638
Views
38
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Smartphone viewing distance and sleep: an experimental study utilizing motion capture technology

, , , , &
Pages 59-65 | Published online: 08 Mar 2017

Abstract

There are studies reporting the negative impact of smartphone utilization on sleep. It is considered that reduction of melatonin secretion under the blue light exposure from smart-phone displays is one of the causes. The viewing distance may cause sleep disturbance, because the viewing distance determines the screen illuminance and/or asthenopia. However, to date, there has been no study closely investigating the impact of viewing distance on sleep; therefore, we sought to determine the relationship between smartphone viewing distance and subjective sleep status. Twenty-three nursing students (mean age ± standard deviation of 19.7±3.1 years) participated in the study. Subjective sleep status was assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, morningness–eveningness questionnaire, and the Epworth sleepiness scale. We used the distance between the head and the hand while holding a smartphone to measure the viewing distance while using smartphones in sitting and lying positions. The distance was calculated using the three-dimensional coordinates obtained by a noncontact motion-sensing device. The viewing distance of smartphones in the sitting position ranged from 13.3 to 32.9 cm among participants. In the lying position, it ranged from 9.9 to 21.3cm. The viewing distance was longer in the sitting position than in the lying position (mean ± standard deviation: 20.3±4.7 vs 16.4±2.7, respectively, P<0.01). We found that the short viewing distance in the lying position had a positive correlation to a poorer sleep state (R2=0.27, P<0.05), lower sleep efficiency (R2=0.35, P<0.05), and longer sleep latency (R2=0.38, P<0.05). Moreover, smartphone viewing distances in lying position correlated negatively with subjective sleep status. Therefore, when recommending ideal smartphone use in lying position, one should take into account the viewing distances.

Introduction

With the dramatic progress of technology and the growing number of software developments, mobile devices have become thoroughly integrated into our daily lives. Smart-phones are used not only for telecommunications but also for various leisure activities, such as net surfing, social networking, games, and taking photos/videos. Due to the reliance on and heavy usage of such devices, people’s lifestyles have greatly changed.

Besides the positive aspects of such devices, we need to take into consideration the negative aspects of their use. A growing amount of evidence suggests that there are potential negative impacts from smartphones on biophysiological processes, especially on sleep. Munezawa et alCitation1 conducted a nationwide survey investigating the association between smartphone use and sleep among Japanese junior and high school students. They found that 84.4% of Japanese adolescents used smartphones every day and that 8.3% of them used smartphones even after they turned off the room lights. In addition, they found that using smartphones after they turned off the room lights related to sleep disturbance.

Melatonin is a hormone produced by the pineal gland at night and under conditions of darkness in both diurnal and nocturnal species. Therefore, exposure to light during the night can affect the production of melatonin. In fact, especially the short-wavelength of blue lights (380–495 nm) at night is reported to retard or even cease the production of nocturnal melatonin.Citation2Citation5 As melatonin plays an important role in controlling the night–day cycle, suppression of melatonin by light during the night has been implicated in the disruption of sleep. High-intensity displays, such as computers and smartphones, contain short wavelengths like a blue light. Therefore, the utilization of those displays could potentially have a negative impact on sleep.

In addition to the brightness of displays, viewing distance might play some role in lowering sleep quality. Bababekova et alCitation6 examined whether the viewing distance of smart-phones could alter asthenopia. They reported that the mean distance for text messaging and net surfing was shorter than a typical distance for viewing written material (36.2, 32.2, and 40 cm, respectively). Furthermore, they found that the closer distance increased demands on both accommodation and vergence, which could exacerbate the symptoms of asthenopia. Although their study added valuable knowledge to the literature, they did not investigate the influence of viewing distance on sleep. Regarding the viewing distance, we have investigated people’s body position when viewing smartphones elsewhere.Citation7 Based on our survey targeting 138 nursing students, we found that they used smartphones almost every night before sleep, even after they turned off the lights. The time spent in the sitting position was shorter than the lying position (P<0.05), and among those in the lying position, they favored the lateral lying position rather than the dorsal and/or prone positions (P<0.01).

Given that exposure to blue lights can have a negative impact on sleep quality and that the viewing distance may modify the magnitude of this impact, we sought to determine the relationship between the smartphone viewing distance and subjective sleep status (eg, sleep quality, sleep efficiency, and sleep latency). We first measured the smartphone viewing distances utilizing an infrared depth sensor. Next, we investigated the severity of sleep disturbance using multiple rating scales and looked for the relationship between the viewing distance and sleep disturbance. We hypothesized that a closer viewing distance would have a greater negative impact on sleep.

