80
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Comparing the influences of age and disease on the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia in Japanese patients with schizophrenia

, &
Pages 1203-1208 | Published online: 14 Aug 2013

Abstract

Background

The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) is an evaluation of cognitive function that can be completed with more cases. However, there are few studies that compare which factor, disease or aging, is a better determinant of performance on the BACS. The present study aimed to investigate the influences of disease and aging on BACS performance in schizophrenic patients using subjects with a wide range of ages.

Methods

Schizophrenic patients (n = 165) and a comparison group (n = 171) were recruited as subjects. All participants completed the Japanese language version of the BACS (BACS-J), and the influences of disease and aging on performance in the BACS were examined with the use of multiple regression analysis.

Results

There was a significant influence of diagnosis and level of education on all six tasks of the BACS and the performance and composite scores. In addition, age was found to influence five tasks and the composite score, the duration of illness influenced four tasks and the composite score, and gender influenced one task and the composite score.

Conclusion

The present study suggests that performance on the BACS was impaired not only by disease but also by level of education and aging.

Background

Cognitive impairments in schizophrenic patients can make it difficult to have a daily life or social rehabilitation because the impairment persists even after improvement of psychotic symptoms. Patients with chronic schizophrenia especially demonstrate impairments that range between one and a half to two standard deviations below healthy controls on verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, attention, executive functions, and verbal fluency.Citation1

Many studies have investigated factors that influence cognitive function. Several studies have reported on the influence of aging on cognitive functions in schizophrenic patients.Citation2,Citation3 Furthermore, Bozikas et al and Sugawara et al reported an age related cognitive decline in healthy population by examination of the Clock Drawing Test (CDT).Citation4,Citation5 Previously, we investigated which factor, disease or aging, is a better determinant of cognitive function in schizophrenic patients using the CDT and reported that cognitive function is impaired not only by disease but also by aging.Citation6

The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) is an evaluation of cognitive function for schizophrenic patients that consists of six tests: verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention, and executive function.Citation1 The BACS is fully portable, and the time required for testing is only approximately 30 minutes with minimal extra time for scoring and training demands.Citation7 Therefore it has fewer burdens, and it is possible to complete the test in more cases, compared to the MATRICS consensus cognitive Battery.Citation8 There are many studies that have compared BACS score before and after medication of antipsychotics.Citation9Citation11 However, there are few studies that have investigated which factor, disease or aging, is a better determinant of cognitive function in schizophrenic patients using the BACS. Therefore the present study aimed to investigate the influences of disease and aging on BACS performance in schizophrenic patients using subjects with a wide range of ages.

Methods

Participants

A total of 222 schizophrenic patients who were diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV)Citation12 and received medication as outpatients (n = 78) or inpatients (n = 144) were recruited from the Hospital of Hirosaki University School of Medicine (n = 99), Hirosaki-Aiseikai Hospital (n = 113), and Kuroishi-Akebono Hospital (n = 10). Of these 222 patients, 74 patients failed to complete all examinations or demonstrate an appropriate intelligence level; hence, 148 patients completed the study.

We also recruited 150 premorbid intelligence-level-matched healthy individuals as a comparison group. They had no psychiatric history or current psychiatric complaints. The majority of the comparison group was employed as medical staff, such as a nurse, nursing assistant, dietician, occupational therapist, or psychiatric social worker at Hirosaki-Aiseikai Hospital and Kuroishi-Akebono Hospital. The characteristics of the two groups are shown in . There were significant differences between patients and comparison subjects with regard to age, level of education, and gender ratio. The mean age of patients was significantly higher than that of the comparisons. The level of education of the comparisons was significantly higher than that of the patients.

Table 1 Characteristics of comparison group and schizophrenic subjects

The data collection for this study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hirosaki University School of Medicine, and all subjects provided written informed consent before participating.

