1,838
Views
129
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Iron deficiency and cognitive functions

Pages 2087-2095 | Published online: 10 Nov 2014

Abstract

Micronutrient deficiencies, especially those related to iodine and iron, are linked to different cognitive impairments, as well as to potential long-term behavioral changes. Among the cognitive impairments caused by iron deficiency, those referring to attention span, intelligence, and sensory perception functions are mainly cited, as well as those associated with emotions and behavior, often directly related to the presence of iron deficiency anemia. In addition, iron deficiency without anemia may cause cognitive disturbances. At present, the prevalence of iron deficiency and iron deficiency anemia is 2%–6% among European children. Given the importance of iron deficiency relative to proper cognitive development and the alterations that can persist through adulthood as a result of this deficiency, the objective of this study was to review the current state of knowledge about this health problem. The relevance of iron deficiency and iron deficiency anemia, the distinction between the cognitive consequences of iron deficiency and those affecting specifically cognitive development, and the debate about the utility of iron supplements are the most relevant and controversial topics. Despite there being methodological differences among studies, there is some evidence that iron supplementation improves cognitive functions. Nevertheless, this must be confirmed by means of adequate follow-up studies among different groups.

Introduction

The importance of iron in cognitive functions is highlighted in some early studies.Citation1,Citation2 In those studies, some alterations with regard to psychomotor development and cognitive functions have been generally linked to the deficiencies of this mineral. Overall, micronutrient deficiencies are linked to different cognitive impairments, especially iodine and iron,Citation3Citation6 as well as to potential long-term behavioral changes.Citation6 Generally, a lack of iron in the neonatal period and early childhood is considered as a key to the development of disturbances in cognitive development.Citation7Citation9 At the same time, there is little consensus about the effectiveness of possible preventive programs.Citation10,Citation11 Cognitive disturbances in general and visuomotor development have been related to iron deficiency (ID),Citation12,Citation13 and more recently a possible link between dieting, ID, and the possible presentation of cognitive alterations has been studied.Citation14

Experimental studies show that ID is capable of causing cognitive impairment in animals and humans,Citation15,Citation16 with brain mitochondrial damage as a basis for these alterations.Citation17 Among the cognitive impairments caused by ID, those related to attention span, intelligence, and sensory perception functions are mainly cited, as well as those related to emotions and behavior. Generally, these impairments have been related to iron deficiency anemia (IDA). In addition, it must be noted that ID without anemia may cause cognitive disturbances.Citation11 With respect to ID and IDA, some concepts must be noted. Iron status is a continuum from IDA to ID without anemia to normal iron status with varying amounts of stored iron and finally to possible iron overload. ID is defined as a condition in which there are no mobilizable iron stores and in which signs of a compromised supply of iron to tissues, including the erythrocytes, are detected. When iron-deficient erythropoiesis occurs, hemoglobin concentrations are reduced to below optimal levels. The existence of anemia is considered when individual hemoglobin levels are below two standard deviations of the distribution mean for hemoglobin in an otherwise normal population of the same sex and age who are living at the same altitude.Citation18

Twenty years ago, ID with anemia affected >45% of children aged <5 years in developing countries and >7% in developed countries.Citation5 Currently, it is estimated to affect 2%–6% of European children, with a significant reduction in prevalence. Enriched foods and the use of supplements have contributed to that reduction, among other factors.Citation19 Despite this improvement in the monitoring of ID, it seems that some health problems are associated with long-term deficits in cognitive and motor function and achievement gaps in education (small to gestational age infants, too short breastfeeding period, ID, severe and moderate protein energy malnutrition in early childhood, and intrauterine ID). In addition, behavioral differences have also been found in long-term follow-up studies of children who had protein energy malnutrition.Citation6

In this field of study, the relevance of ID and IDA with respect to cognitive function is the main topic in the literature. The distinction between the cognitive consequences of ID and those affecting specifically cognitive development is another relevant point. The debate about the utility of iron supplements is also a topic that remains controversial.

Given the relevance of the problem of ID relative to proper cognitive development and the alterations that can persist through adulthood as a result of this deficiency, the objective of this study was to review the current state of knowledge about this health problem, trying to conclude with some practical conclusions.

Material and methods

Searching process

It was based on three relevant electronic databases (Medline, EMBASE, and Scopus), with a general strategy including terms related to “cognition” and “iron deficiency”. The Medical Subjects Headings and the Boolean operators AND/OR were used for the searching process. The shared Mesh terms were: (((“Nutritional deficiencies”[Mesh]) OR (“Micronutrients”[Mesh]) OR (“Iron deficiency”[Mesh]) OR (“Iron deficiency anemia”[Mesh])) AND ((“Attention deficit”[Mesh]) OR (“Intelligence”[Mesh]) OR (“Psychomotor development”[Mesh]) OR (“Emotions”[Mesh]) OR (“Behavior”[Mesh]))).

Additional searching was carried out on references included in the papers, published reviews, and via hand searching. The literature search was mainly but not exclusively based on the last 15 years. Some former articles considered as pioneers and/or especially relevant were also included.

