120
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Differential effects of dopamine-directed treatments on cognition

, &
Pages 1859-1875 | Published online: 29 Jul 2015

Abstract

Dopamine, a prominent neuromodulator, is implicated in many neuropsychiatric disorders. It has wide-ranging effects on both cortical and subcortical brain regions and on many types of cognitive tasks that rely on a variety of different learning and memory systems. As neuroscience and behavioral evidence for the existence of multiple memory systems and their corresponding neural networks accumulated, so did the notion that dopamine’s role is markedly different depending on which memory system is engaged. As a result, dopamine-directed treatments will have different effects on different types of cognitive behaviors. To predict what these effects will be, it is critical to understand: which memory system is mediating the behavior; the neural basis of the mediating memory system; the nature of the dopamine projections into that system; and the time course of dopamine after its release into the relevant brain regions. Consideration of these questions leads to different predictions for how changes in brain dopamine levels will affect automatic behaviors and behaviors mediated by declarative, procedural, and perceptual representation memory systems.

Video abstract

Point your SmartPhone at the code above. If you have a QR code reader the video abstract will appear. Or use:

http://youtu.be/WSx3_3n_5lg

Introduction

In the half a century since its discovery, dopamine (DA) has been among the most widely studied neurotransmitters in the brain.Citation1 Dopaminergic systems have been studied in the context of neuropsychiatric and other disorders (eg, schizophrenia, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Parkinson’s disease [PD], and drug addiction), as well as from the perspectives of cognitive development and aging. This broad interest in DA primarily reflects its important neuromodulatory function in many different subcortical and cortical neural networks, and the wide range of motor control, motivational, and cognitive functions that recruit these networks.

Not surprisingly, brain DA levels have been shown to affect performance of many different behaviors.Citation2 Even so, DA can have qualitatively different effects on some behaviors and on other behaviors it can have little if any effect. There are several reasons for this. First, DA neurons project to much, but not all, of the brain, so behaviors mediated within brain regions that are not DA targets will be relatively unaffected by DA-related medications compared with behaviors mediated within brain regions that receive rich DA projections. Second, the effects of phasic bursts of DA neurons have a vastly different time course in the cortexCitation3,Citation4 and basal ganglia,Citation5,Citation6 and thus, the effects of DA-related medications on cortical-mediated behaviors will be much different than on behaviors mediated by the basal ganglia. Third, there is now overwhelming evidence that humans have multiple learning and memory systems that for the most part are neuroanatomically and functionally distinct.Citation7,Citation8 For example, declarative memory systems are mediated largely within frontal cortical areas that receive a dense DA projection and where DA release has a slow time course.Citation3,Citation4 Procedural memory systems are mediated largely within the basal ganglia, which receive a dense DA projection that has a fast time course,Citation5,Citation6 and the perceptual representation memory system is mediated largely within posterior cortical regions that receive almost no DA projection, at least compared with the heavily innervated frontal cortex.Citation9,Citation10 Thus, to make accurate predictions of how DA-related medications will affect a behavior, it is critical to understand: which memory system is mediating that behavior, the neural basis of that system, the DA projections into the relevant brain regions, and the time course of DA release in those regions.

This article reviews the current literature on each of these four questions. We begin, in the next section, with a brief overview of some of the most common memory systems that have been identified by memory researchers. The following section reviews the neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of the DA system, and subsequent sections highlight the differing roles of DA in different memory systems and examines some of the reasons why brain DA levels can vary so much across individuals. The final section closes with some general comments and conclusions.

Memory systems

The existence of multiple memory systems is supported by a rich armamentarium of behavioral neuroscience studies in non-human animals (eg, single-unit recordings, and lesion and pharmacological studies) and cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology studies in humans (neuropsychiatric patient studies and neuroimaging studies). A common taxonomy is to distinguish between declarative and nondeclarative systems.Citation8 Briefly, declarative memories are those accessible to conscious awareness, whereas nondeclarative memories are stored and retrieved without conscious awareness.

Declarative memory

Declarative memory is a phylogenetically advanced system that allows conscious manipulation of information. It is typically thought to include three interrelated subsystems, ie, working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory. Working memory is a temporary, limited capacity memory system that can store information for brief time periods (up to ~1 minute). Episodic memory is the long-term memory of specific events or episodes and semantic memory is the long-term memory of facts.

Working memory

Working memory is a limited capacity system that temporarily stores and maintains roughly 3–5 chunks of informationCitation11 for online manipulation and organization, thereby supporting the human thought process by providing an interface between perception, long-term memory, and action.Citation12 The term “working memory” has largely superseded the term “short-term memory”, since the latter traditionally only describes retention, whereas the former also includes the ability to manipulate information.

Based on experiments that demonstrate low levels of interference during the maintenance of information acquired via different sensory modalities, it appears that working memory reflects a multidimensional process supported by different brain structures that are controlled by a central executive located in the frontal lobes.Citation12 Executive-attention and working memory circuits dependent on the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) are thought to select and maintain newly acquired stimulus representations from different sensory modalities, integrate these sensory inputs with long-term memory traces, action plans, and goal representations to form strategies that guide behavior.Citation12Citation14 To accommodate this higher-order manipulation of information, working memory processes are subject to prefrontal executive control, such as inhibition of non-relevant information, attentional guidance, and associative monitoring.Citation12,Citation15,Citation16 Thus, working memory processes may be grossly divided into two parts, ie, maintenance of information and the dynamic integration and manipulation of this information, which is heavily dependent on the PFC.Citation16Citation18

Working memory maintenance appears to be a volatile phenomenon that is reflected in short-term electrical/synaptic activity, in contrast with long-term memory, which is encoded in stable structural changes that depend on genetic transcription and are preserved over long periods of time until information is recalled and structural traces become unstable.Citation19,Citation20 By this, we mean that working memories seem to be encoded via the sustained firing of neurons during the maintenance period. When the firing stops, the memories are lost, unless they have been transferred to some long-term structural store. In support of this hypothesis, many single-unit recording studies have found neurons that show sustained activity during delay periods during which an animal is motivated to maintain the memory of the location of some reward. Neurons of this type have been reported in a wide variety of different brain areas, including the PFC,Citation21 the posterior parietal cortex,Citation22 the medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus,Citation23 the caudate nucleus,Citation24,Citation25 and the globus pallidus.Citation26 Ashby et alCitation27 proposed a computational model in which sustained activation in all these structures contributes to working memory maintenance.

As part of the declarative system, working memory is an important conscious gate in the creation of declarative long-term memories. The transition from short-term to long-term memory is a dynamic process that includes acquisition, working memory maintenance, transition to intermediate-term memory, consolidation to long-term memory, and destabilization and restabilization of existing long-term memory traces during retrieval and integration of new information.Citation19

Episodic and semantic memory

Declarative long-term memory is typically subdivided into the episodic memory of events and the semantic memory of facts.Citation28 Episodic and semantic memories are characterized by high specificity, capacity for partial retrieval, conscious awareness, capacity limitations, and slow access. The term “remember” is commonly used to refer to retrieval from these memory systems.Citation29 Declarative memory formation is subject to motivational and emotional controlCitation30 and is potentiated by attention.Citation31

The encoding of declarative information depends on conceptually driven, top-down processing of stimulus associations and experiences. Declarative memories are flexible by nature; flexibility arises because memories are stored in a relational network in which semantic information is shared among multiple episodes.Citation32 The processing sequences that were implemented during encoding also regulate recall. When retrieving information from episodic and semantic memory, a major distinction is made between recollection, which is a slow controlled search process, and familiarity, which is a fast perceptual process.Citation33 The recall of an episodic or semantic memory requires recollection, but recognition can occur via either process.

Episodic and semantic memory systems depend on a widespread neural network. The hippocampus is critical for the consolidation of episodic memories,Citation34 whereas the consolidation of semantic memories also depends on surrounding medial temporal lobe structures, including the entorhinal, perirhinal, and parahippocampal cortices.Citation35 Long-term storage of the memories is distributed across much of the neocortex,Citation32 whereas recall depends on the PFC, medial temporal lobe structures, and posterior midline regions.Citation17

At the cellular level, long-term consolidation depends on long-term potentiation (LTP) at synapses within the hippocampus.Citation36 Hippocampal LTP begins with activation of post-synaptic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors. A complex chemical cascade is then initiated, which eventually leads to gene transcription, causing structural changes that strengthen the synapse.Citation20

Nondeclarative memory

Nondeclarative memories, which are often called implicit memories, are expressed through performance rather than conscious recollection. In contrast with declarative memories that describe the “what” dimension of experience, implicit memories are concerned with the mechanism of “how” the organism interacts with its environment. Memories of this type mature early, appear to be highly specific, perceptually based, habitual, and inflexible. Non-declarative memory is an umbrella term with several subcategories tied to specific brain regions. Here we will focus on two subcategories, ie, the procedural and perceptual representation systems.

Procedural memory

Procedural memory systems store memories of habits and skills that are learned via repeated practice.Citation37 Compared with declarative memories, the major distinguishing characteristics of procedural memories are: little conscious recollection or even awareness of what was learned; slow and incremental learning; learning that requires immediate and consistent feedback;Citation37,Citation38 and learning that includes a motor component.Citation39,Citation40 Traditionally, this type of memory refers to motor skills, such as playing a musical instrument, riding a motorcycle, or tying a shoe (known colloquially as muscle memories).

