714
Views
25
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Spotlight on dream recall: the ages of dreams

, , &
Pages 1-12 | Published online: 09 Jan 2018

Abstract

Brain and sleep maturation covary across different stages of life. At the same time, dream generation and dream recall are intrinsically dependent on the development of neural systems. The aim of this paper is to review the existing studies about dreaming in infancy, adulthood, and the elderly stage of life, assessing whether dream mentation may reflect changes of the underlying cerebral activity and cognitive processes. It should be mentioned that some evidence from childhood investigations, albeit still weak and contrasting, revealed a certain correlation between cognitive skills and specific features of dream reports. In this respect, infantile amnesia, confabulatory reports, dream-reality discerning, and limitation in language production and emotional comprehension should be considered as important confounding factors. Differently, growing evidence in adults suggests that the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the encoding and retrieval of episodic memories may remain the same across different states of consciousness. More directly, some studies on adults point to shared neural mechanisms between waking cognition and corresponding dream features. A general decline in the dream recall frequency is commonly reported in the elderly, and it is explained in terms of a diminished interest in dreaming and in its emotional salience. Although empirical evidence is not yet available, an alternative hypothesis associates this reduction to an age-related cognitive decline. The state of the art of the existing knowledge is partially due to the variety of methods used to investigate dream experience. Very few studies in elderly and no investigations in childhood have been performed to understand whether dream recall is related to specific electrophysiological pattern at different ages. Most of all, the lack of longitudinal psychophysiological studies seems to be the main issue. As a main message, we suggest that future longitudinal studies should collect dream reports upon awakening from different sleep states and include neurobiological measures with cognitive performances.

Introduction

Several issues still puzzle researchers who try to define the qualitative and quantitative features of dream production. The most promising approach highlights the neurobiological basis and cognitive mechanisms involved in dream experience. Considering brain sleep covarying maturation, from fetal development to early childhood, going through adulthood and elderly, some authors have hypothesized a role for dream content and dream recall-related neural correlates in the development of cognitive processes.Citation1Citation7 In this paper, we will review the available evidence on changes in dream recall across the life span, focusing mainly on a cognitive and neurobiological level, and try to pinpoint its complexity and hypothetical role in overall mental development (ie, at a cognitive, emotional, and neural levels). We will try to address how the neurobiological and psychological understanding of the ontogeny of sleep mentation contributes to our understanding of the cognitive and emotional development of a human being.

One of the most recognized explanatory hypotheses of dreaming is the so-called “continuity hypothesis” between waking and sleep mental functioning. First proposed by Bell and Hall in 1971Citation8,Citation9 as the recurrence of thematic content from daily experience in the dreams of their patients, it aimed to study dream recall as a direct psychological correlate of personality and emotional features of daily mental activity.Citation8Citation12 From a different perspective, the hypothesis posits a “continuum” between dreaming and neurobiological and cognitive functioning.Citation12Citation14 It should be underlined that these two viewpoints are not mutually exclusive, and we will consider both perspectives in our review.

In dream research, the methodological limitation to reliably access the mental sleep activity (ie, the impossibility to directly observe what is happening in our minds)Citation15,Citation16 makes the definition of dreaming difficult. The scientists interested in dream experience refer to “dream generation” as the precise moment in which dreams are shaped (producing the “sleep mentation”).Citation15,Citation16 Furthermore, the dream recall studies are directly focused on the retrieval of dream contents upon the awakenings, considering dream recall as a peculiar form of episodic-declarative memory.

The very beginning of a systematic study of dreaming traditionally refers to the discovery of rapid eye movement (REM) sleepCitation15Citation19 and the consequent notion that sleep mentation is a phenomenon restricted to that physiological state. This correspondence has been subsequently confuted by Foulkes,Citation20 who found that sleep mentation in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep simply changes the way we approach it, by defining it not just as story-like contents but as “anything passing through your mind”.Citation20 This precaution led to the discovery of presence of sleep mentation also in NREM sleep, opening new perspectives in methods and theories about dream experience analysis and its neural correlates.Citation25Citation28 Moreover, SolmsCitation25,Citation26 in a review of cases concerning global or partial cessation of dreaming subsequent to the brain lesions (termed as “anoneria”) found no direct relationship between REM sleep and dream recall. Specifically, no correlation was found between lesions in brainstem regions that modulate REM sleep generation and dream recall cessation, while focal cortical lesions can result in dream recall disappearing in a context of still preserved REM sleep production.Citation25 As a result, different theoretical approaches have been developed in the attempt to define the electroencephalographic (EEG) correlates of dream recall.Citation20Citation26 Still far from solving the problem, most investigators moved the dichotomy from REM/NREM sleep mentation to the qualitative difference of dream report in each sleep stage.Citation32 A basic limitation of these models is to assume dream content to be generated at the same stage upon which the subject has been awakened, while the transition from sleep to wakefulness is associated to physiological changes that make understanding the relationship between dream content and its neural correlates difficult.Citation20,Citation21 To overcome this perspective, dream studies have been focused on the search for a parallelism between psychophysiological and neural correlates of dream recall and declarative memory processes, its encoding, and recollection.Citation21,Citation27Citation35 As a result, sleep mentation has been further evaluated by analyzing the last segment of the EEG activity in correspondence with provoked awakenings, to associate EEG patterns and dream recall occurrence.Citation26Citation36

Tools and trends in dream research

Bearing in mind that dream experience is a peculiar object of study, it is necessary to take into account the limitations of the different methods used in dream research.

Investigations on dream content usually use three different self-report techniques: retrospective questionnaires, dream diaries, and prospective reports.Citation37Citation39 Retrospective methods collect past experiences of dreaming spanning periods of variable length (weeks, months, or years).Citation39,Citation40 Nonetheless the advantage of quick data collection from large samples and with low expenses, these methods have a strong memory bias due to the possible influence of everyday activity in the retrieving of quantitative and qualitative dream features.Citation38 Prospective methods concern daily diaries for a short period or, less frequently, a written report of any dream recalled.Citation39 Its advantage consists of an improvement in dream recall rates and more detailed reports,Citation37 allowing longitudinal studies. It also is less influenced by memory biases.Citation37Citation41

Similarly, provoked awakenings associated with dream report collection partly overcome most of the known biases in memory recollection. Moreover, this strategy allows reliable time coupling with polysomnographic (PSG) recordings, with consequent advantages due to the possibility to control over the time of recollection and the sleep stage of awakening. For these reasons, it remains the gold standard in dreams studies.Citation37,Citation41,Citation42 However, sleep laboratory studies may affect dream contents, as can be observed by the frequent incorporation of the experimental setting.Citation41 For this reason, it has been suggested to consider the known “first night effect” not just for what concerns the influence on sleep quality but also for a control over the quality of dream report.Citation43

For what concerns the qualitative features of dreams, the most common method is the coding system developed by Hall and van de Castle in 1966Citation44 in its recent revisions and updates,Citation45Citation47 which systematically investigates some features of dream content (ie, characters, social interactions and activities, success and failure, misfortune and good fortune, emotions, settings, objects, and descriptive elements) through categories and Likert scales.Citation44Citation47 Additionally, these methods permit to look for the memory sources of dreams, searching for a correspondence between dream reports and contents recorded by the subjects in a diary of daytime events.Citation48Citation51 In other words, some dream experiences could incorporate waking events.Citation48Citation51