Studies meeting the following criteria were included in the review: 1) studies focused on cognitive disturbances and ID and 2) controlled trials, randomized controlled trials, and comparative studies. The exclusion criteria were 1) case reports, 2) populations with nonspecific ID-related problems, 3) participants with special conditions/pathologies other than ID (eg, iodine deficiency), and 4) full text/abstract not available. Previous studies (reviews and meta-analysis) were considered as another source of articles, provided that they fitted the inclusion criteria. An analysis was carried out about references included in the papers as well as via hand searching.

The initial search yielded 269 references. References were included in an EndNote 9 library and screened on the basis of title and abstract. As a result, 26 duplicates were excluded. Thereafter, selected references were analyzed based on the full text. After applying the exclusion criteria, 89 studies were finally included.

Thematic analysis

To analyze the selected articles, a thematic analysis was used following the six-step framework of Braun and Clarke:Citation20 becoming familiar with the data, creating initial codes, searching for themes, reviewing themes, defining and naming themes, and producing the report. Fragments of data that identify a significant feature of such data were acknowledged and grouped together into related themes.Citation20,Citation21 As a result, the following main different topics were obtained: anemia vs ID, neurocognitive development and educational performance, and the controversy about treatment and supplementation.

Results

Anemia versus iron deficiency

In a study about the effects of children’s IDA on executive functions at 10 years, it has been found that those who had anemia showed slower reaction times, less accuracy, and higher latency peak N2 and smaller amplitud of the P300 wave (which is related to working memory) in the electroencephalogram (EEG).Citation22,Citation23

Compared with a control group (non-IDA infants), IDA infants (aged 3–15 months) showed lower scores in cognition, fine motor, and social/emotional areas. In addition, they had higher delta/theta and lower alpha power in EEG activity.Citation24 Children aged 8–10 years were studied in order to determine the cognitive effects of IDA by means of the EEG P300 wave and some psychometric tests.Citation25 As a result, P300 latency in the anemic girls was delayed as compared with the control group, but no statistically significant difference was observed for P300 latency and P300 amplitude between the control group and the anemic group. The psychometric test scores for intelligence quotient and transformed quotient were also better but not statistically significant in the control group of girls as compared with anemic girls. However, the hematocrit values showed a significant correlation, with the P300 wave latency showing that hematological status is associated with some effects on cognition. In a recent review, it was concluded that with respect to ID, of particular importance is the effect on the central nervous system, which leads to the defects in the cognition and learning processes in humans.Citation26

A poorer object permanence and short-term memory encoding and/or retrieval have been shown in infants with IDA aged 9–12 months. These cognitive effects were attributable, in part, to IDA-related deficits in socioemotional function. Children with poor socioemotional performance seemed to be more vulnerable to the effects of IDA on cognitive function.Citation27

If IDA occurs during the first 2 years of life, it is associated with poor psychomotor performance and changes in behavior, such as reduced levels of responsitivity to persons and stimuli, irritability, and inhibition. Different studies demonstrate that effects observed during infancy persist in the long-term. It has been reported that formerly anemic preschool children are less active, more inhibited, and more timid than the corresponding controls.Citation28

ID and IDA may have some prolonged effects on behavior and development.Citation29 In a review about the effects of ID in the first 2 years, it is concluded that the relationship between ID and IDA to cognitive and mental development in childhood is still unclear. Generally, follow-up studies have found poorer cognitive scores on measures of mental and cognitive functioning in the long run.Citation30

Non-IDA predominantly due to anemia inflammation has been related to lower performance on tests of cognitive function, this likely being due to decreased delivery of iron to host tissues including the central nervous system.Citation31 In fact, cognitive achievement is strongly related to hemoglobin level (higher hemoglobin levels result in better central nervous system function) and could be expected in anemic patients.Citation32

There is a debate about the cause of the cognitive impairment with respect to the anemia/ID. Is the impairment a result of ID per se or a combination of ID and anemia? How do these conditions interact? It has been found that cognitive function increases with increased hemoglobin concentration in children with ID but does not change with hemoglobin concentration in children with normal serum ferritin level. It seems that children with IDA consistently have the poorest cognitive function. Children with nonanemic ID but with high hemoglobin levels have significantly high cognitive function. So a dose–response relationship between hemoglobin and cognitive function in children with ID has been found, whereas no similar evidence is found in iron-sufficient children.Citation33 Nevertheless, other authors support the idea that some cognitive symptoms get better with iron supplementation, this improvement being independent of the hemoglobin levels.Citation34 In fact, a significant inverse relationship has been found between body iron and central executive function (planning time) in nonanemic college women.Citation35

In a study focused on the effect of ID on cognitive development among children, the authors concluded that the effect of short-term iron treatment in anemic children aged <2 years is controversial.Citation13 With respect to anemic children aged >2 years, it seems that iron treatment would be more effective. Finally, they stated that preventing IDA could produce benefits to development but that they would be small and transient.