More recently, however, it has been discovered that many more purely cognitive behaviors also recruit procedural learning and memory systems. The best evidence for this comes from information-integration category learning tasks. In category learning tasks, two or more categories of unfamiliar visual images are presented to the subject one at a time and the subject’s task is to use trial-by-trial feedback about response accuracy to learn to assign each image to its correct category. Information-integration category learning tasks are those in which accuracy is maximized only if information from two or more non-commensurable stimulus dimensions is integrated at some predecisional stage.Citation41 Typically, the optimal strategy in information-integration tasks is difficult or impossible to describe verbally, so it is unlikely to be discovered via logical reasoning or by a process of explicit hypothesis generation and testing. Tasks in which these latter explicit strategies succeed are called rule-based category learning tasks. shows examples of rule-based and information-integration tasks that are frequently used in laboratory studies. A real life example of an information-integration task might be learning to discriminate between German shepherds and wolves. Much evidence suggests that success in information-integration tasks depends on procedural learning. For example, switching the locations of the response keys interferes with information-integration categorization, but not with rule-based categorization.Citation39,Citation42

Figure 1 Examples of rule-based and information-integration category structures.

Notes: Each stimulus is a sine wave disk that varies across trials in the number of bars per disk (or bar narrowness) and bar orientation. For each task, three illustrative category A and category B stimuli are shown. The plus signs and open circles denote the specific values of all stimuli used in each task. In the rule-based task, only the number of bars per disk carries diagnostic category information, so the optimal strategy is to respond with a one-dimensional bar narrowness rule (thin vs thick). In the information-integration task, both the number of bars per disk and bar orientation carry useful but insufficient category information. The optimal strategy requires integrating information from both dimensions in a way that is impossible to describe verbally.

Figure 1 Examples of rule-based and information-integration category structures.Notes: Each stimulus is a sine wave disk that varies across trials in the number of bars per disk (or bar narrowness) and bar orientation. For each task, three illustrative category A and category B stimuli are shown. The plus signs and open circles denote the specific values of all stimuli used in each task. In the rule-based task, only the number of bars per disk carries diagnostic category information, so the optimal strategy is to respond with a one-dimensional bar narrowness rule (thin vs thick). In the information-integration task, both the number of bars per disk and bar orientation carry useful but insufficient category information. The optimal strategy requires integrating information from both dimensions in a way that is impossible to describe verbally.

Another difference between the declarative and procedural systems is that learning in tasks that depend on declarative memory is flexible with regards to feedback timing, in the sense that long timing delays often have no detrimental effect on learning. In contrast, for procedural learning, the timing of feedback is critical. Learning is best when feedback immediately follows the behavior. Several studies have shown that rule-based category learning is unaffected by feedback delays as long as 10 seconds,Citation38,Citation43 supporting the notion that declarative and especially working memory systems are recruited. In contrast, information-integration category learning is best with a feedback delay of 500 milliseconds, slightly worse with delays of 0 or 1,000 milliseconds,Citation44 and highly impaired with feedback delays of 2.5 seconds or longer.Citation38,Citation43 This complex pattern of results suggests that there is an optimal time for feedback to arrive after a response.

There is now overwhelming evidence that procedural learning depends on the basal ganglia, and especially on the striatum. This evidence comes from single-unit recording studies in non-human animals,Citation45,Citation46 animal lesion experiments,Citation47Citation50 neuropsychological patient studies,Citation51Citation53 and human neuroimaging studies.Citation54Citation56

Perceptual representation memory

The perceptual representation memory system mediates “improvement in identifying or processing a stimulus as the result of its having been observed previously”.Citation57 This type of learning is often referred to as repetition priming and it occurs even in the absence of feedback.Citation58 Phenomenologically, perceptual representation memory can mediate the feeling of visual familiarity that occurs when one sees a visual image that has been seen before, but perceptual representation memory is an implicit system that can operate without conscious awareness.Citation59 Behavioral effects of this type of memory can be observed after only a single stimulus repetitionCitation60 and can persist for months.Citation61

Neuroimaging data and neuropsychological patient studies suggest a locus within the visual cortex. For example, perceptual priming is associated with a repetition-dependent suppression of activity in extrastriate regions of the visual cortex, such as the extrastriate occipital cortex.Citation17,Citation62 Similarly, animal studies show reduced cellular activity in the inferior temporal cortex that is proportional to stimulus repetition.Citation63 However, these data do not provide a clear cellular mechanism for perceptual learning within the perceptual representation system.

Memory of automatic behaviors

Many actions performed by adults are automatic. When we brush our teeth, make our morning coffee, or ride a bicycle, our actions are typically automatic. The evidence is now good that automatic behaviors are not stored via any of the declarative or nondeclarative memory systems discussed so far.Citation64 For example, PD patients have impaired declarative and nondeclarative memories. Even so, some PD patients are nevertheless able to emit an automatic motor response when presented with a familiar visual cue (eg, kicking a ball), despite difficulties in initiating novel voluntary movements.Citation65

Ashby et alCitation66 hypothesized that after practicing a behavior that was initially acquired via procedural learning long enough for it to become automatic, its representation is entirely cortical. According to this view, the development of automaticity is associated with a gradual transfer of control from the striatum to cortical–cortical projections from the relevant sensory areas directly to the premotor areas that initiate the behavior.

Dopamine systems

Neuroanatomy

Although DA cells are found in a number of central nervous system sites, for the purposes of understanding cognitive behaviors, the most important DA neurons originate in the midbrain regions of the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) and the ventral tegmental area (VTA). DA neurons in these areas widely innervate various subcortical and cortical regions via the nigrostriatal, mesolimbic, mesocortical, and thalamic ascending DA projection pathways. The cell bodies of the nigrostriatal DA system, which are located in the SNpc, send dense projections to the striatum. The mesolimbic DA system originates from a more diffuse collection of neurons in the VTA. One portion of these neurons projects to limbic regions such as the nucleus accumbens, the amygdala, the hippocampus, and the ACC. A third pathway, referred to as the mesocortical DA system, also originates from the VTA and projects to virtually all of the frontal cortex. A fourth pathway, recently identified in the primate brain, projects to the thalamus and may be independent from the other systems.Citation67

It is important to note that DA neurons in the VTA and SNpc projecting to striatal, limbic, and cortical areas are at least partly intermixed, suggesting that ascending DA systems interact.Citation68 For example, it is true that most cortical DA comes from the VTA, but there are notable exceptions, such as a group of neurons in the dorsolateral SNpc that project to the dorsolateral PFC,Citation69 and another group in the ventromedial SNpc that projects to the ventromedial PFC, ACC, and orbitofrontal cortex.Citation70,Citation71

Within the basal ganglia, an additional organizational scheme is that striatal neurons (within patch compartments) send direct inhibitory projections to those DA neurons from which they receive DA input and also to other DA neurons that in turn project to a different striatal subregion.Citation72 Thus, ascending DA pathways interact and enable functional interactions via a series of parallel cortical-striatal-cortical loops.Citation73 This spiral organization allows for information flow from ventral parts of the basal ganglia important in emotional learning to dorsal parts related more to cognitive and motor function.Citation74

Through these pathways, the DA systems contribute to a wide variety of cognitive behaviors. Not surprisingly, DA dysfunction in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders such as PD, schizophrenia, depression, ADHD, anorexia, autism spectrum disorders, and drug addiction is associated with multiple types of impairments, including of motor control, cognition, and motivation.Citation75Citation79

Neurophysiology

DA neuron firing includes both phasic and tonic phases.Citation80 The phasic bursts are brief, on the time scale of milliseconds, while the tonic mode includes spontaneous single spike activity that provides a baseline firing rate, which can briefly pause under certain conditions. Spike bursts trigger a high amplitude phasic DA release into targeted synapses,Citation81,Citation82 whereas the tonic firing of DA neurons is responsible for the baseline DA concentration.

After its release, free DA is either reabsorbed into the presynaptic terminal for reuse or broken down by enzymes. DA finely tunes its own synthesis and release with an inhibitory feedback loop mediated by presynaptic (D2) autoreceptors. Dopamine active transporter (DAT) is a membrane-bound, presynaptic protein that serves as a regulator of the synaptic concentration of DA at nerve terminals.Citation83 DAT provides a rapid and efficient mechanism for reuptake of synaptic DA and is essential for the regulation of DA neurotransmission.Citation84

The concentration of DAT serves as a marker of the homeostatic tone of the DA system.Citation85 The highest concentrations of DAT are found in the striatum, with much lower concentrations in the brain stem, thalamus, basolateral amygdala, ACC, and neocortex, including the PFC.Citation86,Citation87 The high striatal concentrations of DAT cause exceptionally fast DA reuptake and therefore rapid clearing of DA from striatal synapses.Citation5 This is an extremely important feature that makes the function of striatal DA unique, where a single phasic DA burst has the ability for temporal coding of the event that generated it. In contrast, low concentrations of cortical DAT cause DA to persist for much longer in cortical synapses.Citation3,Citation88 For example, the delivery of a single food pellet to a hungry rat elevates DA levels in the PFC for approximately 30 minutes.Citation89 In the cortex, DA must either diffuse out of the synapse or be degraded by enzymes such as catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT).Citation90

Most DA receptors are located on post-synaptic neurons. Five receptor subtypes (D1–D5) are currently identified. The DA receptor subtypes have distinct anatomical distributions in the brainCitation91 and can be viewed as markers for different clusters of DA-related functions. The five subtypes are grouped into two families on the basis of structural homology and biochemical characteristics. The family of D1-like receptors includes the D1 and D5 subtypes, and the family of D2-like receptors includes the D2, D3, and D4 subtypes. Of all DA receptors, the D1 and D2 receptors are, by far, the most common. Among these two, D1 receptors are more abundant than D2 receptors, reflecting high concentrations not only in the striatum but also throughout the neocortex.Citation92 For example, in the frontal cortex, there are approximately ten times as many D1 as D2 receptors.Citation93 D2 receptors are highly concentrated in the striatum, lower concentrations are expressed in the brainstem and thalamus, and concentrations are minute in the neocortex.Citation86,Citation94

Receptors of the D1 family are expressed throughout the brain. In contrast, receptors in the D2 family exhibit more regional specificity: D2 receptors are expressed primarily in the dorsal striatum, D3 receptors in the ventral striatum, including the nucleus accumbens but less so in the dorsal striatum, and D4 receptors in the frontal cortex and limbic regions.Citation95 These differences have important consequences for cognition and behavior.Citation96,Citation97

The D1 and D2 receptors are slow-acting metabotropic receptors that are linked to G proteins. Activation of D1 receptors leads to an increase in intracellular levels of cyclic AMP, whereas activation of D2 receptors leads to decreases in the levels of intracellular cyclic AMP. In addition, phasic vs tonic DA release may have differential effects on D1 and D2 receptors.Citation80

Post-synaptic DA receptors are located on neurons that are the targets of the most common excitatory neurotransmitter in the brain, namely glutamate, and there is now substantial evidence that DA modulates the postsynaptic effects of glutamate.Citation98,Citation99 There are two primary mechanisms via which DA exerts these neuromodulatory effects, ie, fast and slow. The fast effect of increases in synaptic DA levels is to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of the postsynaptic glutamate response,Citation100Citation102 resulting in immediate effects on behavior that persist until DA levels decline. The slower effect is to potentiate LTP and long-term depression (LTD), leading to increased levels of synaptic plasticityCitation103,Citation104 which causes long-term changes in learned behaviors.