Procedure

The Japanese version of the National Adult Reading Test (JART)Citation13 was also given to all subjects for the purpose to exclude mental retardation. The JART, standardized by Matsuoka et al, reflects premorbid IQs in Japanese patients with schizophrenia.Citation14

Statistical analysis

Differences in characteristics between the schizophrenic patients and the comparison group were analyzed using t-tests and Chi-square tests. A multiple regression analysis of the possible correlations between the six tasks of the BACS-J (verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, attention, verbal fluency, and executive function) or the composite score and the clinical variables were performed for each group and all subjects. In the comparison group analysis, the six tasks and the composite score of the BACS-J were set as the dependent variables, whereas diagnosis, age, gender, and level of education were set as the independent variables. For the analyses of the patient group and all subjects, the six tasks and the composite score of the BACS-J were set as the dependent variables, whereas diagnosis, age, gender, level of education, and duration of illness were set as the independent variables. A P-value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant. All analyses were performed using SPSS 17.0J for Windows (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

The mean ± standard deviation of the BACS score for all subjects is shown in . Z-scores, standardized BACS results, whereby the mean of comparison was set to zero and the standard deviation set to one were also shown in .

Table 2 The mean ± SD of the BACS for all subjects

shows the result of the multiple regression analysis for each group. For the comparison group, age was found to influence five tasks of the BACS (verbal memory, working memory, attention, verbal fluency, and executive function) and the composite score. The level of education influenced two tasks (verbal memory and motor speed) and the composite score; and gender influenced only the executive function task. For the schizophrenic patient group, the duration of illness influenced four tasks (working memory, motor speed, attention, and executive function) and the composite score. In addition, patient age influenced three tasks (attention, verbal fluency, and executive function) and the composite score; the level of education influenced two tasks (attention and verbal fluency) and the composite score; and gender influenced only the executive function task. shows the results of the multiple regression analysis for all subjects, in which disease was found to influence all six tasks of performance and the composite score. In addition, age influenced five tasks (verbal memory, working memory, attention, verbal fluency, and executive function) and the composite score; and the level of education influenced four tasks (verbal memory, motor speed, attention, and verbal fluency) and the composite score. The duration of illness also influenced four tasks (working memory, motor speed, attention, and executive function) and the composite score; and gender influenced two tasks (working memory and executive function) and the composite score.

Table 3 Factors that influenced the BACS scores in the comparison group and schizophrenic subjects

Table 4 Factors that influenced the BACS scores

Discussion

The present study sought to investigate the influences of disease and aging on performance on the BACS in schizophrenic patients. The results of the multiple regression analysis of all subjects indicated that almost all BACS performance components were attributable not only to disease but also aging, level of education, and duration of illness.

The results from the multiple regression analysis of the comparison group showed that age was the strongest factor influencing almost all BACS tasks and the composite score (). On the other hand, the results from the patient group analysis indicated that the influence of the duration of illness was stronger than that of age for many BACS tasks and the composite score and that the influence of age was diminished. Furthermore, the standardized regression coefficients of the diagnosis for the analysis of all subjects were large for almost all BACS tasks and the composite score (). These results suggest that BACS performance among schizophrenic patients is more strongly influenced by disease than age. The BACS is commonly used to assess aspects of cognition found to be most impaired and most strongly correlated with prognosis in patients with schizophrenia.Citation1 Accordingly, our results indicate that the BACS can serve as a specific assessment tool for the analysis of schizophrenia in a Japanese sample population.

However, the present study found that almost all BACS performance tasks and the composite scores of patients with higher levels of education and a younger age were superior to those of subjects with lower education levels and an older age, and these results are inconsistent with those of a previous study. In particular, Kishi et al reported no influence of age and education level on many aspects of cognitive function, but did detect an influence for the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) composite score and three tasks of the BACS (verbal memory, working memory, and executive function).Citation15 However, the relationship between cognitive function and age and level of education has been reported in many previous studies.Citation1,Citation4,Citation16 In the study by Kishi et al, clinical factors (age, sex, duration of illness, level of education, smoking status, the PANSS score, and medication dosage) served as the independent variables.Citation15 Therefore, the inconsistencies between the results of the present study and those of Kishi et al may be attributable to differences in the variables used for analysis. Furthermore, this previous study found that the influence of the PANSS was much greater than that of other factors such as age or level of education.Citation15