Neurocognitive development and educational performance

A consequence of ID relies on alterations in cognition that occur among iron-deficient individuals during the early parts of their life cycle and perhaps at later times as well. While ID was presumed to cause most of its effects only if anemia was present, it is clear that many organs show morphologic, physiologic, and biochemical changes before there is any significant drop in hemoglobin concentration. ID is associated with alterations in many metabolic processes that may impact brain functioning (eg, mitochondria electron transport, neurotransmitter synthesis and degradation, protein synthesis, organogenesis). It is necessary to separate the developmental aspects of ID and neural functioning from the aspects of ID that could occur at any time in life.Citation36

In a recent study,Citation37 a chronic ID group showed significantly lower scores on language, environmental sound perception, and motor measures when compared with infants with normal nutritional iron status at 6 months and 14–18 months. The conclusion was that the development of language and motor skills and environmental sound perception appeared to be sensitive to the effects of chronic ID in infants. In fact, IDA produces alterations in cognitive processes related to visual attention and concept acquisition in children.Citation38 A lower school achievement and poorer fine-hand movements have been described in children.Citation39

For some authors, iron status does not play a major role in educational performance and intelligence of school-going adolescents, stating that several factors (apart from ID) affect educational performance and intelligence.Citation40 EdenCitation41 highlights that children with ID and IDA, especially those in lower socioeconomic groups, are at high risk for neurodevelopmental impairment both from the ID itself as well as from central nervous system damage caused by the associated increased absorption of other metals like lead. With regards to the low socioeconomic groups, 26% of postinstitutionalized children have been shown to be iron deficient. ID is associated with neurobehavioral alterations months after postadoption, mediated by the effect on attention and activity levels (hyperactivity).Citation42

Maternal genotypes at single nucleotide polymorphisms in the genes HFE (rs1799945) and (rs1800562), TF (rs3811647), and TMPRSS6 (rs1800562) are related to iron, hemoglobin, or transferrin levels. In a study by Lewis et alCitation43 exploring the effect of prenatal iron levels on offspring cognition, the aforementioned mother’s genotype loci were not associated with offspring intelligence quotient at age 8 years, so there is no evidence of an effect of exposure to low levels of iron (within the normal range) in pregnancy on offspring cognition at age 8 years. With respect to the genotypes, it has been shown that elevated iron levels may decrease cognitive speed in older individuals susceptible to cognitive impairment despite cognitive performance over 3 years not being associated with HFE C282Y genotype or iron parameters.Citation44

There is evidence that, despite iron repletion, ID during the brain growth spurt alters metabolism and neurotransmission, myelination, and gene and protein profiles.Citation45 In humans, there is compelling evidence that 6- to 24-month-old infants with IDA are at risk for poorer cognitive, motor, socioemotional, and neurophysiologic development in short- and long-term outcomes. Alterations in the mesolimbic pathway, where dopamine plays a major role in behavioral activation and inhibition, positive affect, and inherent reward may help explain altered socioemotional behavior in iron-deficient infants.Citation46

Different functions and several ages seem to cause controversy in this field of study. On the one hand, in animals, deficits in motor activity are consistently associated with severe IDA, but adverse effects on performance in tests that target cognitive function have not been clearly shown. On the other hand, IDA resistant to iron administration (with the consequent failure of iron therapy) has been observed in most trials of children aged <2 years but not in older children. Similar observations have been made in rodents when IDA occurred before rather than after weaning. In children aged >2 years and in adolescents with IDA, evidence suggests cognitive or behavioral deficits.Citation47 In a recent reviewCitation48 it is stated that despite the clear elucidation of the neural mechanics of ID, there is not unequivocal support for a direct causal relationship between ID and abnormal development. Even accepting the evidence as being overwhelmingly in support of a direct causal link in experimental animals, we are far from being able to do so for humans, at least with any degree of specificity.

Apart from infants and children, women of reproductive age are specially affected by ID, and cognitive–behavioral consequences of that deficiency have been described in that population.Citation49 Not only has ID (eg, as a result of a low intake of foods rich in bioavailable iron) been related to cognitive impairments but a high consumption of foods rich in inhibitors of iron (such as phytate, certain dietary fibres, and calcium) has too.Citation16

The controversy about treatment and supplementation

Several benefits of iron supplementation, independent of increasing hemoglobin, such as those on immune function, physical performance, thermoregulation, cognition, and restless leg syndrome, among others, have been described.Citation10 Considering the cognitive functions, the point is whether iron supplementation can improve them.Citation50

With respect to the behavioral problems associated with ID, it seems that an early iron supplementation of marginally low birth weight infants does not affect cognitive functions at age 3.5 years but significantly reduces the prevalence of behavioral problems.Citation51 For other authors, iron seems to have a modest effect on linear growth in deficient populations.Citation52

Considering a positive effect of iron, does it depend on the previous ID (without anemia) or is it necessary to suffer from IDA? In this regard, Bruner et alCitation53 reported the benefits of iron supplementation in an urban population of nonanemic iron-deficient adolescent girls. That supplementation improved verbal learning and memory. Another study showed that in healthy formerly iron-deficient anemic children the altered cognitive function might be prolonged despite the iron treatment. The authors suggested that long-lasting changes in myelination and energy metabolism, perhaps especially in the hippocampus, might account for these long-term effects on an important aspect of human cognitive development.Citation54 Devaki et alCitation55 tested two groups of adolescents (nonanemic ID and iron-deficient anemic) after 8 months of iron supplementation. This supplementation resulted in significant improvements in cognitive function and school achievements. In a shorter follow-up study (6 months of iron supplementation), Ebenezer et alCitation56 did not find a significant impact on concentration levels and educational tests scores. Comparing iron-deficient anemic children with those who are iron replete, it seems that anemic children show faster motor development than the control children do.Citation57 Another study highlights that there seems to be a dose–response relationship between hemoglobin and cognitive function in children with ID, whereas no similar evidence has been found in iron-sufficient children.Citation58