Participants and methods

All participants provided their written consent before enrollment in the study. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Aino University Junior College. This study was conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were compensated for this study.

Participants

Twenty-three nursing students (male 4/23, mean age ± SD =19.7±3.1 years) participated in the study. We recruited healthy volunteers, but a systematic examination of psychiatric or physical diseases that can alter sleep was not conducted. All participants owned a smartphone and reported that they use it before sleep.

Assessment of subjective sleep status

Subjective sleep status was assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI),Citation8,Citation9 morningness–eveningness questionnaire (MEQ),Citation10,Citation11 and the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS).Citation12Citation15

Determination of the viewing distance

Microsoft Kinect© for Windows was used for motion capture in order to determine participants’ viewing distance of smart-phones. We used the distance between the head and the hand while holding a smartphone to measure the viewing distance. This distance was calculated using the three-dimensional (3D) coordinates obtained by Kinect, a noncontact motion-sensing device. The Kinect sensor acquires depth information by measuring reflection patterns with an infrared camera sensor. Using the Kinect analysis software, we can capture images and track and recognize the human body. Several studies have shown that Kinect is a useful measurement tool for distance, as it provides accurate information with an error of only a few millimeters.Citation16,Citation17 We developed a custom program to measure viewing distances using Kinect. This program provides 3D coordinates of any body part which we can specify using the Kinect’s color images and skeleton data. Details of the materials used in this experiment are listed in . We considered that the bias would be minimal with this device because traditional motion capture systems require invasive sensors or electrical cords.

Table 1 Materials used in the experiment

Experimental conditions

We measured viewing distances while using smartphones in sitting and lying positions for all subjects. We had participants hold their phones for 14.5 minutes in sitting position and 22.3 minutes in lying position because in a previous study we had conducted, people answered that they would use their phones for the respective minutes on average.Citation7 The room lightness was 600 lx based on the eye position of participants. All participants used an Apple iPhone 5 with limited functionality to browse certain content with Google Chrome, but they could not check email, social network sites (SNSs) (such as Twitter and Facebook), or watch videos. They were all iPhone users and were all familiar with operating the phone. The luminance level of the smartphone display was fixed at 500 cd/m2, and participants were not allowed to change the luminance level. In the laboratory room, the temperature was 25±5°C and the humidity was 50±5%. Participants were asked to use the smartphone as they would normally use it. In the sitting position, participants sat in a chair (seat size: 360×360 cm and height: 46 cm) with a straight back without a desk. The Kinect was placed 180 cm vertically to the left side of the chair and 60 cm above the floor. In the lying position, subjects laid on a bed (bed size: 90×200 cm and height: 40 cm) in arbitrary lying positions (eg, dorsal position, prone position, and lateral position) to measure their viewing distance. We set up the Kinect 180 cm above the bed over the participants’ chest area in order to avoid occlusions (). The viewing distance was measured and recorded every 10 seconds by the Kinect for Windows SDK v1.6 program. The experiment was done between 1:30 pm and 5:30 pm. In order for participants to not be affected by the presence of the researcher in the room, a partition was placed between participant and researcher.

Figure 1 The layout for the experimental settings.

Notes: (A) layout for lying position. (B) Layout for sitting position.
Figure 1 The layout for the experimental settings.

Experimental procedures

Participants were randomly assigned to start in either sitting position or lying position. Viewing distance in the sitting/lying position was measured for the length of time mentioned before, which was followed by a 5-minute interval period, after which the viewing distance in the other position was measured. After the experiment, questionnaires were administered, which included demographic characteristics of the participants and a subjective sleep status using multiple rating scales, ie, PSQI, MEQ, and ESS.

Illuminance measurement by multiple distances/angles

As a separate experiment, we investigated the illuminance and spectrum for the light emitted from the smartphone display. In order to take into consideration the decrease in the illuminance in relation to the viewing distance, we measured at distances of 10, 20, and 30 cm. At each distance, we faced the smartphone forward 90, 120, and 150°. The screen image was set at the default Google search engine website in smartphones. The illuminance was measured three times at each distance and the mean was calculated. We measured the experiment room in a blackout situation for 0 lx.

Statistical analyses

Differences between viewing distances in each body position were examined with a paired t-test. For the relationship between viewing distance and subjective sleep status, we used multiple regression analyses. As dependent variables, we used sleep state, sleep efficiency, sleep latency (determined from the PSQI score), circadian rhythm type (determined from the MEQ score), and daytime sleepiness (determined from the ESS score). As predictor variables, we included age, sex, visual acuity, the viewing distance in the sitting position, and the viewing distance in the lying position. All analyses were performed using SPSS 11.0J for Windows.