Dream features, as well as quantitative features of sleep, undergo the influence of circadian rhythms.Citation52Citation57 For this reason, multiple nighttime awakening methods and daytime nap protocols have been developed to understand how dream content, as well as EEG patterns, are influenced by the sleep stage or by the time of the day.Citation58,Citation59

For what concern the morning awakenings, some investigations revealed that dream report features collected after a night of uninterrupted sleep at home are not dissimilar from those collected after night protocol in laboratory.Citation60,Citation61 Furthermore, it should be noted that circadian and ultradian variations could affect the characteristics of dream reports.Citation62 In this respect, qualitative features of dream experiences reported from NREM and REM awakenings in the second half of the night are similar and more ‘‘dreamlike’’. Conversely, dream contents reported from REM and NREM sleep stages at the beginning of the night show different qualitative features (ie, NREM dream reports are more “thought-like” than REM dream reports).Citation63Citation65

All these aspects must be taken into account for a complete comprehension and discussion about dreaming along different protocols.

Dreaming in childhood

Dream studies in infancy have been rarely focused on healthy subjects, being usually a report of bad dream frequency and dream content analysis in parasomnias or psychiatric conditions.Citation66Citation70

In the contest of systematic investigations about healthy cohorts, FoulkesCitation2Citation4 was one of the firsts to investigate dream content in children. In a laboratory study, he found dream frequency rates of 20% for children younger than 7 years, as compared to 80%–90% frequency rate in adults.Citation4 These results partly overlap latest investigations about home dream recall frequency in children compared to a group of young adults, in which the frequency of home dream by retrospective questionnaires was significantly lower in children.Citation71

In an attempt to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms involved in dreaming, FoulkesCitation2Citation4 explored the hypothesis that dream recall is directly reflected by cognitive maturation, in a correlational study between sleep mentation and performances in some neuropsychological tests.Citation2Citation4 Bearing in mind the impossibility to collect detailed dream reports in children below 3 years old, due to the so-called “infantile amnesia” (namely, the impossibility to report memories due to not yet developed memory neural systems),Citation72Citation74 he started from the assumption of a general limitation in language production and emotional comprehension as the key problem of a dream recall report in infancy.Citation75 More interestingly, while no correlation with language development measures was found,Citation2Citation4 the best predictor of dream recall was the Block Design Test (subtest of the Weschsler Intelligence Scale for Children [WISC])Citation76 which is known to be a reliable measure of visuospatial and organizational skills.Citation76,Citation77 The dependent variables in Block Design Test are usually accuracy and speed in completing each item.Citation76,Citation77 In particular, the subjects were asked to use hand movements to rearrange blocks, trying to match with a specific configuration.Citation76,Citation77

Due to these findings, the study of dream recall rates in children moved from a language-emotional level to an assessment of executive functions, with empirical evidence in favor of a correlation between mental imagery and sleep mentation.Citation4Citation6,Citation69

A renewed interest in dream content in childhood recently arose from studies measuring different cognitive skills in early childhood.Citation6,Citation7,Citation67,Citation68,Citation78 Specifically, Sàndor et alCitation6,Citation7 assessed the hypothesis of a relationship between executive abilities and dream content across development. The authors assessed the executive attention development in a group of 40 children from 4 to 8.5 years of age, administering the Stroop Test, the Emotional Stroop Test, and the Child Attention Network Test.Citation7 They found a positive association between the number of characters per dream and the selection of relevant stimuli in a distracting environment (Orienting Network). The number of self-initiated actions and the ratio of gross motor activities in dreams were significantly associated with the Incongruency Index of the Stroop Test.Citation7 Gross motor activities were associated with higher accuracy in the condition of incongruent stimuli at the Stroop Test. Both results were interpreted in terms of more efficient behavioral inhibitory control with more dreamer-involved actions. Additionally, the number of verbal actions per dream correlated positively with the Vocabulary Subtest of the WISC-IV.Citation7 Dreamer-initiated interactions per dream were associated with the behavioral inhibitory control functions, as measured by the Incongruency Index of the Stroop Test.Citation7 Friendly interactions per dream showed an association with the Emotional Interference Index of the Stroop Test, correlating positively with a more efficient control of emotional interference.Citation7 The general results point to a positive association between dream features and a better control over executive functions. In contrast with Foulkes’ study,Citation2Citation4 no correlation between visuospatial abilities (measured through the Block Design Subset – WISC-IV)Citation76,Citation77 and the number of recalled dreams has been found, even when a possible moderating effect of age was controlled. Also, Sàndor et alCitation7 did not find a correlation between dream bizarreness and some neuropsychological measures, partially in disagreement with previous findings by Colace,Citation79 who found a correlation between long-term memory and the bizarreness of dreams in children between 3 and 5 years of age.Citation79

Another perspective for studying sleep mentation in childhood is the general analysis of incidence of bad dreams.Citation40,Citation67Citation69 From studies about dream recall in pathological conditions, such as recurrent nightmares, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and severe trauma, empirical studies have shown an increased dream recall rates compared to healthy cohort, though no univocal explanation about the underlying mechanisms is available so far.Citation40,Citation66Citation70

Dream recall rates in traumatized children have been also investigated.Citation80Citation84 Studies in children living in conditions of war and military violence are in line with previous findings. Specifically, an association between stressful events and higher dream recall rate has been found in a group of Palestinian children aged 6–16 years who lived in a traumatized environment, compared to nontraumatized children.Citation84 The authors found enhanced dream recall and more contextual coherent images in dream content.Citation84 In addition, in children who suffered of PTSD because of a kidnap, the more the children had ability to verbalize their feelings, the more they dreamed about the trauma.Citation83 This is consistent with studies on verbal ability and dream recall frequency.Citation4Citation7,Citation67Citation69,Citation75,Citation83

When applied to memory for dreams, children with PTSD who have more unpleasant and traumatic dreams may have heightened dream recall because of the emotionally salient nature of their dreams.Citation67Citation93 These findings were interpreted in terms of a general sleep disruption and subsequent increased possibility of a post-nightmare awakening, which leads to increased dream recall.Citation84

The modification of memory performance across early childhood, due in the firsts ages to the so-called infantile amnesiaCitation72Citation74 and subsequently to the development of cognitive functions,Citation67 parallel the improvement of dream recall rates in children.