Another relevant point is the moment to introduce the iron supplementation. In a study by Christian et alCitation59 it was reported that fine motor functioning among offspring was positively associated with prenatal iron/folic acid supplementation in an area where ID was prevalent. In another study, Friel et alCitation60 gave an iron supplementation to term breastfed infants (aged 1–6 months). With respect to the development effects, that supplementation resulted in higher visual acuity and developmental indexes at age 13 months. After controlling for background factors, children who had severe, chronic ID in infancy showed poorer arithmetic achievement and written expression, motor functioning, and some specific cognitive processes (spatial memory, selective recall, and tachistoscopic threshold), and this occurred 10 years after treatment.Citation61 Sachdev et alCitation62 have also reported that iron supplementation improves mental development modestly. This effect is particularly apparent for intelligence tests on children aged >7 years and in initially anemic or iron-deficient anemic subjects. There are controversial results with respect to the effect of iron therapy in children. While Sachdev et al state that there is no convincing evidence that iron treatment has an effect on mental development in children aged <27 months or on motor development, and Yalcin et alCitation63 consider that short-term iron supplementation does not change developmental test scores despite the hematologic response in iron-sufficient healthy infants, others seem to report more positive results. Thus, Matiashvili et alCitation64 found that children aged between 16 months and 24 months improved their performance in neurodevelopmental measures when they had received iron in a timely manner. In preschool children, some improvements in cognitive functions have been reported after iron supplementation: eg, in discrimination, specifically selective attention.Citation65 In a former study, it was reported that the administration of heme iron-fortified cookies might improve the intellectual performance of low-income preschool children.Citation66 In addition, Soewondo et alCitation38 and Idjradinata and PollittCitation67 have reported cognitive improvements after iron supplementation in children. With respect to the cognitive development, Thompson et alCitation68 state that iron supplementation produces a small improvement. It has been reported that iron treatment of young children with IDA has a positive short-term effect on psychomotor development or cognitive function, but the effect of longer-term treatment remains unclear.Citation69 Not only iron but also a mixture of iron and folic acid supplementation seems to produce beneficial effects on cognition in young adolescent girls.Citation70 On the contrary, Siegel et alCitation71 state that neither combined nor individual supplements improve the performance on tasks such as A-not-B or fixation duration in infants. Solon et alCitation72 reported similar results. These authors did not find any improvements in cognitive performance after supplementation with a multiple micronutrient-fortified drink. The effect of several drinks containing different micronutrients and sugar seems to be positive when those beverages are given alone. In other cases, the combination attenuates that positive effect on cognition.Citation73 Baumgartner et alCitation74 found that in children with poor iron and n-3 fatty acid status, iron supplementation improved verbal and nonverbal learning and memory, particularly in children with anemia. In contrast, docosahexaenoic acid/eicosapentaenoic acid supplementation had no benefits on cognition and impaired working memory in anemic children and long-term memory and retrieval in girls with ID.

Iron supplementation has been studied in adults with IDA by means of the Mini Mental State Examination, Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised, event-related potentials, and EEG. As a result, authors concluded that IDA is a key in cognitive performance in adults, which can be partially reversed by treatment.Citation75 In young women, Murray-Kolb and BeardCitation76 have found that an improvement in serum ferritin after iron supplementation correlated to better cognitive performance, whereas a significant improvement in hemoglobin correlated to improved speed in completing the cognitive tasks. Recently, Leonard et alCitation77 have analyzed some differences between iron-deficient and iron-sufficient young women with respect to their cognitive function after receiving placebo versus iron supplement. As a result, they have found out that change scores for impulsivity and attention were significantly greater in plasma ferritin improvers than in nonimprovers.

Discussion

Previous reviews on this field of study concluded that ID has a negative impact on cognition, behavior, and motor skills that can have long-term implications.Citation13 Many studies highlight that the relationship between ID and cognitive consequences could be confounded by external factors such as low socioeconomic background. Apart from socioeconomic factors, ID seems to impact on areas such as the hippocampus,Citation61 and the cognitive dysfunction might be due to mitochondrial damage.Citation78 In addition, changes in brain dopamine metabolism are presumed to occur,Citation1,Citation2 as well as altered serotonergic neurotransmissionCitation5 and alterations in the dopamine receptors.Citation79 In fact, brain ID and/or IDA result in major changes in dopamine levels in brain crucial areas.Citation80 These changes related to central (brain) ID might cause different effects than those caused by IDA. More recently, LozoffCitation46 has reported that alterations in the mesolimbic pathway, positive affect, and inherent reward might give support to explain the altered socioemotional behavior that has been described in children with ID. This author states that poorer motor sequencing and bimanual coordination and lower spontaneous eye blink rate in children with IDA are consistent with impaired function in the nigrostriatal pathway. Generally, nutrition has been shown to affect the brain’s macrostructure (eg, development of brain areas such as the hippocampus), microstructure (eg, myelination of neurons), and level and operation of neurotransmitters (eg, dopamine levels or receptor numbers), all of which can have an impact on cognitive development.Citation81 Specifically, nutrients such as iron and omega-3 – polyunsaturated fatty acids, in particular – might have specific effects on frontal lobe functioning.Citation9

