348
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
REVIEW

Depression and Cognitive Impairment: Current Understanding of Its Neurobiology and Diagnosis

ORCID Icon, , , , &
Pages 2783-2794 | Received 12 Sep 2022, Accepted 15 Nov 2022, Published online: 29 Nov 2022

Abstract

Background

Eye movement is critical for obtaining precise visual information and providing sensorimotor processes and advanced cognitive functions to the brain behavioral indicator.

Methods

In this article, we present a narrative review of the eye-movement paradigms (such as fixation, smooth pursuit eye movements, and memory-guided saccade tasks) in major depression.

Results

Characteristics of eye movement are considered to reflect several aspects of cognitive deficits regarded as an aid to diagnosis. Findings regarding depressive disorders showed differences from the healthy population in paradigms, the characteristics of eye movement may reflect cognitive deficits in depression. Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of different eye movement paradigms for MDD screening.

Conclusion

Depression can be distinguished from other mental illnesses based on eye movements. Eye movement reflects cognitive deficits that can help diagnose depression, and it can make the entire diagnostic process more accurate.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a psychological disorder characterized by depression lasting longer than 2 weeks, along with emotional pain, dysfunction, health issues, suicide, and other conditions. MDD is a prominent cause of psychiatric disability as well as a significant financial burden.Citation1 There is no objective biomarker that detects serious depression in the clinical environment, despite physiology and neuroimaging research demonstrating modifications in people with depression.

Cognitive impairment is a common and typically persistent primary depression symptom, accounting for a disproportionately high percentage of patients who have not completely healed their psychosocial function.Citation2 According to the Research Domain Criteria (DOC), cognitive impairment is among the most prominent components of MDD.Citation3 However, the typical cognitive symptoms of people with depression are fewer than those of people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder.Citation4,Citation5 Cohort studies have shown that cognitive impairments such as inattention, poor memory, and decision-making difficulties are common symptoms of patients with major depression.Citation3,Citation6 Valid and effective cognitive dysfunction screening techniques are critical for detecting and treating cognitive dysfunction in MDD.Citation7 Existing clinical interviews are subjective evaluations of cognitive function (eg, COBRA/strengths and difficulties questionnaire), which are affected by feelings and emotions and are not as stable as objective tests. According to the literature, cognitive dysfunction can have a negative impact on organizational, occupational, and social functions.Citation8,Citation9 In addition, cognitive recovery is increasingly becoming a major treatment goal for patients with psychiatric disorders, particularly MDD.Citation10 Therefore, it is imperative to study cognitive function in depression.

The evaluation of neurological data yielded valuable information for the successful screening and assessment of MDD’s cognitive symptoms. Cerebellar-cerebral dynamic FC of the cerebellar subregions communicating with the executive, default-mode, and affective-limbic networks, for example, was impaired in individuals with MDD, according to earlier results.Citation11,Citation12 EEG data report state and trait abnormalities in resting-state brain activity in MDD.Citation13 In functional near-infrared spectroscopy(fNIRs), reduced Hb changes during cognitive engagement have been identified as potential biomarkers for depressive disorders.Citation14 The neurological study of depression is still at the stage of scientific research and is not widely used clinically due to factors such as its high cost and interference from external factors. It is not known whether the accuracy of MRI and fNIRs in the population is consistent with the experimental population. To summarize, identifying cognitive function in people with depression necessitates more sensitive methods than those now available. Eye movement has received much interest recently in the field of neuroimaging.

Eye movement is critical for obtaining precise visual information and providing sensorimotor processes and advanced cognitive functions to the brain behavioral indicator.Citation15 In 1908, Diefendorf and Dodge were the first to describe the smooth pursuit eye movement characteristics of participants with “dementia praecox” and “bipolar disorder”.Citation16 Scholars have found that abnormal eye movements may be a reliable biomarker for schizophrenia.Citation17 Similar to schizophrenia, patients with depression also have cognitive impairment. Researchers have started to pay more attention to eye movement in people with mental illnesses. It has been suggested that the abnormalities of the oculomotor system seen in patients with affective disorders may not be different from other psychomotor disorders.Citation18,Citation19 However, Carvalho came to a different conclusion that eye movement paradigm tests can assist in distinguishing depression from bipolar disorder.Citation20 The reliability of using eye movements to study cortical mechanisms of cognitive function as a new approach for studying advanced cognitive functions is unclear yet.Citation21

The basic features of eye movement include fixation, smooth pursuit eye movement, saccadics. Scholars discovered that scan path length was substantially connected with cognitive measures of MDD in recent eye movement research.Citation22 The study found no effect of the study drug on eye movements in patients with psychotic affective disorder. This information aids in the detection of impaired eye function and the provision of suitable therapy in cognitive studies.Citation23

In this paper, our aim was to review previous clinical studies, reviews, and meta-analyses on saccadic eye movement paradigms (including fixation, smooth pursuit eye movements, memory-guided saccade task and free viewing task) in major depressive disorder. To summarize a narrative review of the application of the eye movement features in depression. In each paradigm, our current research is divided into two categories: features of eye movement paradigms in depression and neurological mechanisms. Then, we provide perspectives for future research.

Fixation

A fixational eye movement occurs when a subject attempts to control their gaze within a restricted space. A person’s eye movements during fixation are caused by the activity of the vestibular and visual compensatory systems. Fixational eye movement can be divided into three categories: microsaccades, ocular drift, and tremor.Citation24

The concept of ocular drift is commonly believed to involve a very slight and very slow movement of the eyes. A typical classical study reported amplitude values ranging between 1.5 and 4 feet, and median velocities of approximately 4 feet per second.Citation25 We generally call the two maintained fixation. Since there is a small difference in drift and tremor, there is less eye movement and current acquisition equipment, fewer studies have been conducted. The occurrence of drifts during fixation selectively activates V1 neurons.Citation26

Microsaccades are miniature replications of the rapid gaze shifts that humans use to explore visual environments (saccades). Studies on maintaining fixation have shown that saccades exceed 12–15 minutes of microsaccades in size. Microsaccades may serve the function of bringing the eye to a series of locations of interest in a similar manner to a larger saccades.Citation27 According to some researchers, microsaccades is associated with attentional shifts, The oculomotor system is subconsciously activated by covert attention in microphthalmoplegia.Citation28 As microsaccades provide a continuous, online measurement of the oculomotor system, they can provide insight into a wide range of cognitive functions, for example, spatial attention,Citation29 temporal attentionCitation30 and working memory.Citation31 Microsaccades are generated by the superior colliculus (SC), a retinotopically organized structure involved in voluntary saccade target selection.Citation32,Citation33

During the resting stability test, TakahashiCitation34 found no significant differences between the depressed and healthy control groups. However, sacral amplitudes were found to be greater in depressed patients by.Citation35 Similarly, Li agrees that there are differences in the fixation task between depression and normal individuals. Compared to healthy controls, patients with depressive disorder showed more fixations, shorter fixation durations, more microsaccades and longer saccade lengthsCitation22. Among populations with mental disorders, fixed paradigm experiments have been studied less extensively.