The present study did not detect any influence of age but did identify that the level of education influenced the participants’ motor speed in the comparison and all subject analyses. Furthermore, Ruff and Parker and Era et al reported similar results concerning the relationship between additional education and faster performance,Citation17,Citation18 and the relationship between motor speed and level of education was also suggested by Jones et al.Citation19 These previous authors reported that sulcus fluid volume, education level, and a measure of general psychiatric symptomology obtained by linear structural relations analysis that relates brain and psychosocial functions to cognitive functioning were significantly related to speed. In addition, Ruff and Parker and Era et al also reported the relationship between age and motor speed,Citation17,Citation18 and Keefe et al reported an association between age and motor speed in the BACS task.Citation1 The reason for these inconsistencies between the present study and previous studies concerning the influence of age on motor speed is unclear, although it is possible that the level of education more strongly influences motor speed than age. The influence of education on attention tasks was also shown in the results of the patient group and the all subjects group. From a meta-analysis, one previous study reported the relationship between attention ability and the reduction in gray matter volume.Citation20 Furthermore, Piras et al reported that for the hippocampus, regions of gray matter may be related to level of education.Citation21 Accordingly, the results of the present study may support these findings. On the other hand, the results of the present study were inconsistent with those of Kishi et al and Jolfael et al, which analyzed the relationship between cognition (eg, attention) and clinical profiles (eg, age, gender, level of education, and severity of symptoms) in schizophrenic patients.Citation15,Citation22 These inconsistencies between the present study and previous studies may have been caused by differences in dependent variables.

The gender influence on executive functions in the BACS tasks was evaluated for the comparison, patient, and all subject groups. Gender was shown to influence working memory and the composite score of all subjects; in particular, males showed better performance on both the BACS tasks and the composite score as compared to females. This result is similar to that of Roesch-Ely et al, who reported that female schizophrenic patients showed worse performance in executive control as compared to male patients.Citation23 In contrast, Leung and Chue reviewed the opposite result as reported by Seidman et al and Goldstein et al.Citation24Citation26 The reason for these inconsistent results may have been due to differences in the examination method. For example, the Tower of London procedure, which was the method used in the present study, is used to specifically evaluate planning ability, whereas the method used by Roesch-Ely et al is a dual performance task that selects high auditory stimulus tones and responds to several types of stimuli and addresses attention ability.Citation23 In contrast, the method used by Seidman et al and Goldstein et al was the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which evaluates the ability of set-shifting.Citation23,Citation26 Accordingly, further studies should compare the differences among several types of executive tasks. The current study also revealed the influence of gender on working memory in all subjects. Although one previous study also reported that male patients performed better than females on visual working memory tasks,Citation27 Lecardeur et al reported the opposite result after examining spatial working memory.Citation28 Furthermore, Minor and Park reported an absence of gender differences on spatial working memory,Citation29 and Bozikas et al and Roesch-Ely et al reported the absence of gender differences on working memory.Citation4,Citation23 Related to these inconsistencies among studies that have analyzed working memory, the reason for the influence of gender on working memory shown in the present study remains unclear.

There were also several limitations to our study. First, the design of this study was cross-sectional. Therefore, future longitudinal studies should take into consideration the accelerated cognitive decline that occurs in patients over 60 years of age in both healthy samplesCitation4,Citation5 and schizophrenic samples,Citation2 and these studies should also investigate the influence of disease and aging. Second, we were not able to examine the influence of other clinical profiles on the scores of the BACS because of the lack of these data. Several previous studies have reported correlations between the BACS score and clinical factors such as differences in medications,Citation24,Citation25 severity of symptoms,Citation13 subtypes of schizophrenic disease,Citation26 duration of untreated psychosis,Citation27 and patient age at onset.Citation28 Therefore, further studies considering the influences of these clinical profiles are needed.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that BACS performance in schizophrenic patients is influenced not only by disease.