There are works in which it is possible to obtain a clear idea about the effects of ID and IDA on behavior and development.Citation29 Generally, in non-IDA, there seems to be lower performance in cognitive functions,Citation31 so a question arises as to whether ID, IDA, and anemia (other than IDA) may cause the same cognitive deficits. This question has an unclear response to date. Some studies correlate hemoglobin levels to cognitive function,Citation33 while others report an improvement in cognitive functions after iron supplementation, which seems not to be correlated to those levels.Citation34 Despite the specific effect of hemoglobin levels or iron status not being clear, IDA has proved to cause different cognitive alterations in all stages of life. A relevant effect of ID is caused during brain growth, as stated by Lozoff et al.Citation45 For others, the effect of ID is not unequivocal considering a possible direct causal relationship between ID and abnormal development.Citation48 Along with the work of Lozoff et al others seem to point out that there are critical periods in which ID might have long-term effects on cognition and behavior (eg, premature babies, infants aged <1 year, and preschool children).Citation82Citation84 Apart from those critical periods, it seems that there are not any substantial different effects of ID or IDA on cognitive functions depending on the age reported clearly in the literature. With regards to the sex, it seems that there are no differences between boys and girls with ID in cognitive test performance.Citation74

With more or fewer consequences and major or minor effects depending on age and sex, for example, there is a common practice that consists of supplementing with iron in case of ID and/or IDA. Here another debate arises. Is it useful?

There seem to be two tendencies considering the supplementation – on the one hand, the use of multimicronutrient supplements and, on the other hand, the use of specific micronutrients such as iron in this field of study. In addition, not always clear is the distinction between supplementation and fortification in the studies based on this topic. Nevertheless, this debate seems to be a perceptive problem. If both fortification and supplementation similarly involve the addition of micronutrients (vitamins or minerals) to the diet, why is there such a disparity in the perceptions of each? The fact is that while food fortification has been viewed by some as a nutritional triumph, individual supplementation with vitamins or minerals has, at best, been deemed unnecessary without underlying deficiency, and at worst been called potentially hazardous. So what are the differences between supplementation and fortification that make one more acceptable than the other?Citation85 So supplementation, fortification, or treatment should be considered without distinction in order to explore the cognitive improvements in case of ID and IDA.

Some authors report a behavioral improvement but not better results on cognitive function after iron supplementation,Citation52 while others highlight an improvement in verbal learning/memory in ID (nonanemic) individuals.Citation53 Despite years of treatment, there seem to be some cognitive alterations that persist.Citation54 The controversial results focus not only on the long-term follow-up studies but on short-term ones too. For example, some authors state that an iron supplementation over a period of 8 months or 30 days is effective, while others point out that the supplementation during 6 months is not effective.Citation55,Citation56,Citation69

Another relevant point highlights the moment to introduce the iron supplementation. When should it be introduced? There are a lot of studies indicating the success of an early iron supplementation: prenatal,Citation59 age 1–6 months,Citation60 age <27 months,Citation62 age 16–24 months,Citation64 and preschool,Citation65,Citation66 among others.

Finally, there are controversial results with respect to the “mixtures” used as supplements. Thus, a beneficial effect of iron and folic acid supplementation has been reported.Citation70 On the contrary, Siegel et alCitation71 and Solon et alCitation72 do not find positive results with combined supplements. For others, the combination of supplements might attenuate some positive effects.Citation73 With respect to the specific action of polyunsaturated fatty acids, Baumgartner et alCitation74 found that docosahexaenoic acid/eicosapentaenoic acid supplementation had no benefits on cognition and impaired working memory in anemic children and long-term memory and retrieval in girls with ID.

Despite iron excess cannot be excreted by the human body (in contrast with other nutrients), iron supplementation is often recommended, that supplementation not always being necessary. Iron supplements seem to be beneficial in iron-deficient children, but there is a risk of adverse effects in those who are iron replete. It has been suggested that excessive iron supplementation of infants may have adverse effects on growth, risk of infections, and even on cognitive development. In addition, excessive iron intake can have negative effects on brain development.Citation86 So in populations with a low prevalence of ID, general supplementation should be avoided. The same applies to iron-fortified foods. In this regard, more studies are needed to better determine the risks and benefits of iron supplementation and iron-fortified foods given to iron-deficient and iron-sufficient children.Citation86

Despite there being methodological differences among studies, there is some evidence that iron supplementation may improve some functions, such as attention, concentration, and intelligence. Nevertheless, these results require confirmation with well-powered, blinded, independently funded, randomized controlled trials of at least 1 year’s duration in different age groups including children, adolescents, adults, and older people and across all levels of baseline iron status.Citation87,Citation88 Many studies differ in the characteristics of the study population, definition of exposure, type of treatment, and confounders, so it is difficult to assess a causal relationship between ID and IDA and cognitive and mental development in childhood,Citation30 and the same applies to the use of supplements.