As physical research techniques are developed, a greater number of studies are expected to reveal 1. Why do fixed eye jumps occur? 2. Are fixational eye jumps characterized differently by different mental disorders? 3. What’s the connection between fixation gaze and attentional bias?

Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements

Since the rediscovery of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia, psychiatry has been researching eye movement.Citation36 The smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEMs) task is a test of the central vestibular system that assesses the patient’s ability to accurately track visual targets in a smooth, controlled manner.Citation37 The SPEMs task assesses depression’s cognitive control, influenced by the attention process.Citation38,Citation39

SPEMs occur in relative motion between the eyes and the observed object, maintaining the eyes fixed on the object and the eyeball following the object’s movement. Areas in the cerebral cortex, such as the middle temporal lobe area and the right temporoparietal junction area, may control SPEMs.Citation40 SPEMs are controlled by a cerebro-pontocerebellar pathway that bypasses the longer-standing centers for saccades in the brainstem tegmentum.Citation41 The frontal eye field (FEF), middle superior temporal area (MlTl), lateral intraparietal cortex, dorsolateral pontine nucleus, and other cortical regions associated with eye movement have been discovered in previous neurophysiology and brain imaging studies in monkeys and humans.Citation42–44 The pursuit characterization of FEF with microstimulation has been linked to the tracking eye movement gain setting for the pursuit eye movement in monkeys.Citation45 The middle temporal area serves as a global motion processor, assisting target movement and causing reverse smooth tracking errors.Citation41 The cerebellum creates SPEMs by sending messages from these cerebral cortexes to the brain. In addition to pursuit-related neurons, the lateral intraparietalCitation46 and ventral intraparietalCitation47,Citation48 areas have also been investigated. Signals related to SPEMs have been observed in the intermediate and medial parts of the dorsal pontine nucleus.Citation49

MRI revealed that multiple brain regions are significantly impaired in MDD patients. Gray matter changes in the frontal lobe, thalamus, and temporal lobes (eg, the hippocampus and amygdala) correspond to eye movements.Citation50 Furthermore, aberrant brain activity in the prefrontal cortex was discovered.Citation51 There is a neurological basis allowing for speculation about whether changes in depressed brain regions cause changes in SPEMs. Coincidentally, after analyzing the effects of electroconvulsive treatment (ECT) on SPEMs in severe MDD, Malaspina et al concluded that SPEM abnormalities might be a state marker in severe MDD.Citation52

Thirty patients with major depression were evaluated three times for smooth pursuit eye movement during a four-week therapeutic study, and most key indicators were unaffected by neuroleptic medication or clinical conditions.Citation17 Thus, SPEMs are considered one of the stable features of eye movement in depression. In the SPEMs, the significant eye movement characteristics in depression are the duration of saccades, peak saccade velocityCitation34 and decreased gain.Citation53 When compared to the healthy controls (HCs), the SPEMs in patients with depression have a high peak speed. Takahashi argues that peak saccade velocity in the SPEMs task is one of the potential biomarkers to distinguish between MDD patients and HCs. Research has shown that peak saccade velocity is 72.1% accurate in discriminating between MDD patients and HCs,Citation34 and the ROC curve indicates that this feature could be used to distinguish MDD patients with moderate accuracy. A study showed more fixation numbers and shorter fixation durations in MDD patients.Citation54 These findings support the concept that SPEMs are a “state marker in severe major depression”.Citation52

However, a recent study also concluded that patients with major depressive disorder, bipolar depression, and mania have similar oculomotor dysfunction in the SPEMs task.Citation54 Whether there are differences in the eye-movement characteristics of depression needs to be further explored.

Saccadics

Saccadic vision is a rapid gaze shift in which the point in the center of the retina is relocated to a new object in the visual scene to achieve accurate and clear vision. Participants in the prosaccade tasks were instructed to pay attention to new stimuli in their environment. In the antisaccade tasks, the subject looks at a fixation point and upon presentation of a visual target, participants were asked to ignore the target and look in the opposite direction.Citation55 In view of the fact that forward prosaccade is often compared with antisaccade tasks, they will be discussed together.

The saccadic network in marmoset monkeys was investigated utilizing task-based fMRI, which revealed that the task elicited a strong visual-saccadic topology.Citation56 There are several key distinctions between saccades and smooth pursuit eye movement. Both types of eye movements are regulated by neural physiological levels and two types of eye movements.Citation41,Citation57 Saccadic eye movements, like smooth pursuit eye movements, are linked to the FEF, which is responsible for coding and triggering the rhythm.Citation58 When deliberately or reflexively glancing around a room, large motions occur. Furthermore, the velocity of saccades is unaffected by deliberate effort or experience.Citation59 The initiation of the saccade, a condensing cognitive control, is subject to the actions of heteromodal neocortical regions, which are aided by other brain regions, additional neural regions, the striatum, and more typical sensorimotor regions.Citation60

Saccades engage a core eye-movement network, including the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), FEFs, supplementary eye fields (SEFs), basal ganglia, the brainstem (eg, superior colliculus), and the cerebellum.Citation61 The malfunctioning neocortical area, other nerve areas, the striatum, and more conventional sensorimotor areas all contribute to the start of saccades. During both comparisons, patients had longer antisaccade latencies and lower antisaccade peak velocities compared to 60 healthy controls (who were matched the precuneus, supplementary eye fields (SEF), and FEF showed anti-saccade-related activity, while the prefrontal cortex (PFC) showed anti-saccade-related activity during a single task comparison.Citation61 FEF activity was related to whether the response to cognitive inhibition was effective, and it plays a critical role in the cortical oculomotor network via mediating visuomotor transformations for controlling saccadic eye movements and spatial attention orientation.Citation62,Citation63 The superior colliculus (SC) is a brainstem center of saccade control.Citation64 Between the FEFs and the SC, there are two signal transmission channels. The direct signaling of FEFs to the SC is one example. Another option is to take a circuitous route through the basal ganglia. The indirect pathway is through the substantia nigra, which suppresses SC activity.Citation65 In contrast to SEF and FEF activation, in the process of antisaccade tasks, activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is involved as an inhibitory component, according to neurophysiology and human lesion evidence.Citation60 Observation of a patient with DLPFC lesions demonstrated that some DLPFC neurons activate exclusively to anti-cues, with high rates of failure to suppress saccades to targets.Citation65 Various approaches have been proposed, and MDD has been linked to increased inhibitory control and reduced motor activity. Increased impulsivity, on the other hand, has also been noted.Citation66 As a result, researchers associate SEF, FEF, and DLPFC with depression and explore their correlation. An increase in right dominant blood flow occurred with prosaccade and anti-eye skipping tests. Patients with MDD had a smaller increase in right dominant blood flow than controls. Patients had a higher error rate than controls in the anti-eye jump test but not in the prosaccade test. The smaller blood flow response may reflect the expected diminished activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices in MDD. In addition, patients with MDD showed an increase in right dominant blood flow during the prosaccade and antisaccade tests, but this increase was smaller than that in controls. This phenomenon may reflect diminished anticipatory activation in dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices in MDD.Citation67

