels with clinical assessments. Fourth, because the analyses in this study were exploratory, correction of multiple testing was not performed. However, if Bonferroni correction was performed to adjust for multiple testing in the correlation matrix (), none of the significant findings would survive. Therefore, the findings in this study may suggest that only a subtle relationship exists between T and ADHD manifestations. Moreover, we collected only one saliva sample for each subject, lacking duplicates to perform further analysis. It is possible that the variations of T levels in saliva are massive and our result should be interpreted with caution. Finally, some important factors (eg, personality traits or prenatal alcohol exposure) have been reported to mediate relationships between T and ADHD psychopathology.Citation18,Citation45 These factors were not evaluated in this study.

Conclusion

We found that a higher level of prenatal T exposure (lower 2D/4D ratio) is associated with comorbid ODD or conduct disorder among patients with ADHD. Among boys with ADHD, neither prenatal T exposure nor current T levels correlated with ADHD clinical symptoms and neuropsychological performance. Among girls with ADHD, the current T level may exert a beneficial effect on nonverbal reasoning and visuospatial ability measured by the WISC-IV, but elevated T levels have a detrimental effect on attention function measured by the CPT. Further studies are warranted to clarify the neurobiological mechanisms underlying T’s involvement in the pathogenesis of ADHD.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Professor Wei-Tsun Soong for granting us the use of the Chinese version of the K-SADS and Professor Shur-Fen Gau for granting our use of the Chinese version of the SNAP-IV. This work was supported by the Taiwan National Science Council (NSC 102-2314-B-182A-009), the Taiwan Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST 103-2314-B-182A-017), and the Chang Gung Memorial Hospital Research Projects (CMRPG8E0561 and CMRPG8E0571).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • PolanczykGde LimaMSHortaBLBiedermanJRohdeLAThe worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysisAm J Psychiatry2007164694294817541055
  • RucklidgeJJGender differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderPsychiatr Clin North Am201033235737320385342
  • HassonRFineJGGender differences among children with ADHD on continuous performance tests: a meta-analytic reviewJ Atten Disord201216319019822290696
  • NussbaumNLADHD and female specific concerns: a review of the literature and clinical implicationsJ Atten Disord20121628710021976033
  • WangLJChenCKHuangYSGender differences in the behavioral symptoms and neuropsychological performance of patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder treated with methylphenidate: A Two-Year Follow-up StudyJ Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol201525650150826262904
  • MartelMMKlumpKNiggJTBreedloveSMSiskCLPotential hormonal mechanisms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and major depressive disorder: a new perspectiveHorm Behav200955446547919265696
  • DaviesWSex differences in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: candidate genetic and endocrine mechanismsFront Neuroendocrinol201435333134624680800
  • BruntonPJProgramming the brain and behaviour by early-life stress: a focus on neuroactive steroidsJ Neuroendocrinol201527646848025688636
  • AlbinAKNorjavaaraEPubertal growth and serum testosterone and estradiol levels in boysHorm Res Paediatr201380210011023988590
  • AuyeungBLombardoMVBaron-CohenSPrenatal and postnatal hormone effects on the human brain and cognitionPflugers Arch2013465555757123588379
  • JuraskaJMSiskCLDonCarlosLLSexual differentiation of the adolescent rodent brain: hormonal influences and developmental mechanismsHorm Behav201364220321023998664
  • MorrisJAJordanCLBreedloveSMSexual differentiation of the vertebrate nervous systemNat Neurosci20047101034103915452574
  • ManningJTScuttDWilsonJLewis-JonesDIThe ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogenHum Reprod19981311300030049853845
  • Cohen-BendahanCCvan de BeekCBerenbaumSAPrenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findingsNeurosci Biobehav Rev200529235338415811504
  • ManningJKilduffLCookCCrewtherBFinkBDigit ratio (2D:4D): a biomarker for prenatal sex steroids and adult sex steroids in challenge situationsFront Endocrinol (Lausanne)20145924523714
  • StevensonJCEversonPMWilliamsDCHipskindGGrimesMMahoneyERAttention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and digit ratios in a college sampleAm J Hum Biol2007191415017160985
  • JamesWHFurther evidence that some male-based neurodevelopmental disorders are associated with high intrauterine testosterone concentrationsDev Med Child Neurol2008501151818173623
  • MartelMMConscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)J Child Psychol Psychiatry200950779079819298468
  • RobertsBAMartelMMPrenatal testosterone and preschool disruptive behavior disordersPers Individ Dif201355896296625598567
  • MahoneEMWhy fewer females than males are diagnosed with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: it might not be hormonesDev Med Child Neurol201052979079120497456
  • LemiereJBoetsBDanckaertsMNo association between the 2D:4D fetal testosterone marker and multidimensional attentional abilities in children with ADHDDev Med Child Neurol2010529e202e20820491856