Neurons select meaningful signals out of noise by suppressing relatively weaker signals, while amplifying stronger ones. To achieve this dual effect, glutamate exerts different effects on two classes of receptors, ie, NMDA and non-NMDA. A common member of the non-NMDA class is the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor. AMPA and NMDA are both ionotropic receptors, but NMDA requires higher levels of presynaptic glutamate release for activation than AMPA. Therefore, at low levels of stimulation, the postsynaptic glutamate response is dominated by activity at AMPA (and other non-NMDA) receptors, whereas at high levels of stimulation, the glutamate response is dominated by activity at NMDA receptors. Evidence suggests that DA potentiates the glutamate response through the NMDA receptorCitation102,Citation105,Citation106 and depresses the glutamate response through the AMPA receptor.Citation100,Citation101 The net effect of these modulations is to weaken the postsynaptic response when presynaptic stimulation is weak and to potentiate the postsynaptic response when presynaptic stimulation is strong. This increases the overall signal-to-noise ratio because noise should cause weak presynaptic stimulation, whereas signal should cause the presynaptic stimulation to be strong. Several mathematical models of this modulatory effect have been proposed.Citation107,Citation108

DA is known to have pronounced effects on synaptic plasticity. This has been observed in hippocampal,Citation109 striatal,Citation110,Citation111 and PFC neurons.Citation112 Evidence suggests that presynaptic glutamate release initiates several postsynaptic intracellular cascades that alter the strength of these synapses, and that DA plays an important role in these processes. Much evidence suggests that, when presynaptic and postsynaptic activation are both strong, DA levels above baseline promote LTP.Citation104 Furthermore, within the striatum at least, if strong presynaptic and postsynaptic activation are present, but DA release is below baseline, then LTD is observed.Citation103

A large literature shows that DA neurons in the VTA and SNpc increase their firing above baseline following unexpected rewards and decrease their firing below baseline following failure to receive an expected reward.Citation113Citation115 Thus, DA-enhanced LTP should be in effect following an unexpected reward in any brain region that is a target of VTA or SNpc DA neurons.

The mechanisms via which DA potentiates LTP are reasonably well understood. One such mechanism is mediated by chemical cascades that are initiated when NMDA receptors are activated by glutamate, which begins the phosphorylation of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII).Citation116 During a brief period of time (0.3–2 seconds)Citation117 when CaMKII is partially phosphorylated, a different chemical cascade that is initiated when DA binds to D1 receptors can potentiate the LTP-inducing effects of CaMKII. The critical step in this DA-induced cascade may be the phosphorylation of DARPP-32 (dopamine and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein) because the phosphorylated version of DARPP-32 deactivates proteins (eg, PP-1) that reduce the LTP effects of CaMKII. Thus, when DA binds to D1 receptors, an important LTP-inhibiting action is reduced. The effect of phosphorylating Ca2+-dependent enzymes, such as CaMKII is to signal the activation of transcription factors and genes that encode for the synthesis of membrane and cytoskeletal proteins to mediate actual structural changes in dendritic spines.Citation118

In the machine-learning literature, a distinction is made between Hebbian learning and reinforcement learning.Citation119,Citation120 In Hebbian learning, any synapses that are (simultaneously) highly active are strengthened and any synapses that are weakly active are weakened. Reinforcement learning is similar to Hebbian learning with the additional requirement of a learning (or reinforcement) signal. If the signal is present, then the learning rules are the same as in Hebbian learning, but if the signal is absent, then all synapses are weakened, regardless of activity. Many researchers have argued that DA plays the role of the reinforcement signal in the basal ganglia.Citation6,Citation121

A necessary feature of any reinforcement signal is high temporal resolution. Consider a task in which the goal is to learn a number of stimulus–response associations. Following a correct response, DA must be released into the relevant synapses quickly, before the critical traces disappear. But after the correct synapses have been strengthened, it is also essential that excess DA be quickly cleared from the synapse. If it is not, and the response on the next trial is an error, then the residual DA will strengthen inappropriate synapses, namely, those responsible for producing the incorrect response. This would undo the beneficial learning that occurred following correct responses, and prevent discrimination learning. Within the striatum, DA is quickly cleared from synapses by DAT and, as a result, the temporal resolution of DA in the striatum is high enough for DA to serve as a trial-by-trial reinforcement signal. However, the low concentrations of DAT in the frontal cortex cause cortical DA levels to change slowly. Thus, the first rewarded behavior in a training session is likely to cause frontal cortical DA levels to rise, and the absence of DAT will cause DA levels in the frontal cortex to remain high throughout the training session. As a result, all synapses that are activated during the session are likely to be strengthened, regardless of whether the associated behavior is appropriate or not. Thus, although DA may facilitate LTP in the frontal cortex, it appears to operate too slowly to serve as a trial-by-trial reinforcement signal.Citation4 Instead, it is thought that cortical LTP/LTD follows Hebbian learning rules,Citation121,Citation122 which means that cortical LTP can occur regardless of whether the resulting behavior was rewarded. Recent work suggests, however, that within the PFC at least, this Hebbian learning depends on baseline DA levels. If the background DA is too low, then activity that might normally facilitate LTP can actually induce LTD.Citation123,Citation124 Thus, disorders that reduce tonic cortical DA levels could have profound effects on the learning of PFC-mediated behaviors.

In summary, DA has two prominent neuromodulatory effects, ie, a fast effect in which the signal-to-noise ratio of the postsynaptic glutamate response is increased, and a slower effect in which synaptic plasticity is potentiated. The consequences of these two different effects are vastly different in the basal ganglia, where DAT concentrations are high, and in other targets of VTA and SNpc DA neurons where DAT concentrations are low (such as in the frontal cortex). For example, in the frontal cortex, a single phasic burst from DA neurons could increase signal-to-noise ratio for many minutes, whereas this increase would likely only last a few seconds in the basal ganglia. Furthermore, these regional differences in DAT concentration mean that synaptic plasticity follows reinforcement learning rules within the striatum and Hebbian rules in the frontal cortex and in other DAT-poor targets of VTA and SNpc DA neurons.

Effect of DA on behaviors mediated by declarative learning and memory systems

Declarative learning and memory systems receive a dense DA projection, mainly from the VTA. Furthermore, DAT concentrations are low in almost all brain regions that contribute to declarative systems. Thus, we should expect that increases in brain DA levels (at least up to some optimal level) should cause persistent increases in signal-to-noise ratio and enhanced Hebbian learning for any behavior mediated by declarative systems.

A wide range of research supports these general predictions, although as we will see, the evidence suggests that there is an optimal DA level for cognitive function, that is, performance is compromised if DA levels are either higher or lower than this optimal level.

Deficits due to DA reductions

PD is caused by the accelerated death of DA-producing neurons. Although the earliest damage is typically to the nigrostriatal system, there is also concomitant damage to the mesocorticolimbic system. In particular, production of DA in the VTA is substantially diminished as the disease progresses.Citation125,Citation126 Since DA neurons die in both the SNpc and the VTA, DA levels decrease in both the PFC and the striatum.Citation2 Within the SNpc, cell loss is predominantly found in the ventral tier with less (but still extensive) damage in the dorsal tier.Citation127,Citation128 This is opposite to the dorsal-to-ventral pattern of the less severe cell loss occurring in normal aging. Parkinsonian motor symptoms appear after a 60%–70% loss of SNpc neurons and a 70%–80% loss of DA levels in the striatal nuclei.Citation128,Citation129 These motor symptoms include tremor, rigidity, bradykinesia, and akinesia.

In addition to motor deficits, non-demented PD patients present cognitive symptoms that resemble those observed in patients with PFC damage (especially those with dorsolateral PFC lesions).Citation130 Numerous studies documenting the cognitive deficits of PD patients have revealed impairment in a wide variety of tasks mediated by declarative memory systems, including working memory, logical reasoning, recall from episodic memory, rule learning, and tasks that require executive attention.Citation131Citation133

For example, PD patients are impaired in rule-based category learning, which requires hypothesis testing about what simple explicit rules can separate perceptual stimuli into categories.Citation133 The most widely known example of a rule-based task is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST),Citation134 and as expected, PD patients are impaired on the WCST.Citation135 Specifically, they show a tendency to perseverate on rules that are no longer successful.