Researchers discovered that 60 MDD patients had longer antisaccade latencies and lower antisaccade peak velocities than 60 HCs (who were matched by gender, age, and years of education).Citation22 In the experiment, participants first fixed their eyes on the black fixation point “+” in the center of the white screen. Participants were asked to gaze at the stimulus (a solid or hollow dot) as fast and efficiently as possible (if the motivation was a solid dot) or to look at the opposite side (if the stimulus was a hollow dot) when the target stimulation appeared. Patients with major depression had significantly longer antisaccade latencies and lower peak antisaccade velocities, implying that aberrant antisaccade brain circuits resulted in decreased eye movement inhibitory functioning. This indicates damage to cognitive function, which is thought to be a sign of depressive disorder.Citation68,Citation69 FEF activity corresponds to the success or failure of the response to cognitive inhibition and plays a crucial role in the cortical oculomotor network in mediating visuomotor transformations for managing saccadic eye movements and in the orientation of spatial attention.Citation62,Citation63 Patients had significantly higher reaction times (RTs) and increased rates of response suppression errorsCitation70 (Mahlberg et al, 2001, Sweeney et al, 1998); thus, patient groups needed more corrective saccades to reach the target than controls (Mahlberg et al, 2001, Sweeney et al, 1998). Depressed patients also have a higher rate of anti-eye darting errors.Citation67 These findings support the idea of active control impairment in MDD. There are conflicting views on research on eye darting. CarvalhoCitation70 discovered that depression lengthens the latent duration of the pro-saccade task, but LiCitation22 suggested that prosaccade amplitude correlates with anxiety symptoms, not depression symptoms. The error rate on the pro-saccade task in depressed patients is similar to that of healthy people.Citation67

A recent study also found that MDD patients had a longer duration of saccades.Citation34,Citation71 The anomalous secular organization of thinking and behavior that was reduced in MDD could be explained by temporal preparation mechanisms.Citation66 Compared to depression patients, depressed patients with suicidal behaviors exhibited fewer errors and a longer time to correct them.Citation72 These findings give credence to the theory that saccades can represent a reduced cognitive function in depression. Furthermore, in addition to being an assessment tool, some researchers have begun to evaluate the possibility of predicting different treatment responses based on AS parameters.

Free Viewing Task

We discovered that depressive patients frequently have difficulties understanding facial emotional expressions and have biased facial emotion detection.Citation73,Citation74 Excessive attention to negative information is one of the hallmarks of depressed cognition.Citation75 In order to benefit the patients with depressive disorder, the researchers developed the free viewing task by taking advantage of the skipping-eye feature.

Free-viewing paradigms are generally used to detect attentional bias and allow the patient see pictures presented on the computer screen, which displays sad, angry, joyful, and neutral expressions simultaneously. The amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, primate prefrontal cortex (PFC), and anterior cingulate cortex are consistently activated during emotional face processing.Citation76,Citation77 Patients with lesions in both the orbital and medial PFC demonstrate a range of social cognition characteristics.Citation78 The term “vmPFC” refers to an interconnected regional network in the lower medial and orbital prefrontal cortex.Citation79,Citation80 During the active regulation of negative emotions, a smaller area in the posterior ventral vmPFC and the left amygdala displayed convergent activity.Citation81 Furthermore, investigations have demonstrated that the amygdala is linked to the medial, lateral (middle frontal and inferior gyrus), and orbitofrontal/ventromedial regions of the PFC.Citation82 A monkey study found that amygdala lesions could cause improper social and emotional behaviors in a context-inappropriate manner.Citation83 In depressed individuals, the amygdala may respond to both positive and negative emotions.Citation84 When processing fearful faces, higher levels of depression were associated with decreased left putamen–right amygdala functional connectivity.Citation85

Free viewing eye-movement studies of depression in emotion recognition are consistent with neurological findings. Depression leads to persistent attention to unfavorable information during development and maintenance.Citation86 Compared to HCs, depressed individuals are generally less focused on the eyes and mouth, representing an avoidance of facial features.Citation87 In neurology, researchers found that the severity of depression was associated with increased brain activity in mood control and emotion-sensitive areas.Citation88 A study found that the total dwell times and fixation on threatening, sad, and optimistic naturalistic face images revealed good to exceptional reliability for attentional biases for dynamic images.Citation89 Positive attentional bias drastically decreases regardless of age, but negative attentional bias increases.Citation89,Citation90 A study investigated the emotional information recognition characteristics of patients with depression; eye movements showed that they were slower to detect positive emotion.Citation85 Patients with MDD spent longer on sad faces in an eye movement test with equally divided viewing periods between happy and neutral faces.Citation90,Citation91 Another result showed longer duration times for null events and saccades and no significant difference between the time spent on different emotional expressions.Citation92 However, it has been suggested that depressed and healthy individuals show a high degree of similarity when visually searching for clusters of facial emotional expressions; there is no difference in reaction time to all emotional target faces.Citation93

Attentional bias toward happy faces remained reduced, and there was still a deficit in positive attentional bias during depression remission.Citation94,Citation95 However, there are different views on whether the negative attentional bias of depressed patients is improved. LiCitation94 thought there were no significant differences in first fixation location and initial attentional maintenance to face pictures; nevertheless, IsaacCitation95 found remitted depression patients to have longer fixation times for all emotional expressions compared to HCs. More research is needed to determine whether a reduction in processing positive information is associated with a history of depression and whether it can be a basis for assessing the risk of relapse and continuation of treatment for depression.

