249
Views
15
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Teenage outcomes after speech and language impairment at preschool age

, , , , &
Pages 221-227 | Published online: 15 May 2012

Abstract

Aim

Ten years ago, we published developmental data on a representative group of children (n = 25) with moderate or severe speech and language impairment, who were attending special preschools for children. The aim of this study was to perform a follow-up of these children as teenagers.

Methods

Parents of 23 teenagers participated in a clinical interview that requested information on the child’s current academic achievement, type of school, previous clinical assessments, and developmental diagnoses. Fifteen children participated in a speech and language evaluation, and 13 participated in a psychological evaluation.

Results

Seven of the 23 teenagers had a mild intellectual disability, and another three had borderline intellectual functioning. Nine had symptoms of disorders on the autism spectrum; five of these had an autism spectrum disorder, and four had clear autistic traits. Six met criteria for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)/subthreshold ADHD. Thirteen of 15 teenagers had a moderate or severe language impairment, and 13 of 15 had a moderate or severe reading impairment. Overlapping disorders were frequent. None of the individuals who underwent the clinical evaluation were free from developmental problems.

Conclusion

A large number of children with speech and language impairment at preschool age had persistent language problems and/or met the criteria for developmental diagnoses other than speech and language impairment at their follow-up as teenagers. Language impairment in young children is a marker for several developmental disorders, particularly intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder.

Introduction

Language impairments (LI) are common in young children and affect about 5% to 6% of preschoolers.Citation1 LI is frequently accompanied by other developmental problems, such as intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).Citation1 Despite the presence of a more complex neurodevelopmental abnormality, LI is often the first deviation noted.

Miniscalco et alCitation1 found that 62% of the children who had had language delay at the time of speech and language screening at 2.5 years at the Child Health Care Center exhibited a major neuropsychiatric/developmental disorder at the follow-up conducted at 7 years.

Snowling et alCitation2 followed 71 children who had had early language problems up to the age of 15 years. The authors concluded that children whose language delay had been resolved by 5.5 years had particularly good outcomes but that the outcome was not as good for children whose language difficulties persisted through their school years. They emphasized that children with language difficulties at the age of their entry into school are a vulnerable group who require not only language-related intervention but also, in some cases, emotional and behavioral support. The follow-up revealed that the type of initial LI and the child’s intelligence quotient (IQ) were related to different clinical subgroups in adolescence. Expressive LI was associated with attention problems, whereas receptive and expressive language difficulties were associated with social difficulties, and those who had a low IQ and both receptive and expressive LI exhibited both attention and social impairments in adolescence.

Proposals of unitary models to explain LI have not been successful, and there is mounting evidence that LI is a complex neurodevelopmental condition. In spite of the heterogeneous nature of the condition, it is clear that it is very often accompanied by limitations in underlying cognitive processes, such as working memory, executive functioningCitation3Citation5 and cognitive processing speed.Citation6,Citation7 For example, verbal working memory has been found to correlate with several language processes in children and adults. It also plays an important role in language comprehension during the acquisition of language because it allows the learner to analyze and determine the structural properties of language.Citation8,Citation9 Limitations in working memory, executive functioning, and processing speed are often seen in both ADHD and ASD,Citation10 conditions, which, as mentioned earlier, often overlap with LI.

We had previously studied a group of children aged from 5 to 7 years with moderate or severe speech and language disorder who were attending special preschools. At that time, 58 children born between 1993 and 1994 were attending these preschools. The children had been assessed by both a speech pathologist and a child psychologist before they were admitted to exclude those who had autism and those who had ID. The preschools accepted only children with moderate to severe LI. Our intention was to include all children, but limitations on the resources available for this project meant that two preschools had to be excluded, which corresponded to 18 children. Of the remaining 40 children, the parents of 15 children declined to participate. There was no indication from preschool teachers or speech pathologists that these children differed in their developmental profile from those who participated. Thus, this group was representative of children in special preschools for children with moderate or severe LI. We found that 90% of the children had additional developmental disorders or problems on the learning/intellectual spectrum, the autism spectrum, and the attention spectrum. We concluded that neurodevelopmental deviations were very common in children with LI and that the optimal route for the assessment and treatment of these children would be a multidisciplinary approach.Citation11

The aim of the study presented here was to undertake a 10-year clinical follow-up of the children who participated in our previous preschool study and analyze their academic achievement, general cognitive abilities, associated developmental disorders or problems, and specific speech and language characteristics.