Owen et al argued that perseverative errors can occur on the WCST for two reasons.Citation130 One is a failure to select an appropriate alternative stimulus feature when feedback suggests that the feature that is the current focus of attention is incorrect, and the second is a failure to switch attention from the current unsuccessful feature to the newly selected feature. Using a clever design that isolated these two cognitive operations, Owen et al found that patients with frontal damage were impaired on attentional switching but not on feature selection, whereas (unmedicated) PD patients were impaired on both operations. Based on these and other results, Ashby et al proposed that feature selection is mediated by a circuit that includes the ACC and the PFC, whereas attentional switching is mediated by a circuit that includes the PFC and the head of the caudate nucleus.Citation136

One line of evidence supporting this hypothesis is that lesioning the DA fibers that project from the VTA into the PFC actually improves the performance of monkeys on a simplified version of the WCST.Citation137 If switching occurs entirely within the PFC, then such lesions should impair switching performance (as seen, for example, in patients with PD). If the basal ganglia play a key role, however, then lesioning DA fibers into the PFC should have no direct effect on switching. How then can one explain that lesioning DA fibers into the PFC actually improves performance on the WCST? An important clue to this apparent paradox comes from reports that such lesions tend to increase DA levels in the basal ganglia.Citation137 If the basal ganglia are responsible for switching, if switching is enhanced by DA, and if lesioning the DA fibers that enter the PFC increases DA levels in the basal ganglia, then lesioning the DA fibers in the PFC should improve switching. Data such as these support the hypothesis that switching is mediated primarily by the basal ganglia whereas selection is mediated primarily by the ACC.

PD patients also suffer from proactive interference in rule application, which also has switching and selection elements. In testing PD patients in the Odd-Man-Out choice discrimination task (a task where subjects need to pick the odd-man-out from a grouping of three stimuli), Flowers and RobertsonCitation138 found that PD patients were relatively unimpaired on the first block of trials using one rule (ie, performance was quite close to controls), but were subsequently impaired in later blocks using either a different rule or the same original rule. In fact, their performance decrement was only slightly improved when told explicitly what rule to use when selecting the Odd-Man-Out stimulus: subjects never reacquired the same performance level as at the beginning of the test. In a similar task where subjects were required to alternate their response strategy on a trial-by-trial basis, PD patients produced more false alarms than controls, but only for long time intervals between targets.Citation139 Taken together, these results show that PD patients are susceptible to proactive interference, which reflects deficits both in switching away from an early response strategy or rule, and selecting a new one.

Episodic memory can be tested by recall (eg, “What did you have for breakfast?”) or by recognition (“Did you have cereal?”). Compared with recognition, recall depends much more heavily on the ability to select the appropriate retrieval strategy, maintain it, and switch to a new strategy if the current strategy is ineffective.Citation140 These processes are similar to those required in rule-based category learning. Not surprisingly, therefore, PD patients are more impaired in recall than in recognition, recalling fewer words than controls while still showing the usual serial position effects of recency and primacy.Citation141 Evidence that this deficit is due to PFC dysfunction comes from studies showing that patients with frontal lobe damage use fewer strategies than healthy controls during episodic memory encoding and recall,Citation142 and have difficulty remembering the sequence (temporal order) and the source (who said what) of items.Citation143

Facilitation due to DA increases

Whereas many studies have shown that DA depletion impairs the performance of behaviors that depend on declarative memory systems, many fewer have directly investigated the effects of increasing DA levels in healthy subjects. Even so, some promising results have been reported. For example, several studies have reported improvements in the working memory of healthy humans who were given small doses of a DA agonist.Citation144,Citation145 Although high doses of DA-targeting drugs, such as psychostimulants, impair PFC function, low doses preferentially improve PFC cognitive function of both healthy subjects and patients with neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and ADHD.Citation146 Even so, most of the data on how DA increases might affect declarative memory-based tasks are indirect.

Ashby et al proposed that events that induce mild positive affect (ie, improved mood) in healthy adults are also often associated with increased cortical DA levels.Citation147 For example, receiving an unexpected gift improves mood and it should also cause cortical DA levels to rise since DA neurons are well known to increase their firing following unexpected rewards.Citation114 There is other evidence linking positive mood to DA. First, drugs that mimic the effects of DA (ie, DA agonists) or that enhance dopaminergic activity elevate feelings. These drugs include morphine and apomorphine (agonists), cocaine (which blocks DA reuptake), amphetamines (which increase DA release), and naturally produced endorphins.Citation148 Finally, DA antagonists (ie, neuroleptics) are thought to flatten affect and render reward ineffective.Citation149

If positive affect increases cortical DA levels, and if increased cortical DA improves executive function, then mild positive affect should be associated with improved executive function. Considerable evidence supports this prediction. In particular, many studies have shown that mild positive affect improves creative problem-solving, facilitates recall, and generally improves cognitive flexibility.Citation147 Perhaps the strongest such evidence comes from studies of creative problem-solving using tasks such as the Remote Associates TestCitation150 and the Duncker candle task.Citation151 Success in both of these requires the selection of a nondominant response (eg, finding unusual compound words that relate usually unrelated words and using the match box as a platform for the candle instead of just storing matches). Thus, both tasks make heavy demands on the selection operation identified by Owen et alCitation130 and hypothesized to depend on an ACC/PFC circuit.Citation136 A number of studies have reported that positive affect improves performance in both tasks.Citation152Citation154

More recently, Nadler et al showed that relative to neutral affect controls, positive affect subjects performed better in rule-based categorization, but not in information-integration categorization.Citation155 These results are expected because the abundant DAT in the striatum should cause the DA released to the positive affect-inducing event to be cleared quickly out of the striatum, and therefore not influence the subsequent information-integration category learning. In contrast, the relative lack of cortical DAT means that the DA released to the positive affect-inducing event should elevate cortical DA levels above baseline for 20–30 minutes, ie, long enough to facilitate the selection and attentional switching processes needed for success in the rule-based task.

Positive affect has also been shown to facilitate the encoding of neutral and positive episodic memories, which is demonstrated by better later recall.Citation156,Citation157 One possibility is that the positive affect facilitates hippocampal-mediated memory consolidation. VTA DA projects heavily to the hippocampus, an important part of the medial temporal lobes thought to be necessary for the consolidation of episodic memories.Citation158,Citation159 Normal hippocampal function depends critically on the neurotransmitter acetylcholine (eg, reductions in hippocampal cholinergic activity produce spatial memory deficits in rats).Citation160 DA has been shown to increase acetylcholine release in the hippocampusCitation161 and to improve memory consolidation in a brightness discrimination task.Citation162 Thus, it is plausible that positive affect-induced DA release improves episodic memory consolidation.

Optimal DA levels

Although declarative memory-mediated behaviors seem to improve when cortical DA levels rise modestly, evidence also strongly suggests that large increases impair performance. In other words, DA appears to have an “inverted U-shaped” influence on PFC function, where either decreases or increases from an optimal level of DA impair declarative memory-mediated behaviors.Citation163 For example, blockade and excessive stimulation of D1 receptors both impair spatial working memory.Citation164,Citation165

Other supporting evidence comes from studies that induced negative affect. One might speculate that if positive affect is associated with increased cortical DA levels, then negative affect may be associated with decreased levels. However, stressful or anxiety-provoking events, which presumably would produce a negative affective state in humans, actually appear to increase DA levels in certain brain regions. In particular, animal studies indicate that stressful events (eg, foot shock, tail pinch) cause increased DA release from the VTA but have little or no effect on DA release from the SNpc.Citation166,Citation167 Working memory deficits during stress exposure are likely mediated by excessive D1 receptor stimulation, because they are prevented by D1 antagonist administration. At high levels of D1 receptor stimulation, the responsiveness and signal-to-noise ratio of PFC neurons are both reduced. Thus, these adverse effects of stress could be due to overstimulation of the DA system.

Changing environmental demands and internal states may necessitate shifts of control from one memory system to another, or a fine-tuned coordination between them. It appears that DA participates in influencing this interplay between memory systems. Under optimal levels of DA, PFC functions such as working memory and selective attention are supported, thereby enabling deliberate, flexible regulation of behavior within a changing environment. Sliding to either side of the “inverted U” function of DA compromises this state. Under chronic stress exposure (eg, over 2 weeks), a decrease in baseline PFC DA levels is observed.Citation168 Such DA reductions are associated with anhedonia, social withdrawal (as in schizophrenia),Citation169 and depression.Citation170

On the other hand, acute uncontrollable stress can produce high levels of DA and norepinephrine, with the effect that control of behavior is switched to the amygdala and other more primitive brain circuits.Citation171 Several lines of evidence suggest that DA may initiate this shift: DA levels in the amygdala are increased under stress;Citation172,Citation173 DA produces disinhibition in the basolateral amygdala;Citation174 and release of DA enhances amygdala-related behavior,Citation175 whereas inhibition of the DA system depresses learned fear responses.Citation176 Shifting control of behavior from the PFC to the amygdala and other subcortical systems creates a bias toward sensory-driven emotional responses, fear conditioning, and stressor-associated procedural and spatial memory formation. The adaptive value of such switches may be that responding becomes more rapid and less effortful. This might save one’s life when there is danger and a need to react rapidly, but it could be detrimental in situations that require thoughtful analysis and inhibitory control.

Effect of DA on behaviors mediated by procedural learning and memory systems

Procedural learning and memory systems receive a dense DA projection, from both the SNpc and the VTA. In addition, DAT concentrations are high in the most critical procedural system brain regions, namely the basal ganglia. Thus, we should expect that increases in DA levels will cause transient increases in signal-to-noise ratio and enhanced reinforcement learning for any behavior mediated by procedural systems. Again, the available data support these general predictions.