Emotion recognition bias is linked to an enlarged DLPFC and deactivated amygdala.Citation96 According to the available neuroimaging evidence, the phenomena during emotional processing comprise general striatal and limbic modifications as well as particular, disorder-specific alterations in the prefrontal areas.Citation97 Researchers verify that depression can re-emerge through neurological changes reflected in the appearance of negative attentional bias.Citation98 In recent years, it has also been found that eye tracking training can be an important therapeutic target by enhancing cognitive function in patients with depressionCitation99 and changing the patient’s mood.Citation99 PolettiCitation100 found that the attention bias in depression can be changed by reward-based eye movement training. The rapid improvement in positive attention bias can predict the later treatment effect of clinical depression reduction. Eye tracking tests are used to determine whether selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) medications may reduce depression’s negative attention bias and generate therapeutic responses. Patients demonstrated an increased tendency to concentrate on happy images and a decreased tendency to concentrate on negative images after eight weeks of SSRI medication.Citation12

In summary, the free-viewing eye movement experimental paradigm is of great interest both in the detection and treatment of cognitive impairment in depression and needs to be explored in greater depth by researchers.

Memory-Guided Saccade Task

Moving the eye to a memory region that flashes momentarily after a delay is named the memory-guided saccade (MGS) task. Participants are required to keep their eyes on the central gaze point. During this time, the target appears outside the center gaze point, and participants are not allowed to stray to the target. After the central gaze point disappears, the subject needs to scan to the location just remembered. This is a delayed oculomotor response task that has been widely utilized in primates to explore spatial short-term memory.Citation101 Specific characteristics of MGS might depend on whether the subject has prematurely erroneous saccades toward the stimulus, a so-called disinhibition error.Citation102

The memory-guided saccade is seen as a full change. The task-dependent timing of this transformation process throughout a memory delay between vision and action was shown by a new set of spatial models (from target to future gaze coding).Citation103 The frontal eye field is essential for transitioning from perception to movement.Citation104 Researchers started a new study to provide first-hand knowledge about how neurons interact and cooperate. According to one theory, one possible interpretation is that when motor neurons become active after the memory delay, other neurons with mixed coding on behavior (perhaps through selective gating) have less influence over behavior. As a result, there is a stronger link between their firing rate and gaze mistakes.Citation105

When two male cynomolgus macaques (Macaca fascicularis) performed a long-duration memory-guided saccade task, local field potentials were recorded. Frontal memory circuits synchronized in response to stimulus presentation and memory encoding; however, they gradually desynchronized to below the baseline level over a long memory period.Citation106 The dorsal pulvinar in spatial coordinate transformationsCitation107 and the mesial temporal lobe are associated with the onset of visual stimuli (image onset).Citation108 The intermediate layers of the SC integrate inputs from visual areas, frontal-parietal regions, and basal ganglia and project directly to the premotor circuit in the brainstem to initiate the orienting response, including not only shifts of attention and gaze but also pupil dilation.Citation109 Chin-WangCitation110 found that pupil constriction occured significantly after the presentation of the bright patch compared to the dark patch at the spatial location of the target in MGS.

Memory saccades are also being explored more extensively in other disorders. In patients with X-linked dystonia-parkinsonism, the amplitude of memory-guided saccades was reduced, and the latency was prolonged.Citation111 Patients with cerebellar dysfunction in clinically isolated syndrome had worse MGS latencies and mistake rates and had lower working memory, executive function, and information processing speed z scores than patients without cerebellar dysfunction in a clinically isolated condition.Citation112 On the memory-guided saccade task, depressed patients showed an increased error in their memory for spatial location information, which implies inadequate spatial working memory.Citation71 However, more studies on memory saccades in people suffering from depression are needed to verify this result.

The Integeration of the Multiple Modalities Model

The role of eye movement in diagnosis still needs to be explored. According to a depression experiment, the scan path length of the free-viewing test and peak saccade velocity of the smooth pursuit test resulted in 81% sensitivity and 69% specificity.Citation34 Others believe that compared with MRI, eye movement measurements are not specific to major depressive disorders. Research has shown that patients with depression and bipolar disorder or schizophrenia have varied preferences in free-view tasks.Citation76,Citation113 Eye movement as a diagnostic tool is still problematic due to tiny sample numbers.

Researchers have combined several biomarkers and classifiers with machine learning algorithms to boost diagnostic accuracy. A content ensemble method based on electroencephalogram (EEG) and eyes movement data has been proposed based on a cross-sectional study. The static and dynamic ensemble methods attain 82.5% and 92.65% accuracy, respectively.Citation114 Another study added galvanic skin reaction, and the highest classification score was obtained by logistic regression algorithms, with an accuracy of 79.63%.Citation115 Low-level eye movement characteristics using a hybrid classifier with 70% accuracy and 75% accuracy were obtained using statistical metrics such as Gaussian Mixture Models and Support Vector Machines.Citation116 Thus, the integration of multiple modalities would improve the classification models’ performance.

However, the combination of functional near-infrared spectroscopy with eyes movement to evaluate cognitive function is still in the research stage. The two technologies were utilized to investigate behavioral and neurophysiological differences.Citation117–119 Multimodal analyses imply that the demand for increased attentional effort and alertness of visuomotor motor control causes activity in the right superior parietal lobule to rise, which is a good candidate for objectively measuring visuomotor cognitive load. This combined application is currently being used in research on the cognitive development of infants and young children, and there are numerous studies on teenage education.Citation120,Citation121 However, it has not been applied to the field of psychiatry. It may be feasible to distinguish between attention and cerebral blood flow in patients with mental disorders both at rest and when doing tasks. On the other hand, combining these methods can aid in the development of a multimodal diagnosis model for mental disease.

Event-related potential and eye movement technologies provide novel means to objectively document receptive language processing in people with neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism.Citation122 The results of one study showed that inconsistent speaker-meaning sentences generated a higher N400 than anomalous sentences. Eye movements showed an evident bias toward the voice-consistent object.Citation123 Another method analyzes fundamental visual and oculomotor processes by combining eye movement, neurophysiological assessment, and computer modeling. The sensory/eye movement and semantic level barriers of schizophrenia are reflected by the ability to read fluently, which appears as changes in saccade number and fixation duration. The outcome explains the cognitive impairment of schizophrenia.Citation124

More neuroimaging and genetic information are being included in diagnosis and prediction models to reduce erroneous diagnosis and ineffective treatment trials for depression. The current model integration type is limited to a single biological marker, and more biological markers need to be integrated.