Acknowledgments

This work was partly supported by a grant from Hirosaki Research Institute for the neurosciences. We would like to thank Norio Sugawara, Manabu Saito, Yasushi Sato, and Hanako Furukori, Shoko Tsuchimine. NS conducted the statistical analysis, and interpreted the data. MS, YS, and HF completed initial survey construction, and recruitment of participants. ST participated in the data collection, and the interpretation of the results.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • KeefeRSGoldbergTEHarveyPDGoldJMPoeMCoughenourLThe Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive batterySchizophr Res2004682–328329715099610
  • HarveyPDSilvermanJMMohsRCCognitive decline in late-life schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of geriatric chronically hospitalized patientsBiol Psychiatry199945132409894573
  • LoewensteinDACzajaSJBowieCRHarveyPDAge-associated differences in cognitive performance in older patients with schizophrenia: a comparison with healthy older adultsAm J Geriatr Psychiatry2012201294022130385
  • BozikasVPGiazkoulidouAHatzigeorgiadouMKaravatosAKosmidisMHDo age and education contribute to performance on the clock drawing test? Normative data for the Greek populationJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol200830219920318938671
  • SugawaraNYasui-FurukoriNUmedaTClock drawing performance in a community-dwelling populations. Normative data for Japanese subjectsAging Ment Health201014558759220614347
  • KanedaAYasui-FurukoriNUmedaTComparing the influences of age and disease on distortion in the Clock Drawing Test in Japanese patients with schizophreniaAm J Geriatr Psychiatry2010181090891620808150
  • KanedaYSumiyoshiTKeefeRIshimotoYNumataSOhmoriTBrief assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: varidation of the Japanese versionPsychiatry Clin Neurosci200761660260918081619
  • KeefeRSGoldbergTEHarveyPDThe Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia:reliability sensitivity and comparison with a standard neurocognitive batterySchizophr Res20046828329715099610
  • KeefeRSHarveyPDGoldbergTENorms and standardization of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS)Schizophr Res20081021–310811518495435
  • ElieDPoirierMChianettaJDurandMGrégoireCGrignonSCognitive effects of antipsychotic dosage and polypharamacy: a study with the BACS in patients with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorderJ Psychopharmacol20102471037104419164494
  • Yasui-FurukoriNKanedaASugawaraNTomitaTKanekoSEffect of adjunctive treatment with aripiprazole to atypical antipsychotics on cognitive function in schizophrenia patientsJ Psychopharmacol201226680681221616975
  • American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV4th edWashington, DCAmerican Psychiatric Association1994
  • MatsuokaKUnoMKasaiKKoyamaKKimYEstimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading TestPsychiatry Clin Neurosci200660333233916732750
  • UetsukiMMatsuokaKKimYEstimation of premorbid IQ by JART in schizophreniaSeishin Igaku20064811522 Japanese.
  • KishiTMoriwakiMKawashimaKInvestigation of clinical factors influencing cognitive function in Japanese schizophreniaNeurosci Res201066434034420025912
  • IraniFKalksteinSMobergEAMobergPJNeuropsychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal studiesSchizophr Bull20113761318132620547571
  • RuffRMParkerSBGender- and age-specific changes in motor speed and eye-hand coordination in adults: normative values for the Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard TestsPercept Mot Skills1993763 Pt 2121912308337069
  • EraPSainioPKoskinenSOhlgrenJHärkänenTAromaaAPsychomotor speed in a random sample of 7,979 subjects aged 30 years and overAging Clin Exp Res201123213514421743291
  • JonesKJAlbertMSDuffyFHHydeMRNaeserMAldwinCModeling age using cognitive, psychosocial and physiological variables: the Boston normative aging studyExp Aging Res19911742272421820288
  • DickinsonDRamseyMEGoldJMOverlooking the obvious. A meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophreniaArch Gen Psychiatry200764553254217485605
  • PirasFCherubiniACaltagironeCSpallettaGEducation mediates microstructural change in bilateral hippocampusHum Brain Mapp201132228228920336658
  • JolfaeiAGMoshkiPAsgharpourMMoshkiHThe relationship between Attention/Vigilance and Symptom Severity in Schizophrenic PatientsIran J Psychiatry201271222523056113
  • Roesch-ElyDHornbergerEWeilandSDo sex differences affect prefrontal cortex associated cotnition in schizophrenia?Schizophr Res20091072–325526119004617
  • LeungAChuePSex differences in schizophrenia, a review of the literatureActa Psychiatr Scand Suppl200040133810887978
  • SeidmanLJGoldsteinJMGoodmanJMSex differences in olfactory identification and Wisconsin Card Sorting performance in schizophrenia: relationship to attention and verbal abilityBiol Psychiatry19974221041159209727
  • GoldsteinJMSeidmanLJGoodmanJMAre there sex differences in neuropsychological functions among patients with schizophrenia?Am J Psychiatry199815510135813649766767
  • TorniainenMSuvisaariJPartonenTSex differences in cognition among persons with schizophrenia and healthy frst-degree relativesPsychiatry Res2011188171221126773
  • LecardeurLMendrekAStipESexual dimorphism of spatial working memory profiles in schizophreniaCogn Neuropsychiatry201015439740520094930
  • MinorKParkSSpatial working memory: Absence of gender differences in schizophrenia patients and healthy control subjectsBiol Psychiatry1999461003100510509184