132
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Relevance of deprivation studies in understanding rapid eye movement sleep

, &
Pages 143-158 | Published online: 29 May 2018

Abstract

Rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) is a unique phenomenon essential for maintaining normal physiological processes and is expressed at least in species higher in the evolution. The basic scaffold of the neuronal network responsible for REMS regulation is present in the brainstem, which may be directly or indirectly influenced by most other physiological processes. It is regulated by the neurons in the brainstem. Various manipulations including chemical, elec-trophysiological, lesion, stimulation, behavioral, ontogenic and deprivation studies have been designed to understand REMS genesis, maintenance, physiology and functional significance. Although each of these methods has its significance and limitations, deprivation studies have contributed significantly to the overall understanding of REMS. In this review, we discuss the advantages and limitations of various methods used for REMS deprivation (REMSD) to understand neural regulation and physiological significance of REMS. Among the deprivation strategies, the flowerpot method is by far the method of choice because it is simple and convenient, exploits physiological parameter (muscle atonia) for REMSD and allows conducting adequate controls to overcome experimental limitations as well as to rule out nonspecific effects. Notwithstanding, a major criticism that the flowerpot method faces is that of perceived stress experienced by the experimental animals. Nevertheless, we conclude that like most methods, particularly for in vivo behavioral studies, in spite of a few limitations, given the advantages described above, the flowerpot method is the best method of choice for REMSD studies.

Introduction

Living organisms and their constituent cells undergo basic rest and activity cycle (BRAC), which varies in phenotypic expressions. For living system(s) higher in evolution, particularly with evolved brain, BRAC has further evolved into sleep and waking. In healthy individuals, the quantity and quality of sleep and waking are expressed at optimum levels, which vary within limit under given physiological conditions. These physiological conscious states are inherently dynamic and are maintained in equilibrium with each other. Initially, sleep was considered as a passive phenomenon, which has been set aside by findings from consistent research in the mid-twentieth century. It is now accepted that sleep is an active, non-monolithic behavioral phenomenon, indispensable for optimal physiological processes including functioning of the vital organs of the body.

Based on electrophysiological parameters, sleep has been broadly classified into rapid eye movement sleep (REMS) and non-REMS (NREMS). By and large, REMS is char- acterized by desynchronized electroencephalogram (EEG), muscle atonia and rapid eye movements. In humans, REMS constitutes about 20% of total sleep time, which is considerably lesser in proportion to one 24 h complete day. Experimentally, it has been shown that animals, for example, dogsCitation1 and rats,Citation2,Citation3 as well as humansCitation4 not only maintain this fundamental process but also, if deprived, adequately compensate the lost REMS by its rebound increase during the post-deprivation recovery period. In the absence of such recovery, REMS deprivation (REMSD) might lead to altered states, which may cause various pathological conditions and even loss of life in extreme conditions.Citation3,Citation5 This means that the living beings are possessive of this state and consider REMS as non-compromisingly necessary for maintaining normal physiological processes.Citation5 Also, the vital role of REMS may be attributed to the fact that its primary control machinery, the neural network, is located in the brainstem, where the neurons for the regulation of other vital physiological processes, for example, cardiovascular and respiratory systems, are located, and its regulation is not under voluntary control. Thus, optimum amount of sleep including REMS is important for maintaining normal and healthy living, and therefore, an understanding of this essential physiological phenomenon is necessary.

After the discovery of REMS in 1953,Citation6 consistent attempts have been made to understand its genesis, mechanism, regulation and functional significance, which have been dealt with in many reviews and scholarly articles.Citation7Citation10 Several methods including transection, stimulation (electrical and chemical), lesion and microinjection into the brain, and deprivation of the subjects (humans or animals) have been carried out to understand the mechanisms of generation, regulation and functions of REMS. All these methods, particularly in vivo behavioral studies, have respective merits, and none is immune to some limitations. Like most studies, the choice of the method to be applied for investigating various aspects of REMS depends on the factor(s) whose expressions are to be explored keeping in view the limitations associated with the method to be used.

Deprivation vs. induction of excess REMS

Normally, functioning of a system along with that of its constituent components viz. cells, tissues and organs is in dynamic equilibrium leading to a healthy life. The optimum output of each of those units (individually or whole) is expressed at a lower level than the maximum that respective unit can possibly deliver. Although a cell or a system can increase its output, it cannot be higher than the maximum, and thus, the difference of maximum from that of the optimum cannot be as high as that of the difference of optimum to the minimum, which would be zero. Attempt to significantly increase the output of such a system could be nonphysiological, particularly if continued for longer duration. Also, it is likely to wear out the system very rapidly and significantly more than that of withdrawal of the output. Therefore, while designing an experiment, an increase in the expression, REMS in this case, may or may not be as significant or may be counterproductive than that of its decrease, that is, withdrawal. Further, an induction usually would need active process and may be invasive, while the withdrawal (deprivation) may not. Hence, the REMSD is the preferred method of choice for exploring, acquiring and advancing our understanding of REMS in its totality, which we review in this article. However, as there are several misapprehensions particularly related to stress associated to REMSD, the researchers face constant challenge to address this issue. We would discuss this issue for better understanding; however, before moving forward, it is important to discuss briefly the neural substrate(s) for the regulation of REMS.