Future Directions

This paper reviews the neurological basis of eye movements as an aid in the diagnosis of depression and addresses the current state of research on it. The majority of available eye movement paradigms studies conclude that depression differs from other psychiatric disorders in its oculomotor features, but it is not proven that these features can be used as diagnostic criteria. Future research should improve the eye-movement paradigm to make it relevant to depression based on neurobiological findings.

Eye movement has been a significant medical diagnostic tool for a long time. Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the efficacy of eye movement paradigms for MDD screening. Eye movement reflects cognitive function deficiencies that can aid in the diagnosis of depression, and it may make the whole diagnosis process more accurate, less cumbersome, and even concealed for the patient. Current research on eye movement in depression has concentrated primarily on antisaccade and free eye movement, and more diverse experimental paradigms should be explored to reflect cognitive impairment. More factors, such as disease subtypes, age and sex, also need to be explored and studied in a more detailed way. The combination of eye movement and other technologies to create multimodal models through algorithms is the general trend that will change current diagnostic methods.

Although a limited number of studies are already confirming this, more research is required to demonstrate the therapeutic feasibility of eye movement. This would be a major advancement in possible neurocognitive alteration of the brain through the eyes. Eye movement characteristics are associated with cognitive impairment in the depressive disorder. However, this still needs to be investigated in longitudinal studies. Eye movement has the advantages of safety, reliability, universality and low cost and will be widely used in clinical diagnosis in the future.

Limitaions

The field of eye movement is one of the most diverse and heterogeneous in the cognitive sciences. Current eye movement measures may not fully assess this neurocognitive domain. Additionally, these neuropsychological measures may not be ecologically valid. Our scoping review is likely generalizable to journal articles and systematic reviews acquired through the North China University of Technology Open Access Policy.

Author Contributions

All authors made a significant contribution to the work reported, whether that is in the conception, study design, execution, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation, or in all these areas; took part in drafting, revising or critically reviewing the article; gave final approval of the version to be published; have agreed on the journal to which the article has been submitted; and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Disclosure

The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest in this work.

Additional information

Funding

This research was supported by the Program of Medical Science Research of Hebei Province (20220700).