Nonetheless dream content and dream recall studies have been made both on healthy cohort and pathological samples at any age, there is no agreement about dream content qualitative features and dream recall rates in the life span among age groups.Citation5,Citation67Citation69,Citation72

This lack of a consistent literature about dream recall across ages that goes from early childhood to first adolescence is partly due to the difficulty in collecting reliable dream report from children. A low compliance in recording dream content may be due to the presence of a stranger (ie, the researcher) and can lead to a bias in reporting dreams.Citation5Citation7 Furthermore, dream reports in children are often collected in absence of EEG monitoring, considered the gold standard for a reliable time coupling between defined sleep stage awakenings and dream reports.Citation27 Consequently, the most used method in children’s dream research is the exclusive collection of dream reports.Citation5,Citation67Citation69 On one hand, it should be considered that the content analysis methods are widely defined in literature,Citation16,Citation44,Citation45,Citation85Citation88 while, on the other hand, the presence of different methods for collecting dream reports in children leads to additional biases, when a comparison with content analysis in adulthood is requested.Citation69 In fact, dream reports are more often collected at home by a caregiver instead of being reported to the researcher or directly by the child, which potentially misleads the actual dream content.Citation69

Interestingly, another issue in facing dream research in infancy is the achievement of the ability to discern a dream from reality, whose age definition is still controversial.Citation4,Citation69,Citation73,Citation89,Citation90 The ages in which children begin to report some dream content seem to parallel their cognitive maturation.Citation73 Some empirical evidence showed that children start at 4 years of age to discern dream from reality and reach a complete comprehension of this difference at the age of 9 years, when cognitive abilities are adequately matured.Citation4,Citation69,Citation73,Citation89,Citation90 At the same time, dream content analysis reveals a full development and adult-like content of dream experience from 9 years of age,Citation4,Citation89,Citation73,Citation91 coherent with mature mental imagery skills.Citation73,Citation92 Specifically, dream content at the age of 2–5 years shows a general absence of people, object, and events, being usually a static report of single characters. Longer reports, but with still low recall rate, are reached between ages 5 and 7 years. A first real development in dream content is reached at the age of 7 years, when the child starts to represent himself in the dream, with higher emotional content, active participation, and narrative structure, in dreams overall more frequent and longer.Citation4,Citation73,Citation89,Citation91

Considering these incongruences and the difficulty to bring together the few findings about dream production in children, we can only claim for further investigations about neuropsychological measures,Citation26 EEG waking mental activity,Citation94 and the EEG correlates of dreaming, with an aim to deepen the knowledge about the role of sleep mentation in brain maturation, for a better comparison with qualitative aspect of dream experience in future.

Dreaming in adulthood

A direct comparison between dream experience in infancy and adulthood cannot be addressed, due to the lack of studies focused on this topic. Actually, PSG recordings with provoked awakenings are the most commonly used method to investigate dream recall in adulthood. For this reason, we will mostly discuss the electrophysiology of dreaming while reviewing studies in this age range.

Studies about brain rhythms in wakefulness indirectly highlight the possible background of development in sleep mentation. The alpha (~8.00–11.75 Hz) and theta (~5.00–7.75 Hz) rhythms seem to be pivotal activities both in brain maturationCitation94 and in a successful dream recall.Citation27,Citation28

Brain maturation has been linked to changes in the alpha and theta activity across the life span.Citation94 Specifically, a progressive increase of the alpha activity in its absolute power has been observed from early childhood until the first adolescence, with a contemporary reduction in the absolute delta (~1.5–3.5 Hz) and theta power.Citation93 In infancy, the alpha rhythm maturation follows a posterior–anterior gradient in wakefulness.Citation94 This trend is mirrored during sleep onset in children between the ages of 0 and 48 months,Citation94 gaining attention due to the relevance of EEG rhythms in posterior areas for a successful dream recall in adulthood.Citation28 Additionally, the passage from resting states to cognitive performance in wakefulness is epitomized by the alpha rhythm suppression and by the shift to a prevalent theta activity.Citation94 The topographical changes of cortical oscillatory activity during sleep involve regions upon which relies a successful memory encoding in wakefulness.Citation96 Namely, from a neuropsychological to an electrophysiological level, dream recall seems to rely on mechanisms comparable to episodic-declarative memory functioning.Citation15,Citation21 The theta activity in its medial frontal gradient, also known as “frontal midline theta”, is involved in memory processesCitation29 and is considered predictive of cognitive performance while awake.Citation33,Citation97 Additionally, the theta activity has been found to be a fundamental property of hippocampus–prefrontal cortex connectivity.Citation98 Furthermore, Norman et alCitation99 suggested its role as a suppressor of the external interference with selective retrieval of target memory during cognitive tasks or in meditative state, and DomhoffCitation11 addressed its importance in the so-called “default mode network” functioning. In this context, common mechanisms in the processes underlying encoding and recall of episodic memories across different states of consciousness have been suggested.Citation21

Recent findings by Scarpelli et alCitation21,Citation27,Citation35 pointed to a state-dependent EEG background for a successful dream recall. This confirmed the pivotal importance in defining the electrophysiology of sleep mentation to solve the controversies of a functional (and developmental) meaning of dream experience. For what concerns the specific EEG patterns of dream recall, both Esposito et alCitation31 and Marzano et alCitation28 observed that a decreased temporoparietal alpha during NREM sleep is predictive of subsequent dream recall, while the theta oscillations in the last 5 minutes of REM sleep predict the subsequent dream recall both during napCitation27 and nighttimeCitation28 investigations. This seems also coherent with the reported relationship between the prefrontal theta activity during REM sleep and the significant positive consolidation of negative vs neutral emotional memories in a nap protocol.Citation96

Recent findings addressed the existence of a “hot zone” (ie, parietal occipital areas), whose low-frequency (1–4 Hz) decrease was predictive of dream recall, after both REM and NREM awakenings.Citation100 Consequently, a successful dream recall was considered dependent on the state of EEG activation.Citation100 Similarly, a previous within-subjects study had revealed that brain activity shows higher power in high-frequency range (gamma activity: 18–50 Hz), which is associated to the presence of recall on awakening both during REM and NREM sleep.Citation59

Despite the dichotomy, REM sleep dreaming is considered outdated; very few explanations on the encoding and retrieval of dream contents from NREM sleep have been proposed. In one of those, Scarpelli et alCitation35 again found a decreased delta power in temporo-parietal regions prior to a successful compared to an unsuccessful dream recall after NREM awakenings in a multiple nap protocol.Citation35 This is coherent with a decline of the delta and sigma EEG power over the centro-parietal regions in dream report from NREM sleep.Citation101 Augmented slow-wave sleep (SWS), together with a longer total sleep time and lower intrasleep awakenings in the recovery sleep after sleep deprivation, almost completely abolishes dream recall.Citation86 Moreover, an increased dream recall frequency has been observed in insomniacs, whose sleep is characterized by a raise in fast-frequency EEG activity,Citation102 as well as in normal sleeper with higher number of intrasleep wakefulness.Citation103,Citation104 In the same vein, the stimulation on the fronto-temporal area in the gamma band by transcranial direct current stimulation during REM sleep elicits lucid dreams.Citation105

However, there are still confounding findings that cannot lead to a univocal explanation of EEG activity and presence/absence of dreaming. Some studies reported a relationship between high-frequency activity and dream recall.Citation59,Citation105 Some of these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that an increased brain activity is necessary for the retrieval of sleep mentation.Citation52,Citation106Citation108 Therefore, it has been suggested that EEG activation could facilitate the consolidation and storage of the mental sleep contents during sleep.Citation35

