109
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

The relationship between self-disgust, guilt, and flow experience among Japanese undergraduates

&
Pages 985-988 | Published online: 17 Jul 2013

Abstract

Purpose

To determine the relationship between self-disgust, guilt, and flow experience.

Methods

A cross-sectional survey was conducted in a convenience sample of 142 Kibi International University students (mean age, 20.09 ± 1.24 years; 85 males and 57 females). Each participant was evaluated using the Flow Experience Checklist, Self-Disgust Scale, and Situational Guilt Inventory. Correlation analysis was used to describe the strength and direction of the relationship between variables. We employed Pearson’s partial correlations, adjusted for age and sex, using dummy variables (female = 0, male = 1).

Results

Analysis of the relationship between the frequency of flow experience and the Self-Disgust Scale scores showed a statistically significant negative correlation, whereas the duration of the activity and the Situational Guilt Inventory score showed a significant positive correlation. The quality of flow experience and the Situational Guilt Inventory score showed a significant positive correlation.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that flow experience could be helpful for those who need treatment to reduce negative emotions.

Introduction

The state in which people are so intensely involved in an activity that nothing else seems to matter, and the experience of which can become so highly enjoyable that even at a great cost, people continue for the sheer sake of the experience, is known as flow.Citation1 Flow enhances positive subjective experiences.Citation1 Flow plays a central role in the area of positive psychology,Citation2 and flow experiences have also been widely applied in the fields of sociology, business administration, education, and sport.Citation3

We have the following evidence regarding flow: (1) a significant correlation was observed between the frequency of the flow experiences and general health perception, social functioning, and mental health;Citation4 (2) subjective stress was significantly reduced in a high-score (high-flow) group compared with that of a low-score (low-flow) group, thereby suggesting that a high flow experience is effective for reducing subjective stress;Citation5 (3) ambiguity tolerance and shyness have been associated with the frequency and quality of the flow experience;Citation6 and (4) physical health is significantly better in groups who experience flow during important daily activities than in an apathetic group.Citation7

Based on these findings, we hypothesized that a flow experience exerts a positive influence on subjective stress and health-related quality of life, but we were also interested to know whether the flow experience has a limiting effect on negative feelings. In addition, Mosing et al have recently suggested that individuals who are genetically predisposed to experience flow, show less behavioral inhibition and feel that they are in control of their own destiny.Citation8

In a previous study, excessive guilt was found to be significantly associated with suicide attempt.Citation9 In Japan, out of 18,104 students (8,620 were 12–15 years of age; 9,484 were 15–18 years of age), 276 (3.3%) of the junior and 396 (4.3%) of the senior high school students reported to have self-harmed.Citation10 Moreover, self-disgust appears to mediate the relationship between dysfunctional cognition and depressive symptomatology, thus demonstrating that self-disgust plays a role in depression.Citation11 Therefore, suppression of guilt and self-disgust is a very important issue in human life. However, the relationship between flow, guilt, and self-disgust has not been clarified completely.

The objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between the flow experience, self-disgust, and guilt, among university students.

Increased knowledge of the interaction between self-disgust, guilt, and the flow experience could help identify treatments for human health care management.

Material and methods

Design

A cross-sectional survey of the relationship between self-disgust, guilt, and flow experience was conducted in Japan.

Participants

The study population was a convenience sample of 152 healthy university students in Okayama, Japan (mean age, 20.01 ± 1.38 years; 89 males and 63 females). All participants provided their written informed consent and were assured anonymity.

Data collection

We distributed a questionnaire to the participants in a lecture room and explained the questionnaire in detail. Upon completion, the questionnaires were immediately collected from the participants. This questionnaire survey was conducted after a regular lecture.

Instruments

Flow Experience Checklist

This scale consists of one item that measures the tendency of a person to experience flow during daily life and ten items that assess the nature of the flow experience when the activity has an intrinsic reward. This checklist was evaluated on a seven-point scale.