First, the induction of positive affect improves learning on declarative memory-mediated rule-based categorization, but not on procedural memory-mediated information-integration categorization.Citation155 This result follows from the assumption that the event that induced the positive affect increases cortical signal-to-noise ratios throughout the subsequent period of category learning, whereas the increased striatal signal-to-noise ratios caused by the same event are too brief to affect the category learning.

Second, many studies have shown that PD patients, who have reduced striatal DA levels, are impaired in procedural learning. The two most heavily studied procedural-learning tasks are information-integration category learning and the serial reaction time task,Citation177 which requires subjects to press keys as quickly as possible in response to stimuli that appear in various locations on the screen. A large improvement in response time is observed when the stimulus sequence is repeated, even when subjects are unaware that a sequence exists. Several studies have reported that patients with PD show reduced implicit learning in the serial reaction time task.Citation178,Citation179 Other studies have reported that PD patients are impaired in information-integration learning, especially when the task is difficult.Citation52,Citation53,Citation180 These results are consistent with the hypothesis that reductions in striatal DA levels decrease cortical-striatal synaptic plasticity.

Effect of DA on behaviors mediated by other learning and memory systems

Perceptual representation memory system

The perceptual representation memory system is mediated primarily within sensory association areas of the cortex. The DA systems do not project in any significant way to the visual or auditory cortex,Citation9,Citation10 so the effects of changes in brain DA levels should be minimal on behaviors mediated by perceptual representation memory.

This prediction is surprisingly difficult to test. On the one hand, some seemingly counter results have been reported. For example, transient stimulation of VTA DA neurons during presentation of an auditory tone increases the extent of the representation of this tone in the auditory cortex.Citation181 Similarly, several functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have reported reward-based learning effects in the visual cortex that are consistent with DA-mediated synaptic plasticity.Citation182,Citation183 One complication, however, in interpreting these results is that the effects of reward in sensory association areas should be similar to the effects of attention, because attention is biased toward stimuli that predict reward.Citation184 In fact, it has been proposed that within the visual cortex, plasticity is mediated by attention-gated reinforcement learning that uses acetylcholine as the reinforcement signal, rather than DA.Citation185 In support of this prediction, stimulation of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain during presentation of an auditory tone also increases the extent of the representation of this tone in the auditory cortex.Citation186 Other studies have shown that cortical learning is impaired following acetylcholine depletion.Citation187 To complicate matters even more, there is abundant evidence that the DA and cholinergic systems interact, so altering DA levels is likely to have concomitant effects on acetylcholine levels.Citation188,Citation189 Clearly, more work is needed on this issue.

Memory of automatic behaviors

If automatic behaviors are stored in the cortex, as hypothesized by Ashby et al,Citation66 then their production should be modestly affected by changes in brain DA levels. The motor and premotor cortex receive a relatively dense DA projection from the VTA, but the visual and auditory cortex do not. These predictions are largely untested, although, as noted earlier, PD patients, who have DA reductions and striatal dysfunction, are impaired in procedural learning but are relatively normal in producing automatic skills.Citation65

Individual differences in DA function

Many factors are known to affect brain DA levels, including age, genetic predisposition, drug-taking history, and neuropsychological patient status.Citation147 For example, brain DA levels are known to decrease by approximately 7% per decade of life during normal aging. However, adolescence is associated with sustained overexpression of DA receptors in the PFC that persists until adulthood.Citation190 Interestingly, this inverted U-shaped peak and decline of DA receptor levels occur earlier in the striatum than in the PFC.Citation191 The firing rate of midbrain DA neurons also peaks during this distinctive developmental period.Citation192 Not surprisingly, adolescents differ from adults on cognitive tasks that are PFC-dependent and DA-dependent, such as decision-making, planning, working memory, and inhibitory control.Citation193,Citation194 These and other developmental shifts in DA functionCitation191 may contribute to the heightened vulnerability to drug abuse and stress during adolescence.Citation195

Neurological diseases that impact the DA system may result from aberrant development at different stages. For example, schizophrenia typically develops during late adolescence and early adulthood,Citation191 while ADHD develops in early childhood, but in 60%–70% of cases symptoms persist into adulthood.Citation196,Citation197 Similarly, single gene mutation diseases such as Fragile X syndrome and Rett syndrome are neurodevelopmental autism spectrum disorders marked by derailment of brain development (including synaptic plasticity) from infancy, and especially during the critical period from ages 1 to 3 years.Citation198Citation200

Not surprisingly, different aspects of the DA system malfunction in these three DA-related disorders. A recent meta-analysisCitation201 found that dopaminergic abnormality in schizophrenia is mainly presynaptic, which affects DA synthesis, baseline synaptic DA levels, and DA release. Current treatment mechanisms rely on D2 receptor blockade, which fail to target these abnormalities. ADHD stimulant medications target DAT levels, which are lower in ADHD patients.Citation202 In Fragile X syndrome and Rett syndrome, the DA system is disrupted and hypofunctional,Citation79,Citation108 and dopaminergic treatment shows promise in ameliorating at least some of the devastating symptoms.Citation203Citation205

It is important to note that DA medications do not have uniform effects throughout the brain. When PD patients are given DA-related medications to improve their motor function (eg, L-dopa), they show a modest improvement on many of the memory and executive tasks on which they are normally impaired.Citation131 However, such drugs may improve some processes but have a minimal, or detrimental, effect on others. For example, Cools et al reported that DA replacement therapy improved the task-switching performance of PD patients, but made the same patients impulsive.Citation206 They argued that by increasing levels of DA, normal function was restored to the system supporting task-switching but the system responsible for decision-making became “overdosed”.

Could DA agonists ameliorate memory problems in people with other kinds of frontal lobe dysfunction, such as due to aging or focal lesion? In cases of focal lesion, this may be only successful if enough tissue is left to respond to the DA drug.Citation207 In one such successful attempt, McDowell et alCitation208 gave either a DA agonist (bromocriptine) or placebo to traumatic brain injury patients (who showed impaired executive function, and probably had some frontal damage). The drug selectively improved performance on many of the executive measures, such as reducing perseverative errors on the WCST.

Another source of variability is genetic factors that affect DA neurotransmission.Citation209 Thanks to recent progress in genomic research, individual differences in a variety of DA-relevant genotypes have been associated with a range of cognitive functions, such as working memory, attention, episodic memory, and reward processing, as well as with psychiatric disordersCitation210,Citation211 and addiction.Citation212 At this point it is too soon to draw any strong conclusions because of possible complex interactions among genes and between genes and the environment. Even so, guided by the framework laid out in this article, some predictions are possible and even supported by recent genomic studies.

For example, several studies have shown that subjects with a COMT genotype (met/met) associated with higher PFC DA levels have better working memory and perform better on the WCST test than subjects with a COMT genotype (val/val) associated with lower levels of PFC DA.Citation213,Citation214 Another example is a recent study showing that carriers of a specific DRD2 genotype that is associated with higher striatal D2 receptor availability were better at information-integration category learning than participants with a genotype associated with lower D2 availability.Citation215 Similarly, other studies have examined the physiological and behavioral effects of DARPP-32.Citation97,Citation216