Maybe, an early-introduced correct (adequate) diet is better than the use of supplements. Thus, some studies suggest that increased meat intake by breastfed infants aged >6 months would adequately support iron requirements.Citation89

Conclusion

There is a consensus on the fact that ID has a negative impact on cognition, behavior, and motor skills. With respect to this negative impact, its causal link with ID might be confounded by external variables such as socioeconomic status.

ID, IDA, and non-ID anemia may cause some cognitive deficits, but it remains unclear if those deficits are the same. These cognitive deficits may appear at any age. Levels of hemoglobin seem to correlate to cognitive performance, but iron supplementation improves cognitive functions regardless of the hemoglobin levels.

Despite the possible action of confounders, ID is clearly related to alterations in areas such as the hippocampus, mitochondrial damage, brain dopamine metabolism, and myelination.

The use of supplements (fortification, treatment) must be based on well-established indications, avoiding their use as a routine. The use of multisupplements does not seem to add a plus with respect to the use of specific supplements. The success of the supplementation might be based on an early prescription after having diagnosed an iron deficiency. It remains controversial whether that supplementation is successful or not, depending on the timing of the therapy (eg, critical periods).

More studies of an appropriate duration in different age groups (children, adolescents, adults, and older people) and across all levels of baseline iron status are required.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • BeardJOne person’s view of iron deficiency, development, and cognitive functionAm J Clin Nutr1995627097107572696
  • KretchmerNBeardJLCarlsonSThe role of nutrition in the development of normal cognitionAm J Clin Nutr199663997S1001S8644701
  • FanjiangGKleinmanRENutrition and performance in childrenCurr Opin Clin Nutr200710342347
  • BlackREGlobal distribution and disease burden related to micronutrient deficienciesNestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser201478212824504203
  • Grantham-McGregorSMAniCCThe role of micronutrients in psychomotor and cognitive developmentBr Med Bull19995551152710746342
  • Grantham-McGregorSMWalkerSPChangSNutritional deficiencies and later behavioural developmentProc Nutr Soc200059475410828173
  • BodnarLMCogswellMEMcDonaldTHave we forgotten the significance of postpartum iron deficiency?Am J Obstet Gynecol2005193364416021056
  • BentonDILSI Europe a.i.s.b.l.Micronutrient status, cognition and behavioral problems in childhoodEur J Nutr200847Suppl 3385018683028
  • BryanJOsendarpSHughesDNutrients for cognitive development in school-aged childrenNutr Rev20046229530615478684
  • AgarwalRNon-hematological benefits of ironAm J Nephrol20072756557117804903
  • BlackMMMicronutrient deficiencies and cognitive functioningJ Nutr20031333927S3931S14672291
  • GonzalezHFMalpeliAEtchegoyenGAcquisition of visuomotor abilities and intellectual quotient in children aged 4–10 years: relationship with micronutrient nutritional statusBiol Trace Elem Res20071209210117916959
  • Grantham-McGregorSAniCA review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in childrenJ Nutr2001131649S668S11160596
  • GreenMWEllimanNAAre dieting-related cognitive impairments a function of iron status?Br J Nutr201310918419222414889
  • TamuraTGoldenbergRLHouJCord serum ferritin concentrations and mental and psychomotor development of children at five years of ageJ Pediatr200214016517011865266
  • SandsteadHHFredericksonCJPenlandJGHistory of zinc as related to brain functionJ Nutr2000130496S502S10721938
  • AtamnaHWalterPBAmesBNThe role of heme and iron-sulfur clusters in mitochondrial biogenesis, maintenance, and decay with ageArch Biochem Biophys200239734535311795893
  • World Health OrganizationIron Deficiency Anaemia Assessment, Prevention, and Control A Guide for Programme ManagersGeneva, SwitzerlandWorld Health Organization2001
  • Sánchez BreversAMonografía Sobre la Anemia Ferropénica [dissertation]University of CantabriaSpain2013
  • BraunVClarkeVUsing thematic analysis in psychologyQual Res Psychol2006377101
  • AveyardHDoing a Literature Review in Health and Social Care2nd edBerkshire, UKMcGraw-Hill2010
  • AllowayTPWhat do we know about the long-term cognitive effects of iron-deficiency anemia in infancy?Dev Med Child Neurol20135540140223464758
  • AlgarinCNelsonCAPeiranoPIron-deficiency anemia in infancy and poorer cognitive inhibitory control at age 10 yearsDev Med Child Neurol20135545345823464736
  • AyalaROteroGAPorcayo MercadoRDelayed CNS maturation in iron-deficient anaemic infantsNutr Neurosci200811616818510805
  • BandhuRShankarNTandonOPEvent related potentials in anemic school-going girls of age group 8 to 10 yearsIndian J Physiol Pharmacol20115523424022471230
  • BatraJSoodAIron deficiency anaemia: effect on cognitive development in children: a reviewIndian J Clin Biochem20052011912523105543
  • CarterRCJacobsonJLBurdenMJIron deficiency anemia and cognitive function in infancyPediatrics2010126e427e43420660551