Methods

Participants

The participants consisted of the previously assessed preschool children with language impairment, who were a group of 25 children comprised of 18 boys and 7 girls between 5 and 7 years old at the time of the first study. When they were referred to the special preschool, they had all been considered to have no other developmental disorders besides LI.

In our previous study (Time 1), three children had an IQ indicating ID (IQ ≤71) and four had a borderline intellectual function (BIF) (IQ 72–85). Nine of the 25 children had symptoms corresponding to ADHD (inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, or combined type). Eight children had symptoms in this area exceeding the 95th percentile of the reference group used. No child had a diagnosed ASD, but six children had definite autistic symptoms, and another five had fewer autistic symptoms that exceeded the 95th percentile of the comparison group. LI was classified as combined receptive-expressive language impairment in 21 children. One child was classified as exclusively receptive, and three children were classified as exclusively expressive.Citation11

All parents of these 25 children were contacted in 2010, which was 10 years after the initial study. Two parents declined to take part in the follow-up study. Twenty-three parents were willing to participate and were interviewed. Fifteen adolescents, now aged 16 to 17 years, took part in the speech and language assessment, and of these, 13 also participated in the cognitive assessment.

Parental interview

The parents of 15 teenagers were interviewed in person regarding the clinical assessment. Eight teenagers declined to take part in the clinical assessment, so their parents were interviewed over the telephone. The interview followed a structured protocol, including the child’s academic achievement, type of school attended, previous assessments and diagnoses given at specialized neuropsychiatric or pediatric units after our initial assessment when the child was of preschool age (Time 1). The parents were also interviewed according to the ADHD symptom list.Citation12 All findings were compared with the corresponding data obtained from the relevant child’s previous assessments.

Cognitive assessment

The cognitive assessment was carried out according to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – III.Citation13 All 13 subtests were used in the assessments. The verbal IQ, performance IQ, full scale IQ, and the four Kaufmann indices were calculated. These are the indices for verbal understanding, perceptual organization, freedom from distractibility (FDI), and processing speed (PSI).Citation13 The FDI is composed of the Digit span and Arithmetic subtests, both of which express working memory. The PSI reflects capacity of simultaneous processing and focus shift. Data were compared with the results from each individual’s previous assessment during preschool. At that time, the Leiter Nonverbal Scale was used.Citation14

In addition, an evaluation of academic performance (final grade after the 9th school year) was performed for each child.

Language, speech and reading assessments

A two-step compilation of language and reading data was performed in order to determine and classify the levels of language and reading achievement of each participant. The following subtests were used (reading related assessments are marked withR and language related assessment are marked withL). LOGOS,Citation15 a standardized computer-assisted test battery, was used to assess the following skills: reading fluencyR, readingR and listening comprehensionL, orthographic readingR (word sight reading), phonological decodingR, phonological awarenessR, and rapid automatic namingL. SpellingR and conceptual word comprehensionL were assessed by a subtest in a Swedish reading test, DLS (Diagnostiskt test för Läsning och Skrivning [Diagnostic Reading and Writing test]).Citation16,Citation17 VocabularyL was assessed by the Boston Naming Test.Citation18,Citation19 Sentence comprehensionL was assessed by the Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG 2).Citation20

In the first step, the results from each subtest were classified as follows: normal if ≥ the 30th percentile or stanine 3: moderate problems if < the 30th percentile or stanine 3; and severe problems if ≤ the 15th percentile or stanine 2. In the second step, a compiled classification of overall receptive language level and overall reading level was defined as follows: normal if the results in all assessments regarding reading or language ≥ the 30th percentile or stanine 3; moderate problems if < 30th percentile or stanine 3 in one or more of the assessments; and severe problems if ≤ the 15th percentile or stanine 2 on one or more of the assessments.

An assessment of phonological and dyspraxic speech symptoms was performed by two independent language pathologists who analyzed speech recordings of each participant as they told a short story. Speech was rated as normal, deviant but not difficult to understand, markedly deviant, or difficult to understand. There was complete interrater agreement.

The Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm approved the study, which was performed under the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

General cognitive data

Thirteen individuals were assessed by the psychologist at the time of the follow-up. Six had an average intellectual function, four had ID, and three girls had BIF. For ten children, information was given at the parental interview. Seven of these had average intellectual function, and three had a diagnosis of ID, which had been made after an assessment by a specialized team.

Mean verbal and performance IQs, as well as verbal understanding and perceptual organization indices, did not differ from age norms. There was no significant difference between the mean verbal and performance IQs. However, the freedom from distractibility and the processing speed indices were significantly lower compared to the age-equivalent norms (t12 = −4.28; P = 0.001 for freedom from distractibility and t11 = −4.06, P = 0.002 for processing speed) (see ). On an individual basis, none showed a significant difference between verbal and performance IQs.

Table 1 Cognitive data in 13 teenagers at follow-up: IQs and Kaufmann indices

ADHD

Two individuals met the full criteria for ADHD symptoms (ie, they met at least six of the nine attention-related criteria) at the follow-up, and four met four of the nine criteria, ie, subthreshold criteria.Citation21 One girl had severe clinical executive function problems, but according to the parental interview, she did not meet the attention symptom criteria. Thus, six of the 23 individuals had definitive ADHD/subthreshold ADHD, and of these six individuals, five also had a diagnosis of ASD or clear autistic traits.

Autism spectrum disorders

Five of the 23 participants had been assessed at specialized units and been given a diagnosis of ASD; autism (n = 1), atypical autism (n = 2), and Asperger’s syndrome (n = 2). Four were found to have clear autistic traits according to data that were communicated by their parents, which was also observed at the cognitive assessment. One of these four had recently been admitted for a diagnostic work-up owing to suspected ASD.

Language and reading data

Thirteen of the 15 individuals (87%) who were assessed during the follow-up had LI; seven were classified as having moderate LI, and six as having severe LI. Thirteen of the 15 (87%) had reading deficits; seven of these were moderate and six were severe. The existence of language and reading-related problems were overlapping except in two cases. Of the six children with severe LI combined with a severe reading disorder, four had ID and two had BIF (see ).

Table 2 Cognitive, ASD, ADHD, and language and reading results at a preschool age and at the follow-ups (time 1 and time 2)

Moreover, it was noteworthy that in the language assessments, 12 out of 15 performed at or below the 15th percentile on the assessment of conceptual word comprehension; four of these 12 individuals had ID and three had BIF. The results for reading fluency and spelling were below the 15th percentile in 12 and 11 of the 15 cases, respectively. In spite of the low reading fluency, 10 of the 15 participants had superior reading comprehension compared to listening comprehension, and of these 10 individuals, 5 had ADHD or subthreshold ADHD.

In six of the 15 individuals, elements of phonological deficits or dyspraxia were evident in their speech. Two of the six individuals had marked articulation deficits and three of these six teenagers also had ID.

The developmental outcome for each individual is presented in . When the cognitive results at the follow-up were compared with the data from the preschool assessment (using the Leiter test), it was found that the three children with ID at the first assessment still fulfilled the criteria for this disability. Another four of the participants were determined to have ID at the follow-up; two of these four had been considered to have BIF and two had a normal intellectual function when they had been assessed in the preschool study. Two children who were considered to have an average intellectual function in the first assessment were found to have BIF by the follow-up. At the time of the preschool study (conducted at 5 to 7 years), three had ID and four had BIF.Citation11 Thus, the intellectual function of a total of six children was found to have deteriorated, whereas 17 remained in the same intellectual category.

None of the nine individuals who were found to have symptoms that corresponded to ADHD during the first assessment in preschool met the ADHD criteria at the time of the follow-up; however, five of them were found to have ASD or clear autistic symptoms at the follow-up. At the follow-up, six of the eight individuals with attention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms exceeding the 95th percentile of the comparison group as preschoolers no longer had such ADHD symptoms; however, one had ADHD and one had subthreshold ADHD. The changes in the ADHD symptoms are illustrated in .