Brain mechanism of REMS regulation

REMS was first identified in humansCitation6 and was later shown in other mammals including rats, cats, dogs and so on. Its expression in species lower in evolution (particularly invertebrates) is debatable, primarily because the brain electrical activity (EEG), one of the basic characteristics for identification of REMS, is not quite evident in species possessing rudimentary or lesser evolved brain. Transection,Citation11 lesionCitation12Citation14 and stimulationCitation15,Citation16 studies identified that the dorsolateral pontine region is responsible for REMS regulation. This region possesses noradrenaline (NA)-ergic REM-OFF neurons in the locus coeruleus (LC) while cholinergic (largely) REM-ON neurons in the laterodorsal tegmentum/pedunculopontine tegmentum (LDT/PPT).Citation15,Citation17,Citation18 As like the regulation by the brain of other complex behaviors and functions, a fine tuning between activation and deactivation of the REM-ON and REM-OFF neurons is necessary for the regulation of REMS. In a series of complementary studies using electrical stimulation,Citation16 microinjection of neurotransmitter (gamma-amino butyric acid [GABA], NA, acetylcholine [ACh]) agonist or antagonist,Citation19Citation21 anti-ouabain antibodies,Citation22 activation of the wake-Citation23 or sleep-inducing brainstem areasCitation24 and upon REMSD,Citation25 it was shown that if the LC REM-OFF neurons were prevented to cease activity, REMS was reduced. These findings suggest that cessation of the LC-REM-OFF neurons is prerequisite for the generation of REMS.Citation26,Citation27 Results from the in vivo studies suggest that GABA acting post- and presynaptically at different sites (on REM-OFF and REM-ON neurons) plays a significant role in inducing inhibition or withdrawal of inhibition for the regulation of REM-ON and REM-OFF neurons that regulate REMS. These studies have been reviewed in detail, and the findings have been consolidated to construct a model explaining the mechanism of regulation of REMS.Citation8 Estimation of NA and GABA levels in different brain regions (explained later in this review) supports the model. It is difficult to experimentally show in vivo in behaving animals the pre- and postsynaptic modulation of inputs on the REM-ON and REM-OFF neurons. Therefore, as an alternative, using the in vivo data, a mathematical model was constructed, which satisfactorily explains the role of (presynaptic) withdrawal of inhibition from the REM-ON neurons, which in turn inhibits the REM-OFF neurons for the initiation of REMS.Citation28 Also, the said model reasonably explains for the first time why REMS does not appear during waking and why it is initiated only after a period of NREMS.Citation8 The in vivo findings from behavioral as well as REMSD studies and those from in vitro (including in silico) studies complement each other to support the proposed neural regulation of REMS.

The GABA-ergic neurons and terminals are scattered throughout the brain.Citation29 The said model included the role of GABA-ergic neurons and terminals in the LC,Citation30 in prepossitus hypoglossus,Citation31 in substantia nigraCitation32 and in LDT/PPTCitation33 in modulating REMS. Subsequently, in recent years, isolated groups have studied the role of GABA-ergic neurons in the periaqueductal grayCitation34,Citation35 and in the dorsal part of the deep mesencephalic reticular nucleus,Citation34,Citation35 and explored their roles in REMS regulation. It is reasonable that the basic and complex physiological process like REMS would be regulated and modulated by neurons containing various neurotransmitters which are located in wider brain areas. Notwithstanding, for confirmation, it needs to be studied in behaving animals how those neurons are affected by NREMS and wake-inducing brain areas, and how the firing of those neurons is related to each other as well as with that of classically known REM-ON and REM-OFF neurons in LDT/PPT and LC, respectively, particularly in relation to modulation of REMS as has been systematically studied for classical REM-ON and REM-OFF neurons. Also, it needs to be experimentally shown if those neurons are affected by REMS loss and what role these neurons and their respective neurotransmitters play in modulating REMS loss-associated functions/symptoms, if at all.

REMSD – its pros and cons

Different approaches have been used to understand the neural regulation and functional significance of REMS, for example, a) phylogenetic or evolution studies, b) ontogenic or development studies and c) experimental studies. The first approach is based on historical observations, evidences from the literature, social and other associated changes, circumstantial evidence and their analyses. For example, among species, there are significant differences in the quality and quantity of time spent in different stages of sleep.Citation36,Citation37 Findings from ontogenic studies allow us to know that REMS helps proper maturation and development of the brain in particular and the nervous system at large;Citation38,Citation39 however, its detailed mechanism of action is not known. Maturation of the brain involves many factors including neurotransmitters.Citation40 As REMS modulates level of NA in the brain, and NA has been reported to affect neuronal growth and development,Citation41 it may play a significant role in REMS-associated maturation of the brain. Notwithstanding, both the phylogenetic and ontogenic studies (particularly in higher species) are slow events, time intensive and often difficult to reproduce repeatedly to prove experimentally and it is difficult to carry out planned control studies to rule out the associated nonspecific effects.