One item provided an index of the frequency of flow experiences in a person’s life, wherein a higher score indicated more daily flow experiences. The other ten items measured the quality of the flow experience. In addition, the time taken for the important activities of daily life per week was measured. The reliability and validity of this scale have been confirmed previously.Citation12

Self-Disgust Scale

In the Self-Disgust Scale, self-disgust is rated on the basis of whether one recognizes a negative feeling or phenomenon related to the self and feels disgusted.Citation13

This scale consisted of one factor and contained 21 items (scored on a five-point scale). A higher score indicated more self-disgust. The reliability and validity of this scale have been confirmed previously.Citation13

Situational guilt Inventory (SGI)

This scale consisted of four factors (hurting others, inconsiderate of others, acting selfishly, and feeling indebted toward others). This scale contains 31 items (scored on a four-point scale).

The reliability and validity of this scale have been confirmed previously.Citation14 We used the total score of the 31 items, based on a method suggested by Arimitsu.Citation15 A high score indicated a tendency to experience a sense of guilt.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated for each variable. We analyzed the relationships between the Flow Experience Checklist score and the Self-Disgust Scale score and between the Flow Experience Checklist and the SGI score. A correlation analysis was used to describe the strength and direction of the relationship between variables. We employed Pearson partial correlations, which were adjusted for age and sex using dummy variables (female = 0, male = 1).

SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 19 software (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) was employed for all analyses. Results were represented as mean ± standard deviation (SD), and a two-sided value of P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Characteristics of study participants

We excluded ten participants with missing values on measurement parameters. Therefore, in total, 142 participants were analyzed; mean age, 20.09 ± 1.24 years; 85 males and 57 females.

shows the mean values and SDs for the measurements of the study participants. The mean frequency of flow experiences was 4.7 ± 1.17, and the mean duration of the activity (hours/week) was 10.36 ± 8.94. The mean quality of the flow experience was 46.27 ± 6.66. The mean Self-Disgust Scale score was 66.03 ± 22.03, and the mean SGI score was 102.71 ± 12.32.

Table 1 Result of measurements of the study participants

The correlations between self-disgust, guilt, and flow experience of the participants are shown in . A significant negative correlation was observed between the frequency of the flow experience and the Self-Disgust Scale score (r = −0.242; P = 0.004). A statistically significant correlation was observed between the duration of activity and SGI score (r = 0.200; P = 0.019), and between the quality of the flow experience and the SGI score (r = 0.209; P = 0.014).

Table 2 Correlations between the self-disgust, guilt, and flow experience

Discussion

Flow experience and self-disgust

A negative correlation was observed between the Self-Disgust Scale score and the frequency of flow experiences; the frequency of the flow experience and level of self-disgust are mutually related. Self-disgust is defined as a negative feeling and is related to a lack of confidence.Citation16 In contrast, because confidence in one’s capability occurs in a positive situation, a negative correlation was observed between the Self-Disgust Scale score and the frequency of flow experiences. In addition, the flow experience has been found to be a strong influence in compulsive behavior addiction,Citation17 and it has been suggested that in pursuing the flow experience, the addict enjoys being devoted to the certain subject that can lower self-disgust. Moreover, according to a previous study on post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), playing “Tetris” produced a significant reduction in flashback frequency after 1 week.Citation18 This was thought to have occurred because “unnecessary memories” were reduced in the dedication to the Tetris activity. Thus, it is possible that unnecessary memories related to self-disgust are reduced by being devoted to a subject.

Besides Tetris of course, other “addictive” activities include sports, such as volleyballCitation4 and listening to music.Citation5

As healthy university students participated in this study, we considered similar outcomes might be found in the wider population of health consumers.

Flow experience and guilt

A correlation was observed between the SGI score and the duration of the flow experience. A correlation was also observed between the SGI score and the quality of the flow experience, demonstrating that the length and the quality of the flow experience and sensitivity to guilty feelings are mutually related. We concluded from this that those who experience guilt are motivated to compensate for their bad actionsCitation19 and thus have long and high quality flow experiences. Moreover, the study findings are consistent with the work of Depue et al,Citation20 in which a negative image was shown to participants who were then directed to remember it positively. In that study, magnetic resonance imaging clearly revealed that the domain relevant to emotional information of the memory representation is controlled by a part of the prefrontal area and that the domain relevant to the memory and emotions is similarly controlled. It has been suggested that this domain can intentionally control emotional memory. Individuals experiencing guilty feelings often take a long time to intentionally experience a flow. Therefore, it has been suggested that this flow experience may be oriented to improve its quality. Again, as healthy university student participated in our study, we considered this could also be true in the wider population of health consumers.