In the preschool study, eleven children had autistic traits that were mild or definite according to preschool reports. At the follow-up, five children had a diagnosis within the autism spectrum and four had traits. Three of the six children who had had definite autistic symptoms at preschool age had a definite autism spectrum disorder at the follow-up. The changes in the autism symptoms are illustrated in .

At the time of the follow up, four individuals were enrolled in the program of general academic studies in upper secondary school (id numbers 11, 12, 14, and 15; ). The others either were in a program for vocational training in the upper secondary school for the mentally retarded or had not yet finished the 9-year program of compulsory school education.

At the time of their referral to the speech and language preschool, no child had received any additional diagnosis although that all of them had been assessed by a psychologist and a speech and language pathologist. Their only diagnosis was LI. However, in connection with our preschool study, we identified accompanying developmental problems in about 90% of the children. At the follow-up conducted when they were teenagers, only one individual was free from language and reading problems. This boy had had an exclusively expressive LI at preschool age, but according to the assessment conducted at that time, he had had ADHD and mild autistic symptoms. At the follow-up ADHD was not confirmed.

Discussion

This follow-up study describes the outcomes for 23 out of 25 adolescents who 10 years earlier had had moderate or severe LI. At that time, 90% were found to display associated developmental problems. The group included in this study is small, but at the time of the initial assessment, the group was representative of all preschool children attending special preschools for language impaired children in the county, and they had been assessed with a multidisciplinary approach.

The main result of this 10-year-follow-up study, also performed with a view to assessing different kinds of developmental problems, was that all adolescents exhibited developmental problems of various types and combinations. For the majority, language impairment was not the main problem. Instead, their developmental profiles were dominated by a variety of cognitive dysfunctions that in many cases overlapped. These were general cognitive impairments, ASD, and deficits within the attention spectrum. Of the 15 individuals who took part in the language and reading assessments, only four (id numbers 1, 2, 8, and 11; ) had language and reading problems as their main developmental disorders, and no other developmental disorder was identified at the follow-up. Of these four individuals, one had had exclusively expressive LI at preschool age.

At the time of the follow-up, one third of the children had ID or BIF. At the first cognitive assessment conducted during preschool, three were found to have an IQ at or below 71, whereas at the time of the follow-up, seven had definite ID. The cognitive test used at the first assessment was a nonverbal test (Leiter), which may have contributed to the low detection rate of children with ID at that time. Moreover, developmental problems that are apparent at an early age are not always easy to disentangle and assign to a definite diagnostic category. This difficulty highlights the importance of always planning a follow-up for children exhibiting any type of developmental disorder. LI is a marker for several developmental disorders, particularly ID and ASD.

The cognitive assessments revealed that the freedom from distractibility and the processing speed indices were significantly lower than the age norms, which supported a close connection between LI and working memory deficits. We found no significant difference between the verbal and performance IQs, neither on a group level nor on an individual level. Our results accord with findings by Tomblin and Zhang,Citation22 who demonstrated that language impaired children, whether with a performance IQ below or above 85, had the same patterns of language deficits and that there was very little evidence to suggest the inclusion of a performance IQ criterion in the clinical diagnosis of developmental language impairment.

Nine of the 23 individuals had ASD or clear autistic traits, which in many cases were combined with ID and/or concomitant attention deficits. Definite attention problems or ADHD were evident in approximately one-fourth of the group, when subthreshold ADHD was taken into account.Citation21 The difficulties in differentiating between ADHD, ASD, ID, BIF, and LI in young children were clearly demonstrated by the shifts in diagnostic categories between the two assessments.

Hence, the clinical picture obtained for this group of children at preschool age and from a follow-up conducted in their mid-teens was in strong accordance with the concept of the Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Early Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examination (ESSENCE).Citation23 This concept highlights the evidence that young children with developmental problems should always be assessed with a broad view to identify disorders in different developmental domains. The concept also highlights the importance of always planning for a follow-up of children with developmental deviations in order to identify changes that may occur in a child’s developmental profile.

One limitation of the study presented here is that it could only encompass the 23 children who were eligible for a follow-up in their teens.Citation11 Another limitation is that only 15 and 13 of the 23 adolescents participated in the direct speech and language and reading assessment and the cognitive assessment, respectively. However, this was partly compensated for by the information obtained from other assessments and the information that the parents provided.