References

  • Collins PY, Insel TR, Chockalingam A, Daar A, Maddox YT. Grand challenges in global mental health: integration in research, policy, and practice. PLoS Med. 2013;10(4):e1001434. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001434
  • Culpepper L, Lam RW, McIntyre RS. Cognitive impairment in patients with depression: awareness, assessment, and management. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017;78(9):1383–1394. doi:10.4088/JCP.tk16043ah5c
  • Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Hasselbalch SG, Gade A, Kessing LV. Cognitive deficits in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2012;26(5):642–651. doi:10.1037/a0029301
  • Bora E, Yücel M, Pantelis C, Berk M. Meta-analytic review of neurocognition in bipolar II disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011;123(3):165–174. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01638.x
  • Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med. 2008;38(6):771–785. doi:10.1017/s0033291707001675
  • Nierenberg AA, Husain MM, Trivedi MH, et al. Residual symptoms after remission of major depressive disorder with citalopram and risk of relapse: a STAR*D report. Psychol Med. 2010;40(1):41–50. doi:10.1017/s0033291709006011
  • Zhang W, Zhu N, Lai J, et al. Reliability and validity of THINC-it in evaluating cognitive function of patients with bipolar depression. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2020;16:2419–2428. doi:10.2147/ndt.S266642
  • Knight MJ, Fourrier C, Lyrtzis E, et al. Cognitive deficits in the THINC-Integrated Tool (THINC-it) are associated with psychosocial dysfunction in patients with major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;80(1). doi:10.4088/JCP.18m12472
  • McIntyre RS, Subramaniapillai M, Park C, et al. The THINC-it tool for cognitive assessment and measurement in major depressive disorder: sensitivity to change. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:546. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00546
  • Russo M, Mahon K, Burdick KE. Measuring cognitive function in MDD: emerging assessment tools. Depress Anxiety. 2015;32(4):262–269. doi:10.1002/da.22297
  • Guo W, Liu F, Liu J, et al. Increased cerebellar-default-mode-network connectivity in drug-naive major depressive disorder at rest. Medicine. 2015;94(9):e560. doi:10.1097/md.0000000000000560
  • Zhang L, Yu F, Hu Q, et al. Effects of SSRI antidepressants on attentional bias toward emotional scenes in first-episode depressive patients: evidence from an eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Investig. 2020;17(9):871–879. doi:10.30773/pi.2019.0345
  • Murphy M, Whitton AE, Deccy S, et al. Abnormalities in electroencephalographic microstates are state and trait markers of major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(12):2030–2037. doi:10.1038/s41386-020-0749-1
  • Suto T, Fukuda M, Ito M, Uehara T, Mikuni M. Multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy in depression and schizophrenia: cognitive brain activation study. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(5):501–511. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.008
  • Broerse A, Crawford TJ, den Boer JA. Parsing cognition in schizophrenia using saccadic eye movements: a selective overview. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(7):742–756. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00155-x
  • Diefendorf AR, Dodge R. An experimental study of the ocular reactions of the insane from photographic records. Brain. 1908;31:451–489. doi:10.1093/brain/31.3.451
  • Brakemeier S, Sprenger A, Meyhöfer I, et al. Smooth pursuit eye movement deficits as a biomarker for psychotic features in bipolar disorder-Findings from the PARDIP study. Bipolar Disord. 2020;22(6):602–611. doi:10.1111/bdi.12865
  • Lipton RB, Levin S, Holzman PS. Horizontal and vertical pursuit eye movements, the oculocephalic reflex, and the functional psychoses. Psychiatry Res. 1980;3(2):193–203. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(80)90036-0
  • Abel LA, Friedman L, Jesberger J, Malki A, Meltzer HY. Quantitative assessment of smooth pursuit gain and catch-up saccades in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29(11):1063–1072. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(91)90248-K
  • Carvalho N, Laurent E, Noiret N, et al. Eye movement in unipolar and bipolar depression: a systematic review of the literature. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1809. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01809
  • Henderson JM, Shinkareva SV, Wang J, Luke SG, Olejarczyk J, Paterson K. Predicting cognitive state from eye movements. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e64937. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064937
  • Li Y, Xu Y, Xia M, et al. Eye movement indices in the study of depressive disorder. Shanghai Archiv Psychiatry. 2016;28(6):326–334. doi:10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.216078
  • Harezlak K, Kasprowski P. Application of eye tracking in medicine: a survey, research issues and challenges. Comput Med Imaging Graph. 2018;65:176–190. doi:10.1016/j.compmedimag.2017.04.006
  • Kowler E. Eye movements: the past 25 years. Vision Res. 2011;51(13):1457–1483. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.12.014
  • Cumming GD. Chapter 6 - eye movements and visual perception. In: Carterette EC, Friedman MP, editors. Perceptual Processing. Academic Press; 1978:221–255.
  • Snodderly DM. A physiological perspective on fixational eye movements. Vision Res. 2016;118:31–47. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.006
  • Steinman RM, Haddad GM, Skavenski AA, Wyman D. Miniature eye movement. Science. 1973;181(4102):810–819. doi:10.1126/science.181.4102.810
  • Hafed ZM, Clark JJ. Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Res. 2002;42(22):2533–2545. doi:10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00263-8
  • Xue C, Calapai A, Krumbiegel J, Treue S. Sustained spatial attention accounts for the direction bias of human microsaccades. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):20604. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-77455-7
  • Denison RN, Yuval-Greenberg S, Carrasco M. Directing voluntary temporal attention increases fixational stability. J Neurosci. 2019;39(2):353–363. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1926-18.2018
  • Willeke KF, Tian X, Buonocore A, Bellet J, Ramirez-Cardenas A, Hafed ZM. Memory-guided microsaccades. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):3710. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-11711-x
  • Khademi F, Chen CY, Hafed ZM. Visual feature tuning of superior colliculus neural reafferent responses after fixational microsaccades. J Neurophysiol. 2020;123(6):2136–2153. doi:10.1152/jn.00077.2020
  • Hafed ZM, Goffart L, Krauzlis RJ. A neural mechanism for microsaccade generation in the primate superior colliculus. Science. 2009;323(5916):940–943. doi:10.1126/science.1166112
  • Takahashi J, Hirano Y, Miura K, et al. Eye movement abnormalities in major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:673443. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.673443
  • Zhang D, Liu X, Xu L, et al. Effective differentiation between depressed patients and controls using discriminative eye movement features. J Affect Disord. 2022;307:237–243. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.03.077
  • Holzman PS, Proctor LR, Hughes DW. Eye-tracking patterns in schizophrenia. Science. 1973;181(4095):179–181. doi:10.1126/science.181.4095.179
  • Winograd-Gurvich C, Georgiou-Karistianis N, Fitzgerald PB, Millist L, White OB. Self-paced and reprogrammed saccades: differences between melancholic and non-melancholic depression. Neurosci Res. 2006;56(3):253–260. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2006.07.003
  • Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(3):218–228. doi:10.1038/nrn1345
  • Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WP, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004;56(2):129–140. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016
  • Haarmeier T, Kammer T. Effect of TMS on oculomotor behavior but not perceptual stability during smooth pursuit eye movements. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20(9):2234–2243. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp285
  • Thier P, Ilg UJ. The neural basis of smooth-pursuit eye movements. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15(6):645–652. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.013
  • Beauchamp MS, Petit L, Ellmore TM, Ingeholm J, Haxby JV. A parametric fMRI study of overt and covert shifts of visuospatial attention. NeuroImage. 2001;14(2):310–321. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0788
  • Ikkai A, Curtis CE. Cortical activity time locked to the shift and maintenance of spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18(6):1384–1394. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm171
  • de Haan B, Morgan PS, Rorden C. Covert orienting of attention and overt eye movements activate identical brain regions. Brain Res. 2008;1204:102–111. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.105