  • De Andraca OyarzunISalas AliagaIde la Parra CieciwaAMother-child interaction and child behavior in preschool children with a history of iron-deficiency anemia in infancyArch Lat Nutr199343191198
  • DeinardASListALindgrenBCognitive deficits in iron-deficient and iron-deficient anemic childrenJ Pediatr19861086816893701513
  • GlazerYBilenkoNEffect of iron deficiency and iron deficiency anemia in the first two years of life on cognitive and mental development during childhoodHarefuah201014930931433520929071
  • OlsonCLAcostaLPHochbergNSAnemia of inflammation is related to cognitive impairment among children in Leyte, the PhilippinesPLoS Neglect Trop D20093e533
  • PetranovicDBatinacTPetranovicDIron deficiency anaemia influences cognitive functionsMed Hypotheses200870707217574345
  • SungthongRMo-suwanLChongsuvivatwongVEffects of haemoglobin and serum ferritin on cognitive function in school childrenAsia Pac J Clin Nutr20021111712212074177
  • AgaogluLTorunOUnuvarEEffects of iron deficiency anemia on cognitive function in childrenArzneimittel-Forschung20075742643017691592
  • BlantonCAGreenMWKretschMJBody iron is associated with cognitive executive planning function in college womenBr J Nutr201310990691322676919
  • BeardJLIron deficiency and neural development: an updateArch Lat Nutr19994934S39S
  • Beltran-NavarroBMatuteEVasquez-GaribayEEffect of chronic iron deficiency on neuropsychological domains in infantsJ Child Neurol20122729730321876067
  • SoewondoSHusainiMPollittEEffects of iron deficiency on attention and learning processes in preschool children: Bandung, IndonesiaAm J Clin Nutr1989506676742773844
  • WalterTEffect of iron-deficiency anemia on cognitive skills and neuromaturation in infancy and childhoodFood Nutr Bull200324S104S11017016952
  • DissanayakeDSKumarasiriPVRNugegodaDBThe association of iron status with educational performance and intelligence among adolescentsCeylon Med J200954757919999786
  • EdenANIron deficiency and impaired cognition in toddlers: an underestimated and undertreated problemPaediatr Drugs2005734735216356022
  • FuglestadAJGeorgieffMKIversonSLIron deficiency after arrival is associated with general cognitive and behavioral impairment in post-institutionalized children adopted from Eastern EuropeMatern Child Health J2013171080108722872286
  • LewisSJBonillaCBrionMJMaternal iron levels early in pregnancy are not associated with offspring IQ score at age 8, findings from a mendelian randomization studyEur J Clin Nutr20146849650224398642
  • SchiepersOJGvan BoxtelMPJde GrootRHMSerum iron parameters, hfe c282y genotype, and cognitive performance in older adults: results from the FACIT studyJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci2010651312132120813792
  • LozoffBArmony-SivanRKacirotiNEyeblinking rates are slower in infants with iron-deficiency anemia than in nonanemic iron-deficient or iron-sufficient infantsJ Nutr20101401057106120335633
  • LozoffBEarly iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunctionJ Nutr2011141740S746S21346104
  • McCannJCAmesBNAn overview of evidence for a causal relation between iron deficiency during development and deficits in cognitive or behavioral functionAm J Clin Nutr20078593194517413089
  • ThomasDGGrantSLAubuchon-EndsleyNLThe role of iron in neurocognitive developmentDev Neuropsychol20093419622219267295
  • Murray-KolbLEIron status and neuropsychological consequences in women of reproductive age: what do we know and where are we headed?J Nutr2011141747S755S21346109
  • AshbyDCan iron supplementation improve cognitive functioning?Lancet19963489738855847
  • BerglundSKWestrupBHagglofBEffects of iron supplementation of lbw infants on cognition and behavior at 3 yearsPediatrics2013131475523230066
  • BhandariNBahlRTanejaSEffect of micronutrient supplementation on linear growth of childrenBr J Nutr200185S131S13711509101
  • BrunerABJoffeADugganAKRandomised study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in non-anaemic iron-deficient adolescent girlsLancet19963489929968855856
  • CongdonELWesterlundAAlgarinCRIron deficiency in infancy is associated with altered neural correlates of recognition memory at 10 yearsJ Pediatr20121601027103322244466
  • DevakiPBChandraRKGeisserPEffects of oral iron(iii) hydroxide polymaltose complex supplementation on hemoglobin increase, cognitive function, affective behavior and scholastic performance of adolescents with varying iron status: a single centre prospective placebo controlled studyArzneimittel-Forschung20095930331019634512
  • EbenezerRGunawardenaKKumarendranBCluster-randomised trial of the impact of school-based deworming and iron supplementation on the cognitive abilities of schoolchildren in Sri Lanka’s plantation sectorTrop Med Int Health20131894295123837404
  • HarahapHJahariABHusainiMAEffects of an energy and micronutrient supplement on iron deficiency anemia, physical activity and motor and mental development in undernourished children in IndonesiaEur J Clin Nutr200054S114S11910902994
  • SungthongRMo-suwanLChongsuvivatwongVOnce-weekly and 5-days a week iron supplementation differentially affect cognitive function but not school performance in Thai childrenJ Nutr20041342349235415333727