Conclusion

The multiple roles via which DA affects behavior have been topics of scientific research for half a century. In contrast, the widespread acceptance of multiple learning and memory systems has occurred only recently. To date, research on DA function has proceeded mostly independently of research on memory systems. But to predict the effects of DA-related medications on a cognitive behavior, it is vital to know what memory systems are mediating that behavior. This knowledge will indicate which brain regions are relevant and whether those regions receive dopaminergic input. The next step in the prediction process is to determine the density of DAT in these relevant regions. Since DAT concentrations are high only in the basal ganglia, essentially this step reduces to determining whether the basal ganglia play a significant role in mediating the behavior. For behaviors that are mediated within brain regions that receive a rich DA projection but lack high concentrations of DAT, the general prediction should be that even a brief rise in brain DA levels could enhance performance for a period of 20–30 minutes. The major exception is if the person already has high DA levels, perhaps because he or she is a healthy adolescent or under the influence of some DA-related drug, in which case the opposite effect should be expected. For behaviors primarily mediated within the basal ganglia, the same brief rise in DA levels should have little if any effect because DAT will quickly clear the excess DA from basal ganglia synapses. However, a treatment that can raise DA levels for a more prolonged period of time (eg, L-dopa) should improve procedural learning, facilitate the initiation of novel movements, and improve attentional switching. Finally, behaviors mediated in brain regions that do not receive a prominent DA projection (eg, the visual cortex) should be relatively unaffected by DA-related treatments, although this prediction is complicated by the known interactions between DA and acetylcholine.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (2R01MH063760).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • BjörklundADunnettSBDopamine neuron systems in the brain: an updateTrends Neurosci20073019420217408759
  • CoolsRDopaminergic modulation of cognitive function – implications for L-dopa treatment in Parkinson’s diseaseNeurosci Biobehav Rev20063012315935475
  • SeamansJKRobbinsTWDopamine modulation of the prefrontal cortex and cognitive functionNeveKThe Dopamine ReceptorsNew York, NY, USAHumana Press2010
  • LapishCCKroenerSDurstewitzDLavinASeamansJKThe ability of the mesocortical dopamine system to operate in distinct temporal modesPsychopharmacology (Berl)200719160962517086392
  • CraggSRiceMGreenfieldSHeterogeneity of electrically evoked dopamine release and reuptake in substantia nigra, ventral tegmental area, and striatumJ Neurophysiol1997778638739065855
  • HoukJCAdamsJLBartoAGA model of how the basal ganglia generate and use neural signals that predict reinforcementHoukJCDavisJLBeiserDGModels of Information Processing in the Basal GangliaCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press1995
  • EichenbaumHCohenNJFrom Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the BrainNew York, NY, USAOxford University Press2001
  • SquireLRDeclarative and nondeclarative memory: multiple brain systems supporting learning and memoryJ Cogn Neurosci1992423224323964880
  • ThierryAMBlancGSobelAStinusLGlowinskiJDopaminergic terminals in the rat cortexScience19731824995014744179
  • Goldman-RakicPSLeranthCWilliamsSMMonsNGeffardMDopamine synaptic complex with pyramidal neurons in primate cerebral cortexProc Natl Acad Sci U S A198986901590192573073
  • CowanNThe magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why?Curr Dir Psychol Sci201019515720445769
  • BaddeleyAWorking memory: looking back and looking forwardNat Rev Neurosci2003482983914523382
  • MillerEKThe prefrontal cortex and cognitive controlNat Rev Neurosci20001596511252769
  • PosnerMIDiGirolamoGJConflict, target detection and cognitive controlParasuramanRThe Attentive BrainCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press1998
  • KaneMJEngleRWWorking-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interferenceJ Exp Psychol Gen2003132477012656297
  • SmithEEJonidesJNeuroimaging analyses of human working memoryProc Natl Acad Sci U S A199895206120689482838
  • CabezaRNybergLImaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studiesJ Cogn Neurosci20001214710769304
  • MillerEKCohenJDAn integrative theory of prefrontal cortex functionAnnu Rev Neurosci20012416720211283309
  • KandelERDudaiYMayfordMRThe molecular and systems biology of memoryCell201415716318624679534
  • AbelTNguyenPVBaradMDeuelTAKandelERBourtchouladzeRGenetic demonstration of a role for PKA in the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term memoryCell1997886156269054501
  • FusterJMUnit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memoryJ Neurophysiol19733661784196203
  • ConstantinidisCSteinmetzMANeuronal activity in posterior parietal area 7a during the delay periods of a spatial memory taskJ Neurophysiol199676135213558871242
  • FusterJMAlexanderGEFiring changes in cells of the nucleus medialis dorsalis associated with delayed response behaviorBrain Res19736179914204130
  • HikosakaOSakamotoMUsuiSFunctional properties of monkey caudate neurons. III. Activities related to expectation of target and rewardJ Neurophysiol1989618148322723722
  • SchultzWRomoRRole of primate basal ganglia and frontal cortex in the internal generation of movements I. Preparatory activity in the anterior striatumExp Brain Res1992913633841483512
  • MushiakeHStrickPLPallidal neuron activity during sequential arm movementsJ Neurophysiol199574275427588747231
  • AshbyFGEllSWValentinVVCasaleMBFROST: a distributed neurocomputational model of working memory maintenanceJ Cogn Neurosci2005171728174316269109
  • TulvingEEpisodic and semantic memoryTulvingEDonaldsonWOrganization of MemoryLondon, UKAcademic Press1972
  • Rovee-CollierCHayneHColomboMThe Development of Implicit and Explicit Memory24Philadelphia, PA, USAJohn Benjamin Publishing2000
  • DamasioARLooking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling BrainNew York, NY, USARandom House2004
  • DuncanJOwenAMCommon regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demandsTrends Neurosci20002347548311006464
  • EichenbaumHHippocampus: cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memoryNeuron20044410912015450164
  • YonelinasAPThe nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of researchJ Mem Lang200246441517
  • Vargha-KhademFGadianDGWatkinsKEDifferential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memoryScience19972773763809219696
  • SquireLRDedeAJConscious and unconscious memory systemsCold Spring Harb Perspect Biol20157a02166725731765
  • BlissTVCollingridgeGLA synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampusNature199336131398421494
  • WillinghamDBA neuropsychological theory of motor skill learningPsychol Rev19981055585849697430
  • MaddoxWTAshbyFGBohilCJDelayed feedback effects on rule-based and information-integration category learningJ Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn20032965066212924865
  • AshbyFGEllSWWaldronEMProcedural learning in perceptual categorizationMem Cognit20033111141125
  • WillinghamDBWellsLAFarrellJMStemwedelMEImplicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locationsMem Cognit200028366375
  • AshbyFGGottREDecision rules in the perception and categorization of multidimensional stimuliJ Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn19881433532963894
  • MaddoxWTBohilCJIngADEvidence for a procedural-learning-based system in perceptual category learningPsychol Bull Rev200411945952
  • MaddoxWTIngADDelayed feedback disrupts the procedural-learning system but not the hypothesis-testing system in perceptual category learningJ Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn20053110010715641908
  • WorthyDAMarkmanABMaddoxWTFeedback and stimulus-offset timing effects in perceptual category learningBrain Cogn20138128329323313835
  • CarelliRMWolskeMWestMOLoss of lever press-related firing of rat striatal forelimb neurons after repeated sessions in a lever pressing taskJ Neurosci199717180418149030639
  • MerchantHZainosAHernadezASalinasERomoRFunctional properties of primate putamen neurons during the categorization of tactile stimuliJ Neurophysiol199777113211549084587
  • EacottMJGaffanDThe role of monkey inferior parietal cortex in visual discrimination of identity and orientation of shapesBehav Brain Res19914695981786117
  • GaffanDEacottMJUncinate fascicle section leaves delayed matching-to-sample intact, with both large and small stimulus setsExp Brain Res19951051751807589313
  • McDonaldRJWhiteNMParallel information processing in the water maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampusBehav Neural Biol1994612602708067981
  • PackardMGMcGaughJLDouble dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systemsBehav Neurosci19921064394461616610
  • FiloteoJVMaddoxWTDavisJDA possible role of the striatum in linear and nonlinear categorization rule learning: evidence from patients with Huntington’s diseaseBehav Neurosci200111578679811508718
  • FiloteoJVMaddoxWTSalmonDPSongDDInformation-integration category learning in patients with striatal dysfunctionNeuropsychology20051921222215769205
  • KnowltonBJMangelsJASquireLRA neostriatal habit learning system in humansScience1996273139914028703077
  • NomuraEMMaddoxWTFiloteoJVNeural correlates of rule-based and information-integration visual category learningCereb Cortex200717374316436685
  • SegerCACincottaCMThe roles of the caudate nucleus in human classification learningJ Neurosci2005252941295115772354
  • WaldschmidtJGAshbyFGCortical and striatal contributions to automaticity in information-integration categorizationNeuroimage2011561791180221316475
  • GazzanigaMSIvryRBMangunGRCognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind4th edCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press2002
  • CasaleMBAshbyFGA role for the perceptual representation memory system in category learningPercept Psychophys20087098399918717385
  • SchacterDLPerceptual representation systems and implicit memoryAnn N Y Acad Sci19906085435712075961
  • WiggsCLMartinAProperties and mechanisms of perceptual primingCurr Opin Neurobiol199882272339635206
  • CaveCBVery long-lasting priming in picture namingPsychol Sci19978322325
  • SchacterDLBucknerRLPriming and the brainNeuron1998201851959491981
  • DesimoneRMillerEKChelazziLLueschowAMultiple memory systems in the visual cortexGazzanigaMSThe Cognitive NeurosciencesCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press1995
  • AshbyFGCrossleyMJAutomaticity and multiple memory systemsWiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci20123363376
  • AsmusFHuberHGasserTSchölsLKick and rush paradoxical kinesia in Parkinson diseaseNeurology20087169518725599
  • AshbyFGEnnisJMSpieringBJA neurobiological theory of automaticity in perceptual categorizationPsychol Rev200711463265617638499