  • Tanaka M, Lisberger SG. Enhancement of multiple components of pursuit eye movement by microstimulation in the arcuate frontal pursuit area in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87(2):802–818. doi:10.1152/jn.00409.2001
  • Bremmer F, Distler C, Hoffmann KP. Eye position effects in monkey cortex. II. Pursuit- and fixation-related activity in posterior parietal areas LIP and 7A. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77(2):962–977. doi:10.1152/jn.1997.77.2.962
  • Colby CL, Duhamel JR, Goldberg ME. Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location and visual response properties. J Neurophysiol. 1993;69(3):902–914. doi:10.1152/jn.1993.69.3.902
  • Schlack A, Hoffmann KP, Bremmer F. Selectivity of macaque ventral intraparietal area (area VIP) for smooth pursuit eye movements. J Physiol. 2003;551(Pt 2):551–561. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2003.042994
  • Dicke PW, Barash S, Ilg UJ, Thier P. Single-neuron evidence for a contribution of the dorsal pontine nuclei to both types of target-directed eye movements, saccades and smooth-pursuit. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19(3):609–624. doi:10.1111/j.0953-816x.2003.03137.x
  • Peng W, Chen Z, Yin L, Jia Z, Gong Q. Essential brain structural alterations in major depressive disorder: a voxel-wise meta-analysis on first episode, medication-naive patients. J Affect Disord. 2016;199:114–123. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.001
  • Zhang FF, Peng W, Sweeney JA, Jia ZY, Gong QY. Brain structure alterations in depression: psychoradiological evidence. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2018;24(11):994–1003. doi:10.1111/cns.12835
  • Malaspina D, Amador XF, Coleman EA, Mayr TL, Friedman JH, Sackeim HA. Smooth pursuit eye movement abnormality in severe major depression: effects of ECT and clinical recovery. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6(1):36–42. doi:10.1176/jnp.6.1.36
  • Kathmann N, Hochrein A, Uwer R, Bondy B. Deficits in gain of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia and affective disorder patients and their unaffected relatives. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):696–702. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.696
  • Wang Y, Lyu HL, Tian XH, et al. The similar eye movement dysfunction between major depressive disorder, bipolar depression and bipolar mania. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2022:1–14. doi:10.1080/15622975.2022.2025616
  • García-Blanco AC, Perea M, Salmerón L. Attention orienting and inhibitory control across the different mood states in bipolar disorder: an emotional antisaccade task. Biol Psychol. 2013;94(3):556–561. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.005
  • Schaeffer DJ, Gilbert KM, Hori Y, et al. Task-based fMRI of a free-viewing visuo-saccadic network in the marmoset monkey. NeuroImage. 2019;202:116147. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116147
  • Krauzlis RJ. Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91(2):591–603. doi:10.1152/jn.00801.2003
  • Wardak C, Olivier E, Duhamel JR. The relationship between spatial attention and saccades in the frontoparietal network of the monkey. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33(11):1973–1981. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07710.x
  • Becker W, Fuchs AF. Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Res. 1969;9(10):1247–1258. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(69)90112-6
  • Sweeney JA, Luna B, Keedy SK, McDowell JE, Clementz BA. fMRI studies of eye movement control: investigating the interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor brain systems. NeuroImage. 2007;36(Suppl2):T54–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.018
  • Dyckman KA, Camchong J, Clementz BA, McDowell JE. An effect of context on saccade-related behavior and brain activity. NeuroImage. 2007;36(3):774–784. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.023
  • Lynch JC, Tian JR. Cortico-cortical networks and cortico-subcortical loops for the higher control of eye movements. Prog Brain Res. 2006;151:461–501. doi:10.1016/s0079-6123(05)51015-x
  • Makino Y, Yokosawa K, Takeda Y, Kumada T. Visual search and memory search engage extensive overlapping cerebral cortices: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;23(2):525–533. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.026
  • Krauzlis RJ, Lovejoy LP, Zénon A. Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2013;36:165–182. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170249
  • Morita K, Miura K, Kasai K, Hashimoto R. Eye movement characteristics in schizophrenia: a recent update with clinical implications. Neuropsychopharmacol rep. 2020;40(1):2–9. doi:10.1002/npr2.12087
  • Colantuoni C, Lipska BK, Ye T, et al. Temporal dynamics and genetic control of transcription in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature. 2011;478(7370):519–523. doi:10.1038/nature10524
  • Hoffmann A, Ettinger U, Montoro C, Reyes del paso GA, Duschek S. Cerebral blood flow responses during prosaccade and antisaccade preparation in major depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2019;269(7):813–822. doi:10.1007/s00406-018-0956-5
  • Richard-Devantoy S, Jollant F, Kefi Z, et al. Deficit of cognitive inhibition in depressed elderly: a neurocognitive marker of suicidal risk. J Affect Disord. 2012;140(2):193–199. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.006
  • Amador XF, Malaspina D, Sackeim HA, et al. Visual fixation and smooth pursuit eye movement abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;7(2):197–206. doi:10.1176/jnp.7.2.197
  • Carvalho N, Noiret N, Vandel P, Monnin J, Chopard G, Laurent E. Saccadic eye movements in depressed elderly patients. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e105355. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0105355
  • Sweeney JA, Strojwas MH, Mann JJ, Thase ME. Prefrontal and cerebellar abnormalities in major depression: evidence from oculomotor studies. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43(8):584–594. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00485-x
  • Barsznica Y, Noiret N, Lambert B, et al. Saccadic eye movements in elderly depressed patients with suicidal behaviors: an exploratory eye-tracking study. Front Psychol. 2021;12:712347. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712347
  • Bistricky SL, Ingram RE, Atchley RA. Facial affect processing and depression susceptibility: cognitive biases and cognitive neuroscience. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(6):998–1028. doi:10.1037/a0025348
  • Zwick JC, Wolkenstein L. Facial emotion recognition, theory of mind and the role of facial mimicry in depression. J Affect Disord. 2017;210:90–99. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.022
  • Strasburger LH. Depression—clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1968;30(6):890.
  • Sanchez A, Vazquez C, Marker C, LeMoult J, Joormann J. Attentional disengagement predicts stress recovery in depression: an eye-tracking study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(2):303–313. doi:10.1037/a0031529
  • Gao L, Cai Y, Wang H, Wang G, Zhang Q, Yan X. Probing prefrontal cortex hemodynamic alterations during facial emotion recognition for major depression disorder through functional near-infrared spectroscopy. J Neural Eng. 2019;16(2):026026. doi:10.1088/1741-2552/ab0093
  • Murray EA, Fellows LK. Prefrontal cortex interactions with the amygdala in primates. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):163–179. doi:10.1038/s41386-021-01128-w
  • Mackey S, Petrides M. Architecture and morphology of the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;40(5):2777–2796. doi:10.1111/ejn.12654
  • Hiser J, Koenigs M. The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83(8):638–647. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030
  • Yang M, Tsai SJ, Li CR. Concurrent amygdalar and ventromedial prefrontal cortical responses during emotion processing: a meta-analysis of the effects of valence of emotion and passive exposure versus active regulation. Brain Struct Funct. 2020;225(1):345–363. doi:10.1007/s00429-019-02007-3
  • Neubert FX, Mars RB, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Connectivity reveals relationship of brain areas for reward-guided learning and decision making in human and monkey frontal cortex. Proce Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112(20):E2695. doi:10.1073/pnas.1410767112
  • Emery NJ, Capitanio JP, Mason WA, Machado CJ, Mendoza SP, Amaral DG. The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci. 2001;115(3):515–544. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.115.3.515
  • García-García I, Kube J, Gaebler M, Horstmann A, Villringer A, Neumann J. Neural processing of negative emotional stimuli and the influence of age, sex and task-related characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;68:773–793. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.020
  • Akhapkin RV, Volel BA, Shishorin RM, Ustyuzhanin DV, Petelin DS. Recognition of facial emotion expressions in patients with depressive disorders: a prospective, observational study. Neurol ther. 2021;10(1):225–234. doi:10.1007/s40120-021-00231-w