The study presented here has confirmed earlier findings that language impairment is usually part of a larger developmental disability. Furthermore, speech and language development is a useful indicator of a child’s overall development and cognitive ability and is related to school success.Citation24 Children with language impairment should be provided with a range of clinical and educational services adapted to their special needs in order to optimize their development.Citation25

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all the parents and teenagers for their participation in the study. Financial support was obtained from the Center for Competence in Care (CKVO) at Stockholm University and from Odd Fellows.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • MiniscalcoCNygrenGHagbergBKadesjöBGillbergCNeuropsychiatric and neurodevelopmental outcome of children at age 6 and 7 years who screened positive for language problems at 30 monthsDev Med Child Neurol20064836136616608544
  • SnowlingMJBishopDVStothardSEChipchaseBKaplanCPsychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a preschool history of speech-language impairmentJ Child Psychol Psychiatry20064775976516898989
  • GathercoleSEBaddeleyADPhonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection?J Mem Lang199029336360
  • GathercoleSEBaddeleyAWorking Memory and LanguageHove, UKErlbaum1993
  • Im-BolterNJohnsonJPascual-LeoneJProcessing limitations in children with specific language impairment: the role of executive functionChild Dev2006771822184117107463
  • KailRA method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairmentJ Speech Hear Res1994374184218028323
  • LeonardLBWeismerSEMillerCAFrancisDJTomblinJBKailRVSpeed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in childrenJ Speech Lang Hear R200750408428
  • JustMACarpenterPAA capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memoryPsychol Rev1992991221491546114
  • BaddeleyAGathercoleSPapagnoCThe phonological loop as a language learning devicePsychol Rev19981051581739450375
  • HappéFBoothRCharltonRHughesCExecutive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and agesBrain Cogn200661253916682102
  • FernellENorrelgenFBozkurtIHellbergGLöwingKDevelopmental profiles and auditory perception in 25 children attending special preschools for language-impaired childrenActa Paediatr2002911108111512434898
  • American Psychiatric Association (APA)Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders4th ed (DSM-IV)Washington, DCAmerican Psychiatric Association1994
  • WechslerDWechsler Intelligence Scale for Children3rd edNew York, NYPsychological Corporation1999
  • RoidGHMillerLJLeiter International Performance Scale, Revised (standardized edition)Wood Dale, ILStoetling Co1997
  • HøienTLogoshandbok: Diagnostisering av dyslexi och andra lässvårigheterBryne, SwedenLogometrica AS2007 Swedish
  • JärpstenBHandledning för Klass 2 och 3Stockholm, SwedenPsykologiförlaget AB1999 Swedish
  • JärpstenBDLSM Handledning för Skolår 7–9 och år 1 gymnasietStockholm, SwedenPsykologiförlaget AB2002 Swedish
  • GoodglassHKaplanEWeintraubSBoston Naming Test Scoring BookletPhiladelphia, PALea and Febiger1983
  • BrusewitzKGómez-OrtegaMBoston naming test in Swedish [MA thesis]Stockholm, SwedenKarolinska Institutet2005
  • BishopDVMTest for Reception of Grammar Version 2 (Trog-2)Stockholm, Sweden08tryck AB2009
  • American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP)Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders for Primary CareWashington, DCAmerican Academy of Pediatrics1997
  • TomblinJBZhangXAre children with SLI a unique group of language learners?Tager-FlusbergHNeurodevelopmental Disorders: Contributions to a new framework from the cognitive neurosciencesCambridge, MAMIT Press1999361382
  • GillbergCThe ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical ExaminationsRes Dev Disabil2010311543155120634041
  • NelsonHDNygrenPWalkerMPanoschaRScreening for speech and language delay in preschool children: systematic evidence review for the US Preventive Services Task ForcePediatrics2006117e298e31916452337
  • TomblinJBValidating diagnostic standards for specific language impairment using adolescent outcomesNorburyCFTomblinJBBishopDVMUnderstanding Developmental Language Disorders: From theory to practiceHove, UKPsychology Press Ltd200893114