For what concern the qualitative features of dream reports, dream bizarreness has been linked to cognitive processes and skills, not only in developmental but also in adult studies.Citation92,Citation109 Interestingly, dream bizarreness was negatively correlated with right hippocampus and left amygdala volumetry in healthy subjects.Citation87 At the same time, lower bizarreness and lower emotional load has been found in Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients with a hypodopaminergic state. Visual vividness has been found to positively correlate with the volume of amygdala and the thickness of the left medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in PD patients, while emotional load is positively correlated with hippocampal volume, suggesting a role of amygdala–mPFC dopaminergic network system in dream recall.Citation88 This partly supports the hypothesis of a role of paralimbic structures in the access and processing of dream contents.Citation43 According to this hypothesis, emotional salience of dream imagery is due to the activation of the paralimbic cortices by the amygdala,Citation87,Citation88 while bizarreness of dream contents is a consequence of prefrontal deactivation, reducing the higher order control on the amygdalar and hippocampal functioning.Citation110 It is well known that the amygdala and the hippocampal formation shows a relatively high metabolic activity during sleep.Citation111,Citation112 Additionally, activation of amygdala and hippocampus seems to play a pivotal role in the processing of emotional contents during REM sleep.Citation113 On the other hand, lesion studies suggested that the perceptual aspects of dreams would be related to the activation of posterior (occipital and temporal) cortices during REM sleep, and that the strong activation of high-order occipito-temporal visual cortex would explain the vivid visual imagery during dreams.Citation103,Citation104,Citation112 Furthermore, the cessation of dream production is associated with posterior cortical lesions, near the temporo-occipital–parietal junction or deep bilateral frontal damage, in correspondence with the ventro-mesial region.Citation11,Citation25

As already stated, direct investigations on cognitive performance and dream features in adulthood have been seldom carried out. However, early studies assessed the predictability of dream recall rates in relationship with visual memory measuresCitation114 and visuospatial abilities.Citation1 Specifically, Cory and OrmistonCitation114 compared a group of high recallers to a group of nonrecallers, selected from a sample of college students who completed the Sleep and Dream Survey, a self-administered questionnaire about sleep characteristics and dream information.Citation114 Then, they tested the memory for visual stimuli by using short- and long-term memory tasks, testing also for the incidental memory condition.Citation114 Considering the number of items recalled as a measure of memory performance, they showed that recallers reported better performance for visually presented stimuli in all three conditions than nonrecallers. Finally, the authors stated that memory performances to visually administered stimuli are predictive of dream recall rates.Citation114

Another work showed a correlation between dream recall rates after REM sleep awakenings and visuospatial abilities.Citation1 As already seen in infancy,Citation2,Citation3,Citation7 adult subjects with better performance at the Block Design Test also reported more dreams than low performers.Citation1 Lately, Dumel et alCitation115 aimed to assess the gain at the mirror tracing task (a visual discrimination, visuospatial motricity, and eye–hand coordination task, assumed to be REM sleep sensitive) in high and low recallers. Coherently with the expectations, the low recallers reported worst performance at the baseline in this task compared to high recallers.

Moreover, Nielsen et alCitation116 found that spindle density – typically related to cognitive functionsCitation117Citation119 – positively correlates with measures of dream recall (ie, dream word count) and visuospatial performance (mirror tracing test) in 53 healthy subjects.Citation116

All these results are consistent with the previously mentioned findings of Foulkes,Citation2,Citation4 suggesting, again, a relationship between cognitive performance, particularly visuospatial abilities, and dreaming.Citation1,Citation114Citation116

Considering the aforementioned findings, it might be interesting to have a detailed investigation on individual performance in neuropsychological tasks in relationship to dream recall frequency and dream content in adulthood. This could also open the perspective of a direct comparison with the findings available in infancyCitation3,Citation7 and of a continuity with an age-related cognitive decline in elderly. Nonetheless no direct investigation about brain maturation and its relationship with dream recall rates in the life span are yet available, further investigations on dream features within a neuropsychological perspective can help addressing this issue.

Bearing in mind the already mentioned limitations in the dream research,Citation21 it should be useful to address some final observations. Specifically, in the discussed EEG studies dream reports were collected after the first sleep cycle,Citation31 the entire night,Citation28 during a 40-hour multiple nap protocol,Citation101 diurnal naps,Citation27,Citation35 and REM or NREM sleep-onset periods.Citation23 The influence of circadian and ultradian factors on dream features and recall ratesCitation52Citation57 could be a possible explanation for the inconsistent findings in EEG correlates of dream recall. Moreover, different time of collection of dream report (ie, in the middle of the night compared to the end or the beginning) could lead to different qualitative features in dream recall, due to a direct influence of circadian and homeostatic influencing factors.Citation52Citation57

Dreaming in elderly

The relationship between sleep and memory functioningCitation119Citation121 gains importance if considered in the frame of age-dependent cognitive deterioration and changes in dream recall rates. Nonetheless, only few studies investigated this issue. The question we would like to introduce is whether the reported overall reduction in dream recall frequency can result from related macro- and microstructural changes of sleep, and its relation with quantitative and qualitative features in dream content.

Sleep in elderly shows an alteration in its architecture, circadian, and homeostatic features.Citation122 Specifically, sleep in elderly people is characterized by lower total sleep time, sleep efficiency, and SWS; a raise in intrasleep awakenings; and higher stages 1 and 2 of sleep and naps compared to younger groups.Citation122

For what concerns REM sleep, some researchers observed a reduction in REM sleep percentage.Citation122 Moreover, elderly people wake up more often from stage 2 than from REM sleep.Citation122 Interestingly, K complexesCitation123,Citation124 and sleep spindlesCitation124Citation126 also decrease. These graphoelements have been addressed as markers for early cognitive deterioration conditions, such as Alzheimer’s disease,Citation127Citation129 where memory deterioration is one of the main complaints. Additionally, a reduced delta activity (ie, slow-wave activity) and SWS,Citation56,Citation123,Citation130 conjointly with an increased beta activity, have been observed in elderly compared to younger subjects.Citation57

Within a sleep psychophysiological perspective, Chellappa et alCitation32 studied EEG activity and its cortical topography that is associated with a successful dream recall in the elderly. They found an NREM pattern of higher delta activity in the frontal areas at sleep offset and higher centro-parietal sigma activity in recallers compared to nonrecallers.Citation32 This is in contrast with the findings of younger adults, where a successful dream recall was found consequently to a reduction in the delta and sigma activity.Citation28,Citation30,Citation31,Citation35,Citation102

Despite the fact that the overall findings disclose an age-related reduction in dream recall frequency, it should be underlined that dream recall frequency seems already reduced in first and middle adulthood.Citation72,Citation132 Consequently, the simple relationship between aging and dream recall decline has been confuted by different studies reporting a reduction in the number of dreams remembered from around 20–38 years.Citation72,Citation132,Citation133 Anyway, these results are not ever confirmed, considering a recent longitudinal study showing that dream recall rates remain stable within 3 years in 75% of the participants included in an extensive online investigation (two groups of 2872 and 2929 persons).Citation143 Anyway, these results do not completely exclude an interpretation in terms of declined cognitive performance. Specifically, in a recent study, NielsenCitation72 assessed dream recall frequency and theme diversity in the life span of an individual, through an online questionnaire. The results point to sex differences in dream recall drop, with a later decline for women (which begins at 40–49 years and shows its nadir around 50–59 years) compared to men (which begins at 30–39 years and reaches its nadir at 40–49 years).Citation72 An additional aim was to assess the dream theme diversity in the life span throughout 56 dream themes (Typical Dreams Questionnaire [TDQ]).Citation72 The results showed a linear decrement in the number of dream themes across ages. The author considered TDQ as a measure of episodic or voluntary autobiographical memory, interpreting the observed flattening in dream theme diversity as linked to the decline in this kind of memory in aging.Citation72