  • Lazarov A, Ben-Zion Z, Shamai D, Pine DS, Bar-Haim Y. Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: a potential target for attention bias modification. J Affect Disord. 2018;238:94–100. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.047
  • Bodenschatz CM, Skopinceva M, Ruß T, Kersting A, Suslow T. Face perception without subjective awareness - Emotional expressions guide early gaze behavior in clinically depressed and healthy individuals. J Affect Disord. 2020;265:91–98. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.039
  • Luo L, Becker B, Zheng X, et al. A dimensional approach to determine common and specific neurofunctional markers for depression and social anxiety during emotional face processing. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(2):758–771. doi:10.1002/hbm.23880
  • Sears C, Quigley L, Fernandez A, Newman K, Dobson K. The reliability of attentional biases for emotional images measured using a free-viewing eye-tracking paradigm. Behav Res Methods. 2019;51(6):2748–2760. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-1147-z
  • Bodenschatz CM, Skopinceva M, Kersting A, Quirin M, Suslow T. Implicit negative affect predicts attention to sad faces beyond self-reported depressive symptoms in healthy individuals: an eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Res. 2018;265:48–54. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.007
  • Duque A, Vázquez C. Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: evidence from an eye-tracking study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2015;46:107–114. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.09.005
  • Figueiredo GR, Ripka WL, Romaneli EFR, Ulbricht L. Attentional bias for emotional faces in depressed and non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society Annual International Conference; 2019:5419–5422. doi:10.1109/embc.2019.8857878.
  • Bodenschatz CM, Czepluch F, Kersting A, Suslow T. Efficient visual search for facial emotions in patients with major depression. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):92. doi:10.1186/s12888-021-03093-6
  • Li M, Lu S, Wang G, Feng L, Fu B, Zhong N. Alleviated negative rather than positive attentional bias in patients with depression in remission: an eye-tracking study. J Int Med Res. 2016;44(5):1072–1086. doi:10.1177/0300060516662134
  • Isaac L, Vrijsen JN, Rinck M, Speckens A, Becker ES. Shorter gaze duration for happy faces in current but not remitted depression: evidence from eye movements. Psychiatry Res. 2014;218(1–2):79–86. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.002
  • Fales CL, Barch DM, Rundle MM, et al. Altered emotional interference processing in affective and cognitive-control brain circuitry in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(4):377–384. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.012
  • Groenewold NA, Opmeer EM, de Jonge P, Aleman A, Costafreda SG. Emotional valence modulates brain functional abnormalities in depression: evidence from a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(2):152–163. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.015
  • Disner SG, Shumake JD, Beevers CG. Self-referential schemas and attentional bias predict severity and naturalistic course of depression symptoms. Cogn Emot. 2017;31(4):632–644. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1146123
  • Woolridge SM, Harrison GW, Best MW, Bowie CR. Attention bias modification in depression: a randomized trial using a novel, reward-based, eye-tracking approach. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2021;71:101621. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2020.101621
  • Poletti B, Carelli L, Solca F, et al. An eye-tracking controlled neuropsychological battery for cognitive assessment in neurological diseases. Neurol Sci. 2017;38(4):595–603. doi:10.1007/s10072-016-2807-3
  • Goldman-Rakic PS. Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(24):13473–13480. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.24.13473
  • Landgraf S, Amado I, Bourdel MC, Leonardi S, Krebs MO. Memory-guided saccade abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and their healthy, full biological siblings. Psychol Med. 2008;38(6):861–870. doi:10.1017/s0033291707001912
  • Sajad A, Sadeh M, Crawford JD. Spatiotemporal transformations for gaze control. Physiol Rep. 2020;8(16):e14533. doi:10.14814/phy2.14533
  • Khanna SB, Snyder AC, Smith MA. Distinct sources of variability affect eye movement preparation. J Neurosci. 2019;39(23):4511–4526. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2329-18.2019
  • Sadeh M, Sajad A, Wang H, Yan X, Crawford JD. The influence of a memory delay on spatial coding in the superior colliculus: is visual always visual and motor always motor? Front Neural Circuits. 2018;12:74. doi:10.3389/fncir.2018.00074
  • Holmes CD, Papadimitriou C, Snyder LH. Dissociation of LFP power and tuning in the frontal cortex during memory. J Neurosci. 2018;38(38):8177–8186. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3629-17.2018
  • Schneider L, Dominguez-Vargas AU, Gibson L, Kagan I, Wilke M. Eye position signals in the dorsal pulvinar during fixation and goal-directed saccades. J Neurophysiol. 2020;123(1):367–391. doi:10.1152/jn.00432.2019
  • Katz CN, Patel K, Talakoub O, Groppe D, Hoffman K, Valiante TA. Differential generation of saccade, fixation, and image-onset event-related potentials in the human mesial temporal lobe. Cerebral Cortex. 2020;30(10):5502–5516. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhaa132
  • Black DW, Grant JE. DSM-5TM Guidebook: The Essential Companion to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Pub; 2015:335–340.
  • Wang CA, Huang J, Yep R, Munoz DP. Comparing pupil light response modulation between saccade planning and working memory. J Cognit. 2018;1(1):33. doi:10.5334/joc.33
  • Sprenger A, Hanssen H, Hagedorn I, et al. Eye movement deficits in X-linked dystonia-parkinsonism are related to striatal degeneration. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2019;61:170–178. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.10.016
  • Moroso A, Ruet A, Lamargue-Hamel D, et al. Preliminary evidence of the cerebellar role on cognitive performances in clinically isolated syndrome. J Neurol Sci. 2018;385:1–6. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2017.11.037
  • García-Blanco A, Salmerón L, Perea M, Livianos L. Attentional biases toward emotional images in the different episodes of bipolar disorder: an eye-tracking study. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(3):628–633. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.039
  • Zhu J, Wang Z, Gong T, et al. An improved classification model for depression detection using EEG and eye tracking data. IEEE Trans Nanobioscience. 2020;19(3):527–537. doi:10.1109/TNB.2020.2990690
  • Ding X, Yue X, Zheng R, Bi C, Li D, Yao G. Classifying major depression patients and healthy controls using EEG, eye tracking and galvanic skin response data. J Affect Disord. 2019;251:156–161. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.058
  • Alghowinem S, Goecke R, Wagner M, Parker G, Breakspear M. Eye movement analysis for depression detection. IEEE Int Conf Image Process. 2013:4220–4224. doi:10.1109/ICIP.2013.6738869
  • Işbilir E, Çakır M, Acartürk C, Neuroscience AT. Towards a multimodal model of cognitive workload through synchronous optical brain imaging and eye tracking measures. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:375. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2019.00375
  • Li H, Hsueh Y, Yu H, Kitzmann KM. viewing fantastical events in animated television shows: immediate effects on Chinese preschoolers’ executive function. Front Psychol. 2020;11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.583174
  • Rca B, Xian ZC, Jan D, It E, Afdch A, Jhcd E. Facial and neural mechanisms during interactive disclosure of biographical information. NeuroImage. 2020;226:117572.
  • Grossmann T, Missana M, Krol KM, Dehaene-Lambertz G. The neurodevelopmental precursors of altruistic behavior in infancy. PLoS Biol. 2018;16(9):e2005281. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2005281
  • Brockington G, Balardin JB, Morais GZ, et al. From the laboratory to the classroom: the potential of functional near-infrared spectroscopy in educational neuroscience. Front Psychol. 2018;9. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01840
  • Key A, Venker C, Sandbank M. Psychophysiological and eye-tracking markers of speech and language processing in neurodevelopmental disorders: new options for difficult-to-test populations. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2020;125(6):465–474. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-125.6.465
  • Barzy M, Black J, Williams D, Ferguson HJ. Autistic adults anticipate and integrate meaning based on the speaker’s voice: evidence from eye-tracking and event-related potentials. J Exp Psychol. 2019;149(6):1097–1115. doi:10.1037/xge0000705
  • Dias EC, Heather S, Antígona M, et al. Neurophysiological, oculomotor, and computational modeling of impaired reading ability in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2020;47(1):97–107.