  • Sánchez-GonzálezMAGarcıa-CabezasMARicoBCavadaCThe primate thalamus is a key target for brain dopamineJ Neurosci2005256076608315987937
  • HauberWDopamine release in the prefrontal cortex and striatum: temporal and behavioural aspectsPharmacopsychiatry201043Suppl 1S32S4120480446
  • WilliamsSMGoldman-RakicPSWidespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine systemCereb Cortex199883213459651129
  • PorrinoLJGoldman-RakicPSBrainstem innervation of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in the rhesus monkey revealed by retrograde transport of HRPJ Comp Neurol198220563766121826
  • WilliamsSMGoldman-RakicPSCharacterization of the dopaminergic innervation of the primate frontal cortex using a dopamine-specific antibodyCereb Cortex199331992228100725
  • HaberSNFudgeJLMcFarlandNRStriatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatumJ Neurosci2000202369238210704511
  • AlexanderGECrutcherMDFunctional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processingTrends Neurosci1990132662711695401
  • CardinalRNParkinsonJAHallJEmotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortexNeurosci Biobehav Rev20022632135212034134
  • EverittBJRobbinsTWNeural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsionNat Neurosci200581481148916251991
  • SalamoneJDCorreaMFarrarAEffort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuitsPsychopharmacology (Berl)200719146148217225164
  • WiseRADopamine, learning and motivationNat Rev Neurosci2004548349415152198
  • KayeWHFrankGKWMcConahaCAltered dopamine activity after recovery from restricting type anorexia nervosaNeuropsychopharmacology19992150350610481833
  • PaulKVenkitaramaniDVCoxCLDampened dopamine-mediated neuromodulation in prefrontal cortex of fragile X miceJ Physiol201359141133114323148316
  • GotoYOtaniSGraceAAThe Yin and Yang of dopamine release: a new perspectiveNeuropharmacology20075358358717709119
  • FlorescoSBWestARAshBMooreHGraceAAAfferent modulation of dopamine neuron firing differentially regulates tonic and phasic dopamine transmissionNat Neurosci2003696897312897785
  • GraceAAPhasic versus tonic dopamine release and the modulation of dopamine system responsivity: a hypothesis for the etiology of schizophreniaNeuroscience1991411241676137
  • GirosBMestikawySGodinotNCloning, pharmacological characterization and chromosome assignment of the human dopamine transporterMol Pharmacol199233833901406597
  • GirosBJaberMJonesSRWightmanRMCaronMGHyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporterNature19963796066128628395
  • JaberMJonesSGirosBCaronMGThe dopamine transporter: a crucial component regulating dopamine transmissionMov Disord1997126296339380041
  • ItoHTakahashiHArakawaRTakanoHSuharaTNormal database of dopaminergic neurotransmission system in human brain measured by positron emission tomographyNeuroimage20083955556517962043
  • SeamansJKYangCRThe principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in the prefrontal cortexProg Neurobiol20047415715381316
  • TzschentkeTPharmacology and behavioral pharmacology of the mesocortical dopamine systemProg Neurobiol20016324132011115727
  • FeenstraMGBotterblomMHRapid sampling of extracellular dopamine in the rat prefrontal cortex during food consumption, handling and exposure to noveltyBrain Res199674217249117391
  • VidgrenJSvenssonLALiljasACrystal structure of catechol O-methyltransferaseNature19943683543588127373
  • Meador-WoodruffJHUpdate on dopamine receptorsAnn Clin Psychiatry1994679907804392
  • HallHSedvallGMagnussonOKoppJHalldinCFardeLDistribution of D1- and D2-dopamine receptors and its metabolites in the human brainNeuropsychopharmacology1994112452567531978
  • LidowMSGoldman-RakicPSGallagherDWRakicPDistribution of dopaminergic receptors in the primate cerebral cortex: quantitative autoradiographic analysis using [3H]Raclopride, [3H]Spiperone and [3H]Sch23390Neuroscience1991406576712062437
  • KesslerRMWhetsellWOAnsariMSIdentification of extrastriatal dopamine D2 receptors in post moterm human brain with [125I] epideprideBrain Res19936092372438099521
  • CortésRCampsMGueyeBProbstAPalaciosJMDopamine receptors in human brain: Autoradiographic distribution of D1 and D2 sites in Parkinson syndrome of different etiologyBrain Res198948330382650806
  • NemodaZSzekelyASasvari-SzekelyMPsychopathological aspects of dopaminergic gene polymorphisms in adolescence and young adulthoodNeurosci Biobehav Rev2011351665168621527290
  • FrankMJDollBBOas-TerpstraJMorenoFPrefrontal and striatal dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in exploration and exploitationNat Neurosci2009121062106819620978
  • CepedaCRadisavljevicZPeacockWLevineMSBuchwaldNADifferential modulation by dopamine of responses evoked by excitatory amino acids in human cortexSynapse1992113303411354399
  • SmileyJFLeveyAICiliaxBJGoldman-RakicPSD1 dopamine receptor immunoreactivity in human and monkey cerebral cortex: predominant and extrasynaptic localization in dendritic spinesProc Natl Acad Sci U S A199491572057247911245
  • Del ArcoAMoraFNMDA and AMPA/kainate glutamatergic agonists increase the extracellular concentrations of GABA in the prefrontal cortex of freely moving rat: modulation by endogenous dopamineBrain Res Bull20025762363011927365
  • UrbanNNGonzalez-BurgosGHenzeDALewisDABarrionuevoGSelective reduction by dopamine of excitatory synaptic inputs to pyramidal neurons in primate prefrontal cortexJ Physiol200153970771211897842
  • WangJO’DonnellPD1 dopamine receptors potentiate NMDA-mediated excitability increase in layer V prefrontal cortical pyramidal neuronsCereb Cortex20011145246211313297
  • RonesiJLovingerDMInduction of striatal long-term synaptic depression by moderate frequency activation of cortical afferents in ratJ Physiol200556224525615498813
  • ArbuthnottGWInghamCAWickensJRDopamine and synaptic plasticity in the neostriatumJ Anat200019658759610923989
  • HemmingsHCWalaasSIOuimetCCGreengardPDopaminergic regulation of protein phosphorylation in the striatum: DARRP-32Trends Neurosci198710377383
  • WickensJRStriatal dopamine in motor activation and reward mediated learning: steps towards a unifying modelJ Neural Transm Gen Sect1990809312407269
  • AshbyFGCasaleMBA model of dopamine modulated cortical activationNeural Netw20031697398414692632
  • Servan-SchreiberDPrintzHCohenJDA network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behaviorScience19902498928952392679
  • YangSNSustained enhancement of AMPA receptor-and NMDA receptor-mediated currents induced by dopamine d1/d5 receptor activation in the hippocampus: an essential role of postsynaptic Ca2+Hippocampus200010576310706217
  • LevineMSLiSCepedaCCromwellHCAltemusKLNeuro-modulatory actions of dopamine on synaptically-evoked neostriatal responses in slicesSynapse19962465789046078
  • UmemiyaMRaymondLADopaminergic modulation of excitatory postsynaptic currents in rat neostriatal neuronsJ Neurophysiol199778124812559310416
  • ChenLYangCRInteraction of dopamine D1 and NMDA receptors mediates acute clozapine potentiation of glutamate EPSPs in rat prefrontal cortexJ Neurophysiol2002872324233611976371
  • HollermanJRSchultzWDopamine neurons report an error in the temporal prediction of reward during learningNat Neurosci1998130430910195164
  • MirenowiczJSchultzWImportance of unpredictability for reward responses in primate dopamine neuronsJ Neurophysiol19942102410277983508
  • SchultzWPredictive reward signal of dopamine neuronsJ Neurophysiol1998801279658025
  • LismanJSchulmanHClineHThe molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic and behavioural memoryNat Rev Neurosci2002317519011994750
  • YagishitaSHayashi-TakagiAEllis-DaviesGCUrakuboHIshiiSKasaiHA critical time window for dopamine actions on the structural plasticity of dendritic spinesScience20143451616162025258080
  • LamprechtRLeDouxJStructural plasticity and memoryNat Rev Neurosci20045455414708003
  • DayanPAbbottLFTheoretical NeuroscienceCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press2001
  • SuttonRSBartoAGIntroduction to Reinforcement LearningCambridge, MA, USAMIT Press1998
  • DoyaKComplementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor controlCurr Opin Neurobiol20001073273911240282
  • FeldmanDESynaptic mechanisms for plasticity in neocortexAnnu Rev Neurosci200932335519400721
  • MatsudaYMarzoAOtaniSThe presence of background dopamine signal converts long-term synaptic depression to potentiation in rat prefrontal cortexJ Neurosci2006264803481016672653
  • KolomietsBMarzoACabocheJVanhouttePOtaniSBackground dopamine concentration dependently facilitates long-term potentiation in rat prefrontal cortex through postsynaptic activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinasesCereb Cortex2009192708271819282455
  • Javoy-AgidFAgidYIs the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson disease?Neurology198030132613306109265
  • ScattonBRouquierLJavoy-AgidFAgidYDopamine deficiency in the cerebral cortex in Parkinson diseaseNeurology198232103910407202156
  • FearnleyJMLeesAJAgeing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivityBrain1991114228323011933245
  • GibbWRLeesAJAnatomy, pigmentation, ventral and dorsal subpopulations of the substantia nigra, and differential cell death in Parkinson’s diseaseJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry1991543883961865199
  • BernheimerHBirkmayerWHornykiewiczOJellingerKSeitelbergerFBrain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington: clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlationsJ Neurosci197320415455
  • OwenAMRobertsACHodgesJRRobbinsTWContrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson’s diseaseBrain1993116115911758221053
  • GothamAMBrownRGMarsdenCD‘Frontal’ cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease ‘on’ and ‘off’ levodopaBrain19881112993213378138
  • LevinBELabreMMWeinerWJCognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson’s diseaseNeurology1989395575612927680
  • PriceAFiloteoJVMaddoxWTRule-based category learning in patients with Parkinson’s diseaseNeuropsychologia2009471213122619428385
  • HeatonRKCheluneGJTalleyJLKayGGCurtissGWisconsin Card Sorting Test ManualOdessa, FL, USAPsychological Assessment Resources1993
  • LeesAJSmithECognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson’s diseaseBrain19831062572706850270
  • AshbyFGAlfonso-ReeseLTurkenAUWaldronEMA neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learningPsychol Rev19981054424819697427
  • RobertsACDe SalviaMAWilkinsonLS6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamineJ Neurosci199414253125448182426
  • FlowersKRobertsonCThe effect of Parkinson’s disease on the ability to maintain a mental setJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry1985485175294009189
  • RavizzaSMIvryRBComparison of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in shifting attentionJ Cogn Neurosci20011328529711371307
  • BurgessPWStrategy application disorder: the role of the frontal lobes in human multitaskingPsychol Res20006327928811004881
  • BreenEKRecall and recognition memory in Parkinson’s diseaseCortex199329911028472561