While these resultsCitation72 support only indirectly the perspective of a memory–dreaming relationship, WatermanCitation131 tried to directly address this relationship in a systematic study with elderly people. Interestingly, quantitative features of home-collected dreams in elderly people (61–75 years) compared to middle-aged adults (45–60 years) did not show any age effect.Citation131 More in detail, dream narrative length and frequency of dream recall showed no significant differences on either measure of recall comparing the ages of 45 and 75 years.Citation131 At the same time, the authors found a strong positive correlation between visual memory measures and dream length, but not with dream recall frequency.Citation131 This suggests that narrative length and recall frequency may reflect different aspects of dream recall.

Considering the paucity of studies aimed to evaluate the neural correlates of dream recall in elderly, we can only hypothesize these changes are associated to the general reduction of dream recall frequency, as reported throughout retrospective questionnaires.Citation72,Citation131Citation134 Some authors suggested the observed age-dependent delay in circadian rhythms could be an influencing factor on dream recall.Citation32,Citation53,Citation72,Citation135,Citation138 In this regard, Antrobus et alCitation53 observed that a delay of 3 hours in sleep onset leads to longer and more vivid dream report, compared to a control group with normal sleep schedule, specifically at the end of the night. Chellappa et alCitation32 found that elderly people have more intense and anticipated emotional load in dream content compared to a younger group.

Previous findingsCitation132,Citation137 report that the global impoverishment of REM sleep could parallel the reduction in dream recall rates, in line with evidence of patients suffering from mild degenerative dementia who show both decreases of dream recall and REM sleep compared to healthy persons.Citation138

In the perspective of qualitative and quantitative features of dream content, a reduction in dream salience linked to aging has been interpreted as diminished interest and overall impact associated to dream contents.Citation61,Citation132 This interpretation has been substantiated by an increment of dream recall rates in elderly people motivated to remember their dreams.Citation139,Citation140 Anyway, retrospective questionnaires – commonly used in these studies – could lead to a bias in the actual numbers of dreams remembered.Citation132 In fact, elderly people are more susceptible to memory interference.Citation141 Collecting dream report immediately upon awakening may partly overpass this problem.Citation132,Citation133 In this context, the actual finding of a reduced dream recall frequency in elderly people after provoked awakenings, as compared to younger subjects,Citation131,Citation142 gives strength to this interpretation in terms of age-related cognitive decline. Moreover, the motivational explanationCitation139,Citation140 can be interpreted in the context of a training-related gain in performance: focusing on the task of reporting dream could promote a substantial increase in salience of dream content, with a parallel improvement in memory for dreams.

Summing up, the findings available about qualitative and quantitative features of dream experience in elderly do not follow a coherent trend, leaving several opened questions about the actual involvement of an age-related cognitive decline.Citation61,Citation132 Moreover, the finding of a drop in dream recall rates starting from mid-adulthoodCitation72 seems counterintuitive in respect to the age-related decline hypothesis. Notwithstanding, the results of a relationship between visual memory and dream length reported by Waterman,Citation131 as well as the indirect link between dream recall decline and declarative memory addressed by Nielsen,Citation72 strengthen the hypothesis of parallel development of cognitive performance and qualitative/quantitative features of dream reports across the life span. However, the specificity of this relationship has still to be clarified.

In electrophysiological perspective, age-related microstructural changes of sleep have been linked to cognitive deterioration.Citation123,Citation127Citation129 Consistently, investigations in pathological cohorts could directly assess the relationship between microstructural changes and drop in dream recall rate. At the same time, in a neuropsychological perspective, specific rehabilitation programs aiming to enhance episodic-declarative performance in the elderly people may help to investigate the relationship between dream recall and cognitive performance.

Conclusion and perspectives

The aim of this review was to summarize the current investigations about sleep mentation in the life span, in the perspective to understand whether these changes are associated to an age-related mental development. In this contest, the available literature highlighted a general gap in the investigations about dream recall across the life span. The studies on neural correlates of dreaming almost completely refer to young and middle adulthood. On the other hand, very few studies directly looked for a relationship between dream content and cognitive skills.Citation1,Citation2,Citation4,Citation6,Citation7,Citation114Citation116 Consequently, definitive conclusions about dream mentation in the life span could not be addressed, either on a neural or on cognitive level.

It should be outlined that the indirect access to dream content is a stable issue in dream studies,Citation21 nonetheless the reasons could change among ages. The variety of methods used to investigate dream mentation is probably one of the main problems that confound the interpretation in terms of basic cognitive functions. Firstly, children’s studies are mainly based on reports by the caregiver.Citation5,Citation7,Citation79 Furthermore, access to dream recall in children is not always possible. As an example, infantile amnesia and confabulatory reports in infancy are the main reasons why reliable studies in this age range cannot be carried out.Citation73,Citation74

Although for different reasons, in late ages also the study of dream mentation is challenged by a less reliable access to dream content. A decline of interests in dreaming and its emotional salience leads to a diminished access to dreaming,Citation61,Citation132 and consequently to a drop in dream recall rates. Moreover, a deterioration of long-term memory could be the basis for a reduced access to dream content,Citation61 while the interference of environmental stimuli in sleep offset results in the forgetting of dream content (ie, a consolidation issue).Citation132

Some studies on dreaming in adults focused on the hypothesis of common electrophysiological mechanisms of dream recall and wake declarative memory, aiming to overcome the limitation of an indirect access to dream mentation.Citation21,Citation27,Citation28 In other words, we suggested a continuity between wake and sleep mental activity, hypothesizing that dream recall and episodic-declarative memory share some common mechanisms.Citation21,Citation27,Citation28,Citation35 Interestingly, brain maturational markers such as alpha and theta rhythmsCitation94 have been found to be involved both in dream recallCitation21,Citation27,Citation28,Citation30Citation32,Citation35 and episodic-declarative memoryCitation61,Citation94,Citation97 However, this still remains an open question and the findings about this relationship are not conclusive.

Current studies do not directly address age-related memory decline as an explanation for the drop in dream recall rates. In fact, the changes in dream recall rate seem to follow a linear decrement from first adulthood to late ages.Citation72 Executive attention and mental imagery skills have been related to dream content in children,Citation3,Citation7 while a positive correlation between visual memory and dream length has been reported in late ages.Citation131 This evidence suggests the need for longitudinal investigations on specific cognitive mechanisms associated to dream experience (ie, executive functions, visuospatial ability, and episodic-declarative memory).Citation3,Citation17

Additionally, transversal studies should compare younger and older people with respect to both characteristics of dream recall and tasks reflecting an age-related cognitive decline. Conversely, studies in cognitive impaired cohorts, enrolled in rehabilitation protocols, should evaluate the possible covariation between changes in dreaming and the gain in cognitive performance.