Results

Sleep scale scores and viewing distances

The mean ± SD scores of PSQI, ESS, and MEQ were 5.82±1.76, 10.95±3.59, and 44.05±7.81, respectively.

The average viewing distances

The smartphone viewing distance in the sitting position ranged from 13.3 to 32.9 cm among participants. In the lying position, it ranged from 9.9 to 21.3 cm. The mean ± SD of viewing distance was longer in the sitting position than in the lying position (20.3±4.7 vs 16.4±2.7, respectively, t(21)=3.604, P<0.01).

The relationship between viewing distance and subjective sleep status

Based on multiple regression analyses, we found that the short viewing distance in the lying position related positively to a poorer sleep state (R2=0.27, P<0.05), lower sleep efficiency (R2=0.35, P<0.05), and longer sleep latency (R2=0.38, P<0.05). On the other hand, there were no significant associations between the sitting viewing distance and any of the sleep variables ().

Table 2 The relationship between the viewing distance and sleep determined by PSQI, MEQ, and ESS

Illumination intensity of smartphones

Within the spectrum of the smartphone light, the peak was at 453 nm (). As shown in , the illuminances decreased as the smartphone was moved farther away. Regarding the angle of the smartphone, the illuminance was the highest when the smartphone was directly facing the illuminometer (90°) as opposed to when it was placed at tilted positions (120° and 150°). Based on the findings of the mean distances of smartphone viewing distances in sitting and lying positions and assuming that the viewing angle ranged from 90 to 120°, the calculated illuminance was from 25.3 to 42.6 lx when sitting and from 50.5 to 80.4 lx when lying.

Figure 2 Spectral radiometric profile of the smartphone (measured at 90°/10 cm from iPhone 5).

Abbreviation: CCD, Charge-Coupled Device.
Figure 2 Spectral radiometric profile of the smartphone (measured at 90°/10 cm from iPhone 5).

Figure 3 Illuminance of smartphone at various distances and angles.

Notes: Gray circles and diamonds show the illuminances at the mean distances measured in this experiment.
Figure 3 Illuminance of smartphone at various distances and angles.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study investigating the viewing distance of smartphones before sleeping and the relationship with subjective sleep status. We found that the viewing distance in the lying position was shorter than that in the sitting position. Based on the findings of measured illuminances from smartphones placed at multiple distances, it was estimated that students were exposed to 50.5–80.4 lx of lights, with the peak spectrum beinĝ453 nm. Moreover, based on a multiple regression analysis, only the distance of smartphones in the lying position predicted a poorer sleep state (R2=0.27, P<0.05), lower sleep efficiency (R2=0.35, P<0.05), and longer sleep latency (R2=0.38, P<0.05) as assessed by PSQI. These findings suggest that people are exposed to light through smartphone monitors in high illuminance that mainly consists of blue light. It is of note that the viewing distance also correlated negatively with the subjective sleep status. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) express photopigment melanopsin, and ipRGCs project to several brain nuclei, including the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN).Citation18 The SCN sends information to the pineal gland to modulate body temperature and produce hormones such as cortisol and melatonin.Citation19Citation21 Because melanopsin photoreceptors reach peak light absorption at blue light wavelengthŝ480 nm, it is thought that blue light affects the circadian rhythms.Citation18

In 1996, Boivin et alCitation22 reported that even low intensity light (~180 lx) can affect circadian rhythm based on the body temperature measurement, whereas it had been thought that bright light (~7000–13000 lx) was needed to synchronize circadian rhythm in humans. Boivin and CzeislerCitation23 confirmed the findings by showing the effect of light on melatonin suppression in 1998. In 2000, Brainard et alCitation24 reported that the spectrum of 446–477 nm was the most potent wavelength region in providing circadian input for the regulation of melatonin secretion. Thapan et alCitation25 also confirmed this finding. They reported that a spectrum of 457–462 nm suppressed melatonin secretion the most. Since then, there has been increasing evidential support for the theory that blue light – even when the illuminance is low – can affect sleep. Lockley et alCitation26 compared 460 nm 5 lx light and 555 nm 68.1 lx light and found that the former light quality suppressed melatonin more. Warman et alCitation27 showed that short-wavelength light (~460 nm) even as low as 8 lx induced a melatonin phase shift that was seen when exposed to 12000 lx white light.