  • LiuJPortnoyJRaineAAssociation between a marker for prenatal testosterone exposure and externalizing behavior problems in childrenDev Psychopathol201224377178222781854
  • NguyenTVMcCrackenJTAlbaughMDBotteronKNHudziakJJDucharmeSA testosterone-related structural brain phenotype predicts aggressive behavior from childhood to adulthoodPsychoneuroendo-crinology201663109118
  • RiceTRPostnatal testosterone may be an important mediator of the association between prematurity and male neurodevelopmental disorders: a hypothesisInt J Adolesc Med Health2017292pii
  • OhzekiTManellaBGubelin-De CampoCZachmannMSalivary testosterone concentrations in prepubertal and pubertal males: comparison with total and free plasma testosteroneHorm Res1991365–62352371823081
  • HergunerSHarmanciHToyHAttention deficit-hyperactivity disorder symptoms in women with polycystic ovary syndromeInt J Psychiatry Med201550331732526449924
  • ScerboASKolkoDJSalivary testosterone and cortisol in disruptive children: relationship to aggressive, hyperactive, and internalizing behav-iorsJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1994338117411847982868
  • PajerKTabbahRGardnerWRubinRTCzambelRKWangYAdrenal androgen and gonadal hormone levels in adolescent girls with conduct disorderPsychoneuroendocrinology200631101245125617126492
  • YuYZShiJXRelationship between levels of testosterone and cortisol in saliva and aggressive behaviors of adolescentsBiomed Environ Sci2009221444919462687
  • DornLDKolkoDJSusmanEJSalivary gonadal and adrenal hormone differences in boys and girls with and without disruptive behavior disorders: contextual variantsBiol Psychol2009811313919428966
  • BiroFMPinneySMHuangBHormone changes in peripubertal girlsJ Clin Endocrinol Metab201499103829383525029416
  • KaufmanJBirmaherBBrentDSchedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity dataJ Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry19973679809889204677
  • GauSFSoongWTPsychiatric comorbidity of adolescents with sleep terrors or sleepwalking: a case-control studyAust N Z J Psychiatry199933573473910544999
  • YangPChengCPChangCLLiuTLHsuHYYenCFWechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th edition-Chinese version index scores in Taiwanese children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderPsychiatry Clin Neurosci2013672839123438160
  • BaronISTest review: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV)Child Neuropsychol200511547147516306021
  • BussingRFernandezMHarwoodMParent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sampleAssessment200815331732818310593
  • GauSSShangCYLiuSKPsychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, version IV scale – parent formInt J Methods Psychiatr Res2008171354418286459
  • GauSSLinCHHuFCPsychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham, Version IV Scale-Teacher FormJ Pediatr Psychol200934885086119074488
  • ConnersCKThe computerized continuous performance testPsychopharmacol Bull19852148918924089110
  • ConnersCKConners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPTII) for Windows Technical Guide and Software ManualNorth Tonwanda, NYMHS2004
  • XuYZhengYThe digit ratio (2D:4D) in China:a meta-analysisAm J Hum Biol201527330430925284473
  • WongWIHinesMInterpreting digit ratio (2D:4D)-behavior correlations: 2D:4D sex difference, stability, and behavioral correlates and their replicability in young childrenHorm Behav201678869426542674
  • ConnorDFSteeberJMcBurnettKA review of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder complicated by symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorderJ Dev Behav Pediatr201031542744020535081
  • de BruinEIVerheijFWiegmanTFerdinandRFDifferences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disordersDev Med Child Neurol2006481296296517109783
  • MartelMMRobertsBAPrenatal testosterone increases sensitivity to prenatal stressors in males with disruptive behavior disordersNeurotoxicol Teratol201444111724819590
  • CliftonSMacdowallWCopasAJSalivary testosterone levels and health status in men and women in the British general population: findings from the Third National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles (Natsal-3)J Clin Endocrinol Metab2016101113939395127552539
  • BjorkJMMoellerFGDoughertyDMSwannACEndogenous plasma testosterone levels and commission errors in women: a preliminary reportPhysiol Behav2001731–221722111399314
  • MadaanVDaughtonJLubberstedtBMattaiAVaughanBSKratochvilCJAssessing the efficacy of treatments for ADHD: overview of methodological issuesCNS Drugs200822427529018336058
  • PutsDACardenasRABaileyDHBurrissRPJordanCLBreedloveSMSalivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or womenHorm Behav201058228228920226788
  • ShangguanFShiJPuberty timing and fluid intelligence: a study of correlations between testosterone and intelligence in 8- to 12-year-old Chinese boysPsychoneuroendocrinology200934798398819249158
  • OstatnikovaDCelecPPutzZIntelligence and salivary testosterone levels in prepubertal childrenNeuropsychologia20074571378138517166527
  • SchoningSEngelienAKugelHFunctional anatomy of visuo-spatial working memory during mental rotation is influenced by sex, menstrual cycle, and sex steroid hormonesNeuropsychologia200745143203321417689571
  • Rizk-JacksonAMAcevedoSFInmanDHowiesonDBeniceTSRaberJEffects of sex on object recognition and spatial navigation in humansBehav Brain Res2006173218119016887201