Finally, our paper would provide a better understanding of the changes in dreaming during the life span as associated to an age-related mental development. However, we are aware that other factors influence sleep mentation. As an example, some studies revealed that gender differences, gender orientation,Citation72,Citation143Citation145 and personalityCitation146 also affect dream recall frequency and dream features. However, it should be underlined that the aim of this review was not to be exhaustive of the vast and complex body of literature on dreaming, but to offer a guideline for further investigations.

As a concluding remark, we would like to suggest that dreaming should be considered as a changing process, covarying and possibly paralleling mental development. Far from being a simple by-product of a sleeping mind, dreaming may also play a possible role in neural and cognitive development.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant to Serena Scarpelli from “BIAL Foundation (grant 2016/2017 number 32/16)”.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • ButlerSFWatsonRIndividual differences in memory for dreams: the role of cognitive skillsPercep Mot Skills1985613823828
  • FoulkesDChildren’s Dreams: Longitudinal StudiesNew YorkJohn Wiley & Sons Inc1982
  • FoulkesDHollifeldMSullivanBBradleyLTerryRREM dreaming and cognitive skills at ages 5–8: a cross-sectional studyInt J Behav Dev1990134447465
  • FoulkesDDreaming and REM sleepJ Sleep Res1993219920210607095
  • SándorPSzakadátSBódizsROntogeny of dreaming: a review of empirical studiesSleep Med Rev201418543544924629827
  • SándorPSzakadátSKertészKBódizsRContent analysis of 4 to 8 years-old children’s dream reportsFront Psychol2015653425983708
  • SándorPSzakadátSBódizsRThe development of cognitive and emotional processing as reflected in children’s dreams: active self in an eventful dream signals better neuropsychological skillsDreaming20162615878
  • BellAHallCThe Personality of a Child Molester: An Analysis of DreamsChicago, ILAldine1971
  • HallCA cognitive theory of dreamsJ Gen Psychol1953491273282
  • DomhoffGWThe neural substrate for dreaming: is it a subsystem of the default network?Conscious Cogn2011201163117421450492
  • DomhoffGWThe invasion of the concept snatchers: he origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesisDreaming20172711439
  • HobsonJASchredlMThe continuity and dis-continuity between waking and dreaming: a Dialogue between Michael Schredl and Allan Hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notionsInt J Dream Res2011437
  • RossJHThe continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming from the perspective of an analytical psychological construct. Commentary on “The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A Dialogue between Michael Schredl and Allan Hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions”Int J Dream Res20114281
  • SchredlMContinuity in studying the continuity hypothesis of dreaming is needed reply to the comments on “The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A dialogue between Michael Schredl and Allan Hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions”Int J Dream Res20125118
  • De GennaroLMarzanoCCipolliCFerraraMHow we remember the stuff that dreams are made of: neurobiological approaches to the brain mechanisms of dream recallBehav Brain Res2012226592.e622024432
  • CipolliCFerraraMDe GennaroLPlazziGBeyond the neuropsychology of dreaming: insights into the neural basis of dreaming with new techniques of sleep recording and analysisSleep Med Rev20173582027569701
  • AserinskyEKleitmanNRegularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleepScience195311827327413089671
  • DementKleitmanNThe relation of eye movements during sleep to dream activity: an objective method for the study of dreamingJ Exp Psychol19575333934613428941
  • DementWKleitmanNCyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreamingElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19579467369013480240
  • FoulkesDDream reports from different stages of sleepJ Abnorm Soc Psychol196265142513894288
  • ScarpelliSD’AtriAGorgoniMFerraraMDe GennaroLEEG oscillations during sleep and dream recall: state- or trait-like individual differences?Front Psychol2015660525999908
  • NielsenTA review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: covert REM sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing modelsBehav Brain Sci20002385186611515145
  • HobsonJAMcCarleyRWThe brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream processAm J Psychiat19771341335134821570
  • FoulkesDDreaming: A Cognitive-Psychological AnalysisHillsdale, NJErlbaum1985
  • SolmsMThe Neuropsychology of Dreams: A Clinico-Anatomical StudyHillsdaleErlbaum1997
  • SolmsMDreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanismsBehav Brain Sci20002843850
  • ScarpelliSMarzanoCD’AtriAGorgoniMFerraraMDe GennaroLState-or trait-like individual differences in dream recall: preliminary findings from a within-subjects study of multiple nap REM sleep awakeningsFront Psychol2015692826217264
  • MarzanoCFerraraMMauroFRecalling and forgetting dreams: theta and alpha oscillations during sleep predict subsequent dream recallJ Neurosci2011316674668321543596
  • HsiehLTRanganathCFrontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrievalNeuroimage20148572172923933041
  • TakeuchiTOgilvieRDMurphyTIFerrelliAVEEG activities during elicited sleep onset REM and NREM periods reflect different mechanisms of dream generationClin Neurophysiol200311421022012559227
  • EspositoMJNielsenTAPaquetteTReduced alpha power associated with the recall of mentation from Stage 2 and Stage REM sleepPsychophysiology20044128829715032994
  • ChellappaSLMünchMBlatterKKnoblauchVCajochenCDoes the circadian modulation of dream recall modify with age?Sleep200932120119750925
  • NyhusECurranTFunctional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memoryNeurosci Biobehav Rev2010341023103520060015
  • Corsi-CabreraMMiroEdel-Rıo-PortillaYPerez-GarciEVillanuevaYGuevaraMARapid eye movement sleep dreaming is characterized by uncoupled EEG activity between frontal and perceptual cortical regionsBrain Cogn200251337345
  • ScarpelliSD’AtriAMangiarugaAPredicting dream recall: EEG activation during NREM sleep or shared mechanism with wakefulness?Brain Topogr201730562963828434101
  • BartolacciCScarpelliSDe GennaroLL’attività elettrica cerebrale (EEG) predice la presenza del ricordo dei sogni?Riv Sperim Freniatria201727999
  • SchredlMQuestionnaires and diaries as research instruments in dream research: methodological issuesDreaming2002121726
  • SchredlMContinuity between waking and dreaming: a proposal for a mathematical modelSleep Hypnosis200352640
  • RobertGZadraAMeasuring nightmare and bad dream frequency: impact of retrospective and prospective instrumentsJ Sleep Res20081713213918482103
  • LevinRNielsenTADisturbed dreaming, posttraumatic stress disorder, and affect distress: a review and neurocognitive modelPych Bull2007133482528
  • SchredlMWittmannLDreaming: a psychological viewSchweiz Arch Neurol Psychiatr2005156484492
  • SchwartzSMaquetPSleep imaging and the neuropsychological assessment of dreamsTrends Cogn200262330