This study had at least three limitations. First, our data were from a cross-sectional survey, and this prevents us from discussing causality between variables. Therefore, a longitudinal study should be conducted in the future. Second, as the correlation coefficients between variables were approximately 0.2, suggesting weak correlations, a cautious interpretation of the results is necessary. Participants included were only from one university, so the results cannot be generalized. Further, the weak correlation observed might be due to the small sample size. Finally, there may be other factors that influence university student participants. We employed Pearson partial correlations, which were adjusted for age and sex using dummy variables.

It will be necessary to scrutinize and control for possible confounding factors (eg, home environment, sleep time, interpersonal relationships, etc) in the future.

Conclusion

There were correlations found in the relationships between the frequency of flow experience and self-disgust; the duration of the flow activity and guilt; and the quality of flow experience and guilt. Therefore it is important to promote the experience of flow in daily life. These findings suggest that a flow experience could be helpful for those who need treatment, to reduce negative emotions.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • CsikszentmihalyiMFlow: The Psychology of Optimal ExperienceNew York, NYHarper and Row1990
  • FullagarCJKellowayEKFlow at work: An experience sampling approachJ Occup Organ Psychol2009823595615
  • HiraoKKobayashiRFlow experience in daily living and health-related quality of lifeGirouxJPValleeCActivities of Daily Living: Performance, Impact on Life Quality and AssistanceNew York, NYNova Science Publishers2013169174
  • HiraoKKobayashiROkishimaKTomokuniYInfluence of flow experience during daily life on health-related quality of life and salivary amylase activity in Japanese college studentsJJOMT20115911318
  • HiraoKKobayashiROkishimaKTomokuniYYabuwakiKInfluence of different intensities of brief flow experiences on subjective and objective stressMedicine and Biology201115510713720 Available from http://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/40019018652Accessed February 12, 2012
  • HiraoKKobayashiRYabuwakiKAssociation of cognitive judgment and shyness with frequency and quality of flow experiencePsychol Res Behav Manag2012515916223162374
  • HiraoKKobayashiROkishimaKTomokuniYFlow experience and health-related quality of life in community dwelling elderly JapaneseNurs Health Sci2012141525722288733
  • MosingMAPedersenNLCesariniDGenetic and environmental influences on the relationship between flow proneness, locus of control and behavioral inhibitionPLoS ONE2012711e4795823133606
  • BiBXiaoXZhangHA comparison of the clinical characteristics of women with recurrent major depression with and without suicidal symptomatologyPsychol Med201242122591259822716960
  • WatanabeNNishidaAShimoderaSHelp-seeking behavior among Japanese school students who self-harm: results from a self-report survey of 18,104 adolescentsNeuropsychiatr Dis Treat2012856156923209369
  • OvertonPGMarklandFETaggartHSBagshawGLSimpsonJSelf-disgust mediates the relationship between dysfunctional cognitions and depressive symptomatologyEmotion20088337938518540753
  • IshimuraIKodamaMFlow experience in activities with intrinsic rewards and relation with emotional stateProceedings of the 14th Congress of Japanese Society of Research on Emotions2006Japanese
  • MizumaRThe construction of self-disgust scaleKyoiku Shinrigaku Kenkyu1996443296302 Japanese
  • ArimitsuKStructure of guilt eliciting situations in Japanese adolescentsShinrigaku Kenkyu2002732148156 Japanese12199059
  • ArimitsuKGuilt, shame/embarrassment, and empathyShinrigaku Kenkyu200677297104 Japanese17017713
  • SatohYSelf-Disgust and self-affirmation in university studentsJpn J Educ Psychol2001493347358 Japanese
  • ChouTJTingCCThe role of flow experience in cyber-game addictionCyberpsychol Behav20036666367514756934
  • HolmesEAJamesELCoode-BateTDeeproseCCan playing the computer game “Tetris” reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive sciencePLoS ONE200941e415319127289
  • TangneyJPBurggrafSAWagnerPEShame-proneness, guilt-proneness, and psychological symptomsTangneyJPFischerKWSelf-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and PrideNew York, NYGuilford Press1995343367
  • DepueBECurranTBanichMTPrefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase processScience2007317583521521917626877