Since smartphone monitors emit blue light, there are multiple studies that have investigated the negative impact of smartphones on sleep. Chang et alCitation3 reported that evening use of an e-Reader phase delays the circadian clock and suppresses melatonin secretion. They conducted a randomized crossover study with 12 healthy adults to compare the impact of light-emitting devices with a printed book, on sleep. They found that light-emitting devices suppressed evening melatonin levels by 55.12±20.12%, whereas the printed book did not (–18.77±98.57%). Moreover, based on polysomnography (PSG) and the Karolinska sleepiness scale, light-emitting devices decreased rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and decreased sleepiness in the evening. Figueiro et alCitation4 investigated the impact of light emitted from computer monitors. When participants were exposed to 40 lx short-wavelength light from blue light-emitting goggles while watching a computer monitor, their melatonin levels were significantly reduced. When participants were exposed only to the computer monitors, the melatonin reduction was not as great as when wearing goggles, but a reduction was still seen compared to when they viewed the monitors through orange-tinted glasses. Wood et alCitation5 conducted a similar study that investigated the impact of the light emitted from tablet displays, such as iPad2s. They reported that participants’ melatonin levels decreased when they were exposed to the tablet’s light as compared to the control conditions, although the effect was not as strong as when they were exposed to tablets and blue light-emitting goggles.

Although these experiments enriched our knowledge of how smartphones and/or tablets can affect sleep, these experiments were mainly conducted in an experimental environment and may not predict real-world usage of these devices. Most of the studies described earlier fixed the distance of the smartphone to 30–50 cm, whereas our study showed that the average viewing distance in the real-world was 20.3 cm when sitting and 16.4 cm when lying. These statistics were determined based on markerless motion capture techniques where the subjects were not affected by experimental equipment and were able to exhibit their natural behavior.

The results of this study need to be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. First, we did not measure the duration of participants’ smartphone usage, which can affect sleep quality given the findings of previous studies. We only recruited students who use smartphones before sleeping every night; however, the duration of use may be different day-to-day. Because we investigated the participants’ sleep quality with rating scales, which assess the examinee’s subjective sleep in the past month, it would not be practical to take into consideration a duration of usage that could fluctuate every day. Relating to this, the rating scales that were used in the study assess not the state, but rather trait or usual sleep habits of the subjective sleep status of the past month. In this study, it was impossible to examine the direct influence of the viewing distance on the subject’s quality of sleep; rather, this study is suited to examining the relationship between normal smartphone-viewing habits and subjective sleep quality. Second, we did not measure melatonin levels, electroencephalogram (EEG), and/or other biological indices. The focus of this study was to investigate the viewing distance of smartphones and its impact on subjective sleep status in daily life, and trying to take such measurements would have disrupted the participants’ natural usage of smartphones which we were attempting to observe. Third, we prepared bedding such as sheets and comforters for the research, and this unfamiliar bedding could have affected the participants’ body position, as opposed to how they would sit/lay in their own beds. Moreover, we measured the distance only in two body positions – namely sitting and lying – but some people may prefer other positions. In addition, we set the room illuminance as 600 lx in the experiment. The brightness of each participant’s room at home will differ, which might have some influence on the viewing distance. Fourth, we did not rule out potential psychiatric and/or physical diseases that can alter sleep. Although we recruited healthy volunteers for this study, some of them might have had such diseases and could have biased the results of the study. Finally, we conducted this study in the daytime.

Conclusion

We investigated the viewing distance of smartphones by using markerless motion capture techniques. We found that the distances were shorter in the lying position than in the sitting position, and altogether, these distances were shorter than those observed in previous studies. Moreover, smartphone distances correlated negatively with subjective sleep status. Therefore, when recommending ideal smartphone use, one should take into account the viewing distances.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Mr Tsukasa Sugiura for his very helpful suggestions about programming the Kinect.