  • HobsonJAPace-SchottEFStickgoldRDreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious statesBehav Brain Sci20002379384211515143
  • HallCSVan de CastleRLThe Content Analysis of DreamsNew YorkAppleton-Century-Crofts1966
  • DomhoffGWNew directions in the study of dream content using the Hall and Van de Castle coding systemDreaming199992–3115
  • NielsenTADream analysis and classification: the reality simulation perspectiveKryegerMRothTDementWCPrinciples and Practice of Sleep MedicineNew YorkElsevier2010595603
  • SchredlMDream content analysis: basic principlesInt J Dream Res2010316573
  • Van RijnEEichenlaubJBLewisPAThe dream-lag effect: selective processing of personally significant events during rapid eye movement sleep, but not during slow wave sleepNeurobiol Learn Mem20151229810925683202
  • BlagroveMHenley-EinionJBarnettAEdwardsDSeageCHA replication of the 5–7day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to future events as control for baseline matchingConscious Cogn201120238439120739193
  • BlagroveMFouquetNCHenley-EinionJAAssessing the dream-lag effect for REM and NREM Stage 2 dreamsPLoS One2011610e2670822046336
  • VallatRChatardBBlagroveMRubyPCharacteristics of the memory sources of dreams: a new version of the content-matching paradigm to take mundane and remote memories into accountPLoS One20171210e018526229020066
  • AntrobusJSDreaming: cognitive processes during cortical activation and high afferent thresholdsPsychol Rev19919895121
  • AntrobusJKondoTReinselRFeinGDreaming in the late morning: summation of REM and diurnal cortical activationConscious Cogn199542752997497109
  • WamsleyEJHirotaYTuckerMASmithMRAntrobusJSCircadian and ultradian influences on dreaming: a dual rhythm modelBrain Res Bull200771434735417208651
  • ChellappaSLCajochenCUltradian and circadian modulation of dream recall: EEG correlates and age effectsInt J Psychophysiol201389216517023524011
  • CajochenCMünchMKnoblauchVBlatterKWirz-JusticeAAge-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleepChronobiol Int20062346147416687319
  • MünchMSilvaEJRondaJMCzeislerCADuffyJFEEG sleep spectra in older adults across all circadian phases during NREM sleepSleep201033338940120337198
  • SiclariFLaRocqueJJPostleBRTononiGAssessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigmFront Psychol2013454223970876
  • SiclariFLaRocqueJJBernardiGPostleBRTononiGThe neural correlates of consciousness in sleep: a no-task, within-state paradigmbioRxiv Epub122014
  • WeiszRFoulkesDHome and laboratory dreams collected under uniform sampling conditionsPsychophysiology197065885964320884
  • CohenDBRemembering and forgetting dreamingKihlstromJFEvansFJFunctional Disorders of MemoryNewYorkWiley1979239274
  • NielsenTChronobiological features of dream productionSleep Med Rev20048540342415336239
  • SnyderFThe phenomenology of dreamingMeadowLSnowLThe Psychodynamic Implications of the Physiological Studies on DreamsSpringfieldThomas1970124151
  • FoulkesDPopeRPrimary visual experience and secondary cognitive elaboration in stage REM: a modest confirmation and an extensionPercept Mot Skills1973371071184353828
  • FoulkesDSchmidtMTemporal sequence and unit composition in dream reports from different stages of sleepSleep198362652806622882
  • WittmannLZehnderDSchredlMJenniOGLandoltMAPost-traumatic nightmares and psychopathology in children after road traffic accidentsJ Trauma Stress201023232e920419731
  • SimardVNielsenTTremblayREBoivinMMontplaisirJYLongitudinal study of preschool sleep disturbance. The predictive role of maladaptive parental behaviors, early sleep problems, and child/mother psychological factorsArch Pediatr Adolesc Med20081624
  • SimardVNielsenTATremblayREBoivinMMontplaisirJYLongitudinal study of bad dreams in preschool-aged children: prevalence, demographic correlates, risk and protective factorsSleep2008311627018220079
  • SiegelABChildren’s dreams and nightmares: emerging trends in researchDreaming2005153147154
  • GermainANielsenTASleep pathophysiology in posttraumatic stress disorder and idiopathic nightmare sufferersBiol Psychiatry200354101092109814625152
  • SchredlMHome dream recall in children and young adultsInt J Dream Res2009225859
  • NielsenTVariations in dream recall frequency and dream theme diversity by age and sexFront Neurol201243106
  • NirYTononiGDreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiologyTrends Cogn Sci2010148810020079677
  • LlewellynSSuch stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampusBehav Brain Sci20133658965924304746
  • FoulkesDHome and laboratory dreams: four empirical studies and a conceptual reevaluationSleep19792233e51232567
  • WechslerDManual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of IntelligenceSan AntonioThe Psychological Corporation1967
  • Groth-MarnatGTealMBlock design as a measure of everyday spatial ability: a study of ecological validityPercept Mot Skills200090252252610833749
  • VossUFrenzelCKoppehele-GosselJHobsonALucid dreaming: an age-dependent brain dissociationJ Sleep Res201221663464222639960
  • ColaceCChildren’s Dreams: From Freud’s Observations to Modern Dream Research1st edLondonKarnac Books Ltd2010
  • ValliKRevonsuoAPälkäsOIsmailKHAliKJPunamäkiR-LThe threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: evidence from dreams of traumatized childrenConscious Cogn200514188e21815766897
  • DukeTDavidsonJOrdinary and recurrent dream recall of active, past and non-recurrent dreamers during and after academic stressDreaming200212185197
  • PunamakiRLDeterminants and mental health effects of dream recall among children living in traumatic conditionsDreaming19977235263
  • TerrLCChildren of Chowchilla: a study of psychic traumaPsychoanal Study Child197934547e623504534
  • HelminenEPunamäkiRLContextualized emotional images in children’s dreams: psychological adjustment in conditions of military traumaInt J Behav Dev200832177187
  • De GennaroLFerraraMCristianiRCurcioGMartiradonnaVBertiniMAlexithymia and dream recall upon spontaneous morning awakeningPsychosom Med20036530130612651998
  • De GennaroLMarzanoCMoroniFCurcioGFerraraMCipolliCRecovery sleep after sleep deprivation almost completely abolishes dream recallBehav Brain Res201020629329819788898
  • De GennaroLCipolliCCherubiniAAmygdala and hippo-campus volumetry and diffusivity in relation to dreamingHum Brain Mapp2011321458147020740648
  • De GennaroLLanteriOPirasFDopaminergic system and dream recall: an MRI study in Parkinson’s disease patientsHuman Brain Mapp20163711361147
  • StrauchIREM dreaming in the transition from late childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal studyDreaming200515155e69
  • EvansRCDream conception and reality testing in childrenJ Am Acad Child Psychiatr19731217392
  • StrauchILederbogenSThe home dreams and waking fantasies of boys and girls between ages 9 and 15: a longitudinal studyDreaming199992–3153
  • CicognaPCOcchioneroMNataleVEspositoMJBizarreness of size and shape in dream imagesConscious Cogn20071638139016870474
  • NielsenTThe stress acceleration hypothesis of nightmaresFront Neurol2017820128620339
  • KlimeschWEEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysisBrain Res Rev19992916919510209231
  • NovelliLD’atriAMarzanoCMapping changes in cortical activity during sleep in the first 4 years of lifeJ Sleep Res201625438138926854271
  • NishidaMPearsallJBucknerRLWalkerMPREM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memoryCereb Cortex2009191158116618832332
  • KlimeschWDoppelmayrMRusseggerHPachingerTTheta band power in the human scalp EEG and the encoding of new informationNeuroreport19967123512408817539
  • AndersonKLRajagovindanRGhacibehGAMeadorKJDingMTheta oscillations mediate interaction between prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe in human memoryCereb Cortex2010201604161219861635
  • NormanKANewmanELDetreGA neural network model of retrieval-induced forgettingPsychol Rev200711488795317907868
  • SiclariFBairdBPerogamvrosLThe neural correlates of dreamingNat Neurosci201720687287828394322
  • ChellappaSLFreySKnoblauchVCajochenCCortical activation patterns herald successful dream recall after NREM and REM sleepBiol Psychol201187225125621419827
  • SchredlMSchäferGWeberBHeuserIDreaming and insomnia: dream recall and dream content in patients with insomniaJ Sleep Res1998731911989785274
  • EichenlaubJBNicolasADaltrozzoJResting brain activity varies with dream recall frequency between subjectsNeuropsychopharmacology2014391594160224549103
  • EichenlaubJBBertrandOMorletDRubyPBrain reactivity differentiates subjects with high and low dream recall frequencies during both sleep and wakefulnessCereb Cortex2014241206121523283685
  • VossUHolzmannRHobsonAInduction of self-awareness in dreams through frontal low current stimulation of gamma activityNat Neurosci20141781081224816141
  • ZimmermanWBSleep mentation and auditory awakening thresholdsPsychophysiology197065405494320882
  • KoulackDGoodenoughDRDream recall and dream recall failure: an arousal-retrieval modelPsychol Bull197683975
  • RosenblattSIAntrobusJIZimlerJPThe effect of post-awakening differences in activation on the REM–NREM report effect and recall of information from filmsAntrobusJSBertiniMThe Neuropsychology of Sleep and DreamingHillsdale, NJErlbaum1992
  • ColaceCDream bizarreness reconsideredSleep Hypn200353105128
  • MaquetPPétersJMAertsJFunctional neuroanatomy of human rapid eye movement sleep and dreamingNature19963831631668774879
  • NofzingerEAMintunMAWisemanMKupferDJMooreRYForebrain activation in REM sleep: an FDG PET studyBrain Res19977701922019372219
  • BraunARBalkinTJWesenstenNJRegional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle: an H2O-15 positron emission tomography studyBrain1997120117311979236630
  • HobsonJAPace-SchottEFThe cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal system, consciousness and learningNat Rev Neurosci20023967969312209117
  • CoryTLOrmistonDWPredicting the frequency of dream recallJ Abnorm Psychol19758432612661133253
  • DumelGCarrMMarquisLPBlanchette-CarriereCPaquetteTNielsenTInfrequent dream recall associated with low performance but high overnight improvement on mirror-tracingJ Sleep Res20152437238225726721
  • NielsenTCarrMBlanchette-CarrièreCNREM sleep spindles are associated with dream recall sleep spindles & cortical up states2016112741
  • FogelSMSmithCTThe function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidationNeurosci Biobehav Rev20113551154116521167865
  • ClemensZFaboDHalaszPTwenty-four hours retention of visuospatial memory correlates with the number of parietal sleep spindlesNeurosci Letters20064031–25256
  • DiekelmannSBornJThe memory function of sleepNat Rev Neurosci201011211412620046194
  • EllenbogenJM1PayneJDStickgoldRThe role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none?Curr Opin Neurobiol200616671672217085038
  • WalkerMPStickgoldROvernight alchemy: sleep-dependent memory evolutionNat Rev Neurosci2010113218 author reply 21820168316
  • Pace-SchottEFSpencerRMSleep-dependent memory consolidation in healthy aging and mild cognitive impairmentCurr Top Behav Neurosci20152530733024652608
  • SchwarzJFAAkerstedtTLindbergEGruberGFischerHTheorell-HagloWJAge affects sleep microstructure more than sleep macro-structureJ Sleep Res20172627728728093830
  • CrowleyKTrinderJKimYCarringtonMColrainIMThe effects of normal aging on sleep spindle and K-complex productionClin Neurophysiol20021131615162212350438
  • LandoltHPDijkDJAchermannPBorbélyAAEffect of age on the sleep EEG: slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle- aged menBrain Res19967382052128955514
  • CarrierJMonkTHBuysseDJKupferDJSleep and morningness-eveningness in the “middle” years of life (20–59 y)J Sleep Res199762302379493522
  • De GennaroLGorgoniMRedaFThe fall of sleep K-complex in Alzheimer diseaseSci Rep201773968828045040
  • RedaFGorgoniMLauriGIn search of sleep biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: K-complexes do not discriminate between patients with mild cognitive impairment and healthy controlsBrain Sci201775piiE51
  • GorgoniMLauriGTrugliaIParietal fast sleep spindle density decrease in Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mild cognitive impairmentNeural Plast20162016837610827066274
  • BliwiseDLNormal agingPrinc Pract Sleep Med200542438
  • WatermanDAging and memory for dreamsPercept Mot Skills1991733553651766755
  • GiambraLMJungREGrodskyAAge changes in dream recall in adulthoodDreaming1996617
  • FunkhouserATHirsbrunnerHPCornuCBahroMDreams and dreaming among the elderly: an overviewAging Ment Health199931020
  • ZanasiMDe PersisSCaporaliMSiracusanoADreams and agePercept Mot Skills200510092593816158679
  • SuzukiHUchiyamaMTagayaHDreaming during non-rapid eye movement sleep in the absence of prior rapid eye movement sleepSleep2004271486149015683138
  • ChellappaSLMuenchMKnoblauchVCajochenCAge effects on spectral electroencephalogram activity prior to dream recallJ Sleep Res20122124725621851439
  • WoodruffDSArousal, sleep, and agingBirrenJESchaieWBengtsonVJarvikLSalthouseTHandbook of the Psychology of Aging1985261295
  • GuenoleFMarcaggiGBaleyteJMGarmaIDreams in normal and pathological agingPsychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil20108879620525540
  • StrunzFTräumen im Alter: Bestand und Nutzung [Dreams in the elderly: contents and clinical usefulness]Gesundheitswesen19935511595601 German8286901
  • StrunzFDie Kultivierung der Traumerinnerung-Ein Instrument [The cultivation of dream recall: a tool]Psychischer Hygiene Pravention1988116770 German
  • KauslerDExperimental Psychology, Cognition, and Human Aging2nd edNew YorkSpringer-Verlag1991
  • FeinGFeinbergIInselTRSleep mentation in the elderlyPsychophysiology1985222182253991849
  • SchredlMGöritzASChanges in dream recall frequency, nightmare frequency, and lucid dream frequency over a 3-year periodDreaming20152528187
  • SchredlMGöritzASGender differences in dream content: related to biological sex or sex role orientation?Imagination, cognition and personality2010302171183
  • GeorgiMSchredlMHenley-EinionJBlagrgoveMGender differences in dreaming in childhood and adolescence: The UK Library studyInt J of Dream Res201252125129
  • MalinowskiJEDreaming and personality: wake-dream continuity, thought suppression, and the Big Five InventoryConscious Cogn20151538915