  • ChristianPMurray-KolbLEKhatrySKPrenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in NepalJAMA20103042716272321177506
  • FrielJKAzizKAndrewsWLA doublemasked, randomized control trial of iron supplementation in early infancy in healthy term breast-fed infantsJ Pediatr200314358258614615726
  • LozoffBJimenezEHagenJPoorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancyPediatrics2000105E5110742372
  • SachdevHGeraTNestelPEffect of iron supplementation on mental and motor development in children: systematic review of randomised controlled trialsPublic Health Nutr2005811713215877905
  • YalcinSSYurdakokKAcikgozDShort-term developmental outcome of iron prophylaxis in infantsPediatr Int20004262563011192518
  • MatiashviliKManjavidzeNGhonghadzeTInfluence of ferrotherapy on psychomotor development of children of yearly age with iron deficiency anemiaGeorgian Med News2012206384122870835
  • Metallinos-KatsarasEValassi-AdamEDeweyKGEffect of iron supplementation on cognition in Greek preschoolersEur J Clin Nutr2004581532154215226754
  • Salinas-PielagoJEVega-DienstmaierJMRojas-OblitasMEffect of biscuits fortified with haem iron on the intellectual status of pre-school childrenRev Neurol1998274004049774808
  • IdjradinataPPollittEReversal of developmental delays in iron-deficient anaemic infants treated with ironLancet19933418836147678046
  • ThompsonJBiggsBAPasrichaSREffects of daily iron supplementation in 2- to 5-year-old children: systematic review and meta-analysisPediatrics201313173975323478873
  • WangBZhanSGongTIron therapy for improving psychomotor development and cognitive function in children under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemiaCochrane Database Syst Rev20136CD00144423744449
  • SenAKananiSJImpact of iron-folic acid supplementation on cognitive abilities of school girls in VadodaraIndian Pediatrics20094613714319242031
  • SiegelEHKordasKStoltzfusRJInconsistent effects of iron-folic acid and/or zinc supplementation on the cognitive development of infantsJ Health Popul Nutr20112959360422283033
  • SolonFSSarolJNJrBernardoABIEffect of a multiple-micronutrient-fortified fruit powder beverage on the nutrition status, physical fitness, and cognitive performance of schoolchildren in the PhilippinesFood Nutr Bull200324S129S14017016955
  • TaljaardCCovicNMvan GraanAEEffects of a multimicronutrient-fortified beverage, with and without sugar, on growth and cognition in South African schoolchildren: a randomised, double-blind, controlled interventionBr J Nutr20131102271228423823584
  • BaumgartnerJSmutsCMMalanLEffects of iron and n-3 fatty acid supplementation, alone and in combination, on cognition in school children: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled intervention in South AfricaAm J Clin Nutr2012961327133823097272
  • KhedrEHamedSAElbeihEIron states and cognitive abilities in young adults: neuropsychological and neurophysiological assessmentEur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci200825848949618574611
  • Murray-KolbLEBeardJLIron treatment normalizes cognitive functioning in young womenAm J Clin Nutr20078577878717344500
  • LeonardAJChalmersKACollinsCEA study of the effects of latent iron deficiency on measures of cognition: a pilot randomised controlled trial of iron supplementation in young womenNutrients201462419243524959952
  • AmesBNThe metabolic tune-up: metabolic harmony and disease preventionJ Nutr20031331544S1548S12730462
  • PollittEThe developmental and probabilistic nature of the functional consequences of iron-deficiency anemia in childrenJ Nutr2001131669S675S11160597
  • YoudimMBYehudaSThe neurochemical basis of cognitive deficits induced by brain iron deficiency: involvement of dopamine-opiate systemCell Mol Biol20004649150010872737
  • LozoffBBeardJConnorJLong-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancyNutr Rev200664S34S43 discussion S72–S9116770951
  • Armony-SivanRKaplan-EstrinMJacobsonSWIron-deficiency anemia in infancy and mother-infant interaction during feedingJ Dev Behav Pediatr20103132633220431398
  • LozoffBCastilloMClarkKMIron supplementation in infancy contributes to more adaptive behaviour at 10 years of ageJ Nutr201414483884524717366
  • YehudaSYehudaMLong lasting effects of infancy iron deficiency-preliminary resultsJ Neurol Trans Suppl200671197200
  • ConnollyKMSupplementation versus food fortification. The unfounded disparity in perceptionTotal Health2011 Available from: http://www.totalhealthmagazine.com/articles/diet/supplementation-versus-food-fortification-the-unfounded-disparity-in-perception.htmlAccessed September 22, 2014
  • DomellofMBenefits and harms of iron supplementation in iron-deficient and iron-sufficient childrenNestle Nutr Workshop Ser Pediatr Program201065153165
  • LoganSMartinsSGilbertRIron therapy for improving psychomotor development and cognitive function in children under the age of three with iron deficiency anaemiaCochrane Database Syst Rev20012CD00144411405989
  • FalkinghamMAbdelhamidACurtisPThe effects of oral iron supplementation on cognition in older children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysisNutr J20109420100340
  • KrebsNFDietary zinc and iron sources, physical growth and cognitive development of breastfed infantsJ Nutr2000130358S360S10721906