Disclosure

Taishiro Kishimoto has received consultant fees from Dainippon Sumitomo, Novartis, and Otsuka and has received speaker’s honoraria from Banyu, Eli Lilly, Dainippon Sumitomo, Janssen, Novartis, Otsuka, and Pfizer, outside the submitted work. He has also received grant support from the Pfizer Health Research, Takeda, Tanabe-Mitsubishi, Dainippon-Sumitomo, Otsuka, and Mochida, outside the submitted work. Masaru Mimura has received grants and/or speaker’s honoraria from Abbvie, Asahi Kasei, Astellas, Chugai, Cracie, Daiichi Sankyo, Dainippon-Sumitomo, Eisai, Eli Lilly, Fuji Film, Janssen, Meiji Pharma, Mochida, MSD, Novartis, Ono, Otsuka, Pfizer, Shionogi, Takeda, and Yoshitomi Pharmaceutical within the past 3 years, outside the submitted work. Kazuo Tsubota has received grants from Jins Co., Ltd, Toshiba Materials, and Tsubota Lav, outside the submitted work; in addition, he has a patent Tsubota Lab., Inc., pending and a patent Jins Co., Ltd., pending. The authors report no other conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • MunezawaTKaneitaYOsakiYThe association between use of smartphones after lights out and sleep disturbances among Japanese adolescents: a nationwide cross-sectional surveySleep20113481013102021804663
  • CajochenCFreySAndersDEvening exposure to a light-emitting diode (LED)-backlit computer screen affects circadian physiology and cognitive performanceJ Appl Physiol201111051432143821415172
  • ChangAMAeschbachDDuffyJFCzeislerCAEvening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertnessProc Natl Acad Sci U S A201411241232123725535358
  • FigueiroMGWoodBPlitnickBReaMSThe impact of light from computer monitors on melatonin levels in college studentsNeuro Endocrinol Lett201132215816321552190
  • WoodBReaMSPlitnickBFigueiroMGLight level and duration of exposure determine the impact of self-luminous tablets on melatonin suppressionAppl Ergon201344223724022850476
  • BababekovaYRosenfieldMHueJEHuangRRFont size and viewing distance of handheld smartphonesOptom Vis Sci201188779579721499163
  • YoshimuraMKitazawaMKishimotoTMimuraMTsubotaKA survey of Japanese young adults’ postures when using smartphones before sleepingJ Mobile Technol Med2016525153
  • BuysseDJReynoldsCFMonkTHBermanSRKupferDJThe Pitts-burgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and researchPsychiatry Res19892821932132748771
  • DoiYMinowaMUchiyamaMPsychometric assessment of subjective sleep quality using the Japanese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI-J) in psychiatric disordered and control subjectsPsychiatry Res20009716517211166088
  • HorneJAÖstbergOA self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythmsInt J Chronobiol19764971101027738
  • IshiharaKMiyashitaAInugamiMFukudaKYamazakiKMiyataYThe results of investigation by the Japanese version of Morningness–Eveningness QuestionnaireShinrigaku Kenkyu198657287913784166
  • JohnsMWA new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scaleSleep19911465405451798888
  • JohnsMWReliability and factor analysis of the Epworth sleepiness scaleSleep19921543763811519015
  • JohnsMHockingBDaytime sleepiness and sleep habits of Australian workersSleep199720108448499415943
  • TakegamiMSuzukamoYWakitaTDevelopment of a Japanese version of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (JESS) based on item response theorySleep Med200910555656518824408
  • KhoshelhamKElberinkSOAccuracy and resolution of Kinect depth data for indoor mapping applicationsSensors (Basel)20121221437145422438718
  • KurilloGHanJJObdržálekSUpper extremity reachable workspace evaluation with KinectStud Health Technol Inform201318424725323400165
  • LucasRJPeirsonSNBersonDMMeasuring and using light in the melanopsin ageTrends Neurosci20143711924287308
  • AltunAUgur-AltunBMelatonin: therapeutic and clinical utilizationInt J Clin Pract200761583584517298593
  • KräuchiKCajochenCWerthEWirz-JusticeAFunctional link between distal vasodilation and sleep-onset latency?Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol20002783R741R74810712296
  • SaperCBLuJChouTCGooleyJThe hypothalamic integrator for circadian rhythmsTrends Neurosci200528315215715749169
  • BoivinDBDuffyJFKronauerRECzeislerCADose-response relationships for resetting of human circadian clock by lightNature199637965655405428596632
  • BoivinDBCzeislerCAResetting of circadian melatonin and cortisol rhythms in humans by ordinary room lightNeuroreport1998957797829579664
  • BrainardGCRollagMDHanifinJPVan den BeldGSanfordBThe effect of polarized versus nonpolarized light on melatonin regulation in humansPhotochem Photobiol200071676677010857374
  • ThapanKArendtJSkeneDJAn action spectrum for melatonin suppression: evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humansJ Physiol2001535pt 126126711507175
  • LockleySWBrainardGCCzeislerCAHigh sensitivity of the human circadian melatonin rhythm to resetting by short wavelength lightJ Clin Endocrinol Metab20038894502450512970330
  • WarmanVLDijkDJWarmanGRArendtJSkeneDJPhase advancing human circadian rhythms with short wavelength lightNeurosci Lett2003342374012727312