  • AlexanderMPStussDTFansabedianNCalifornia Verbal Learning Test: performance by patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesionsBrain20031261493150312764068
  • ThaissLPetridesMSource versus content memory in patients with a unilateral frontal cortex or a temporal lobe excisionBrain20031261112112612690051
  • LucianoMDepueRAArbisiPLeonAFacilitation of working memory in humans by a D2 dopamine receptor agonistJ Cogn Neurosci19924586823967857
  • MüllerUvon CramonDYPollmannSDl-versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humansJ Neurosci199818272027289502829
  • BerridgeCWArnstenAFCatecholamine mechanisms in the prefrontal cortex: proven strategies for enhancing higher cognitive functionCurr Opin Behav Sci201543340
  • AshbyFGIsenAMTurkenAUA neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognitionPsychol Rev199910652955010467897
  • HarteJLEifertGHSmithRThe effects of running and meditation on beta-endorphin, corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol in plasma, and on moodBiol Psychol1995402512657669835
  • WiseRANeuroleptics and operant behavior: the anhedonia hypothesisBehav Brain Sci198253988
  • MednickMTMednickSAMednickEVIncubation of creative performance and specific associative primingJ Abnorm Psychol196469848814177069
  • DunckerKOn problem solvingPsychol Monogr1945581113
  • EstradaCYoungMIsenAMPositive affect influences creative problem solving and reported source of practice satisfaction in physiciansMotiv Emot199418285299
  • GreeneTRNoiceHInfluence of positive affect upon creative thinking and problem solving in childrenPsychol Rep198863895898
  • IsenAMDaubmanKANowickiGPPositive affect facilitates creative problem solvingJ Pers Soc Psychol198752112211313598858
  • NadlerRTRabiRMindaJPBetter mood and better performance learning rule-described categories is enhanced by positive moodPsychol Sci2010211770177620974709
  • IsenAMShalkerTEClarkMKarpLAffect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: a cognitive loop?J Pers Soc Psychol197836112621625
  • TeasdaleJDFogartySJDifferential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memoryJ Abnorm Psychol197988248257500952
  • McClellandJLMcNaughtonBLO’ReillyRCWhy there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memoryPsychol Rev19951024194577624455
  • SquireLRAlvarezPRetrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspectiveCurr Opin Neurobiol199551691777620304
  • KimJSLevinEDNicotinic, muscarinic and dopaminergic actions in the ventral hippocampus and the nucleus accumbens: effects on spatial working memory in ratsBrain Res19967252312408836529
  • ImperatoAPuglisi-AllegraSCasoliniPAngelucciLChanges in brain dopamine and acetylcholine release during and following stress are independent of the pituitary-adrenocortical axisBrain Res19915381111172018923
  • GreckschGMatthiesHThe role of dopaminergic mechanisms in the rat hippocampus for the consolidation in a brightness discriminationPsychopharmacology (Berl)1981751651686798605
  • VijayraghavanSWangMBirnbaumSGWilliamsGVArnstenAFInverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memoryNat Neurosci20071037638417277774
  • SawaguchiTGoldman-RakicPSD1 dopamine receptors in pre-frontal cortex: involvement in working memoryScience1991251947950
  • ZahrtJTaylorJRMathewRGArnstenAFTSupranormal stimulation of dopamine D1 receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performanceJ Neurosci199717852885359334425
  • AbercrombieEDKeefeKADiFrischiaDSZigmondMJDifferential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortexJ Neurochem198952165516582709017
  • SorgBAKalivasPWEffects of cocaine and footshock stress on extracellular dopamine levels in medial prefrontal cortexNeuroscience199353695703
  • GreschPJSvedAFZigmondMJFinlayJMStress-induced sensitization of dopamine and norepinephrine efflux in medial prefrontal cortex of the ratJ Neurochem199463575583
  • AndreasenNCRezaiKAlligerRHypofrontality in neurolepticnaive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia: assessment with xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of LondonArch Gen Psychiatry199249943958
  • GalynkerIICaiJOngsengFFinestoneHDuttaESerseniDHypofrontality and negative symptoms in major depressive disorderJ Nucl Med1998396086129544664
  • ArnstenAFStress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and functionNat Rev Neurosci20091041042219455173
  • InglisFMMoghaddamBDopaminergic innervation of the amygdala is highly responsive to stressJ Neurochem19997210881094
  • YokoyamaMSuzukiESatoTMarutaSWatanabeSMiyaokaHAmygdalic levels of dopamine and serotonin rise upon exposure to conditioned fear stress without elevation of glutamateNeurosci Lett2005379374115814195
  • GraceAARosenkranzJARegulation of conditioned responses of basolateral amygdala neuronsPhysiol Behav20027748949312526988
  • BorowskiTBKokkinidisLThe effects of cocaine, amphetamine, and the dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF 38393 on fear extinction as measured with potentiated startle: implications for psychomotor stimulant psychosisBehav Neurosci19981129529659733202
  • NaderKLeDouxJEInhibition of the mesoamygdala dopaminergic pathway impairs the retrieval of conditioned fear associationsBehav Neurosci199911389190110571473
  • NissenMJBullemerPAttentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measuresCogn Psychol198719132
  • JacksonGMJacksonSRHarrisonJHendersonLKennardCSerial reaction time learning and Parkinson’s disease: evidence for a procedural learning deficitNeuropsychologia1995335775937637854
  • SiegertRJTaylorKDWeatherallMAbernethyDAIs implicit sequence learning impaired in Parkinson’s disease? A meta-analysisNeuropsychology20062049049516846267
  • SageJRAnagnostarasSGMitchellSAnalysis of probabilistic classification learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease before and after pallidolomy surgeryLearn Mem20031022623612773587
  • BaoSChanVTMerzenichMMCortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neuronsNature2001412798311452310
  • ArsenaultJTNelissenKJarrayaBVanduffelWDopaminergic reward signals selectively decrease fMRI activity in primate visual cortexNeuron2013771174118623522051
  • WeilRSFurlNRuffCCRewarding feedback after correct visual discriminations has both general and specific influences on visual cortexJ Neurophysiol20101041746175720660419
  • AndersonBALaurentPAYantisSValue-driven attentional captureProc Natl Acad Sci U S A2011108103671037121646524
  • RoelfsemaPRvan OoyenAWatanabeTPerceptual learning rules based on reinforcers and attentionTrends Cogn Sci201014647120060771
  • KilgardMPMerzenichMMCortical map reorganization enabled by nucleus basalis activityScience1998279171417189497289
  • WinklerJSuhrSTGageFHThalLJFisherLJEssential role of neocortical acetylcholine in spatial memoryNature19953754844877777056
  • CalabresiPPicconiBParnettiLDi FilippoMA convergent model for cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson’s disease: the critical dopamine–acetylcholine synaptic balanceLancet Neurol2006597498317052664
  • ZmarowskiASarterMBrunoJPNMDA and dopamine interactions in the nucleus accumbens modulate cortical acetylcholine releaseEur J Neurosci2005221731174016197513
  • NaneixFMarchandARDi ScalaGPapeJRCoutureauEParallel maturation of goal-directed behavior and dopaminergic systems during adolescenceJ Neurosci201232162231623223152606
  • McCutcheonJEMarinelliMAge mattersEur J Neurosci200929997101419291226
  • MarinelliMWhiteFJEnhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of midbrain dopamine neuronsJ Neurosci2000208876888511102497
  • CroneEAvan der MolenMWDevelopmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortexDev Neuropsychol20042525127915147999
  • LucianaMCollinsPFOlsonEASchisselAMTower of London performance in healthy adolescents: the development of planning skills and associations with self-reported inattention and impulsivityDev Neuropsychol20093446147520183711
  • CrewsFHeJHodgeCAdolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addictionPharmacol Biochem Behav20078618919917222895
  • BiedermanJMickEFaraoneSVAge-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom typeAm J Psychiatry200015781681810784477
  • KesslerRCChiuWTDemlerOWaltersEEPrevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey ReplicationArch Gen Psychiatry20056261762715939839
  • NaiduSMurphyMMoserHWRettARett syndrome: natural history in 70 casesAm J Med Genet1986246172
  • ArmstrongVLWhat is so critical? A commentary on the reexamination of critical periodsDev Psychobiol20064832633616617464
  • RiceDBaroneSJrCritical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal modelsEnviron Health Perspect2000108Suppl 351153310852851
  • HowesODKambeitzJKimEThe nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment: meta-analysis of imaging studiesArch Gen Psychiatry20126977678622474070
  • VolkowNDWangGJNewcornJBrain dopamine transporter levels in treatment and drug naive adults with ADHDNeuroimage2007341182119017126039
  • SzczesnaKde la CaridadOPetazziPImprovement of the Rett syndrome phenotype in a Mecp2 mouse model upon treatment with levodopa and a dopadecarboxylase inhibitorNeuropsychopharmacology2014392846285624917201
  • LimCSHoangETViarKEStornettaRLScottMMZhuJJPharmacological rescue of Ras signaling, GluA1-dependent synaptic plasticity, and learning deficits in a fragile X modelGenes Dev20142827328924493647
  • WangHWuLJKimSSFMRP acts as a key messenger for dopamine modulation in the forebrainNeuron20085963464718760699
  • CoolsRBarkerRASahakianBJRobbinsTWL-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s diseaseNeuropsychologia2003411431144112849761
  • ArnstenAFRobbinsTWNeurochemical modulation of prefrontal cortical function in humans and animalsStussDTWillsHKnightRTPrinciples of Frontal Lobe functionNew York, NY, USAOxford Press2002
  • McDowellSWhyteJD’EspositoMDifferential effect of a dopaminergic agonist on prefrontal function in traumatic brain injury patientsBrain1998121115511649648550
  • HaileCNKostenTRKostenTAGenetics of dopamine and its contribution to cocaine addictionBehav Genet20073711914517063402
  • GizerIRFicksCWaldmanIDCandidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic reviewHum Genet2009126519019506906
  • Meyer-LindenbergANicholsTCallicottJHImpact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain functionMol Psychiatry20061186787716786032
  • Le FollBGalloALe StratYLuLGorwoodPGenetics of dopamine receptors and drug addiction: a comprehensive reviewBehav Pharmacol20092011719179847
  • BruderGEKeilpJGXuHCatechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotypes and working memory: associations with differing cognitive operationsBiol Psychiatry20055890190716043133
  • DickinsonDElvevågBGenes, cognition and brain through a COMT lensNeuroscience2009164728719446012
  • XieZMaddoxWTMcGearyJEChandrasekaranBThe C957T polymorphism in the dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene modulates domain-general category learningJ Neurophysiol20151133281329025761959
  • Meyer-LindenbergAStraubRELipskaBKGenetic evidence implicating DARPP-32 in human frontostriatal structure, function, and cognitionJ Clin Invest200711767268217290303