Experimental studies/approaches, on the other hand, enable us to explore the role of REMS in modulating neuronal excitability, apoptosis, cytomorphology, behavior, thermoregulation and their mechanisms. Although the physiological significance of REMS is known to a limited extent, its detailed cellular, molecular and genetic mechanisms of action need to be deciphered. Indeed, those are the subjects of active research across the globe and for which the first two methods of gathering knowledge are least helpful. Also, in vivo studies have obvious limitations to explore and understand the mechanism of effect(s) at the molecular level(s), for which often we extend studies using in vitro cells as models. Notwithstanding, almost all experimental methods are prone to some limitations, technical or otherwise. Selection of a method for the investigation of any biological process depends on the parameter to be explored and analyzed, the kind of question being raised and the suitability of the method to achieve desired results. Thus, the question remains as to what extent does the applied method is specific to induce REMSD, what are the nonspecific factors/variables, how can they be controlled and how the effects of nonspecific factors can be eliminated so that the observed results can be correlated primarily to REMS loss alone. As such, various techniques have been used to induce REMSD; their merits and limitations are discussed below for ease of understanding.

Methods for REMSD

Flowerpot method

The flowerpot method was first used by Jouvet et al to deprive cats, particularly of REMS.Citation42 The novelty of this method is that it exploits a fundamental characteristic defining REMS of the experimental animal, the muscle atonia. As this method overcomes to a large extent most of the limitations encountered by other methods, it has been extensively used on experimental cats, rats and mice across the world.Citation2,Citation42 In this method, the animal is placed on a small island (platform) protruding over the surface of water in a tank. To rule out nonspecific effects, the control animal is placed on a slightly larger platform control (LPC) projected over surrounding water in the same room; that is, all conditions, except the platform size, remain comparable to that of the experimental animal on the smaller platform. The method derived its name from its maiden use of flowerpots by Michel Jouvet; subsequently, it has also been known as water tank, platform or pedestal method.

The success of this method depends on the ratio of platform size (both small and large) to the animal body weight.Citation43 On the small platform, the experimental animal (though carefully) can stand, sit, move and most importantly can experience NREMS. However, at the onset of REMS due to muscle atonia, the animal tends to stretch and relax its body completely which the size of the small platform does not allow. Therefore, initially, the animal might even fall occasionally into the surrounding water and eventually (at the onset of REMS) possibly learns to wake up, often with a jerk, and saves itself from falling into the water. Although it is possible that the animal might learn to wake up before falling, it is likely that the atonia-induced jerk wakes up the animal and not the fall into the water per se. It may be compared with waking an animal with other external non-tactile stimulus, for example, sound. The primary advantage of this method is that it uses a critical REMS-associated physiological signal originating within to trigger awakening of the same animal. This may be comparable to or even better than someone waking up from sleep due to changes in blood pressure, respiratory distress, indigestion, filling of bladder and so on. Although there may not be reported quantified classical experimental verification of all these examples waking a person, from associated knowledge, we know that they affect sleep in normal and diseased individuals, for example, snoring, sleep apnea and hypertension. Also, even if the animal learns, REMS pressure due to loss of REMS overrides the learning. This is because (we have observed) if the REMSD is continued for longer period, for example, more than about 9 days (our observation), some rats might fall into the water and sometimes even fail to climb up on the platform and die of drowning. In shorter duration of REMSD, if the rats fall, they usually climb up onto the platform and save themselves.

On the larger platform, the animals enjoy both NREMS and REMS. To confirm the efficacy of the method, Mendelson et alCitation2 continuously monitored the electrophysiological signals from the experimental and control rats for 4 days and evaluated wakefulness, NREMS and REMS. They observed that during the first 24 h of experiment, the experimental (on small platform) as well as LPC rats had comparable REMS, although that was significantly less than the normal home-cage control rats. Thus, although the small-platform rats are deprived of REMS even during short term (24 h deprivation), due to lack of appropriate control, the method is not suitable for short-term REMSD. However, on the fourth 24 h through the REMSD experiment, that is, after 72 h REMSD, the experimental (on small platform) rats showed significantly reduced REMS as compared to home-cage control rats as well as that of the LPC rats. Also, the rat on the smaller platform showed significantly more REMS rebound during post-REMSD recovery period than both the LPC and cage control rats. Thus, the platform method is most suitable for those studies where the animals are to be REMS deprived for 72 h and more.

The major drawback of the method is that the animals might experience some undesirable discomfort due to muscular overactivity, isolation, some amount of associated loss of NREMS and not complete (100%) loss of REMS. By and large, the restricted mobility-induced effects may not differ significantly (i.e. remain comparable) on the small and large platforms. In some studies, to rule out the effects due to restricted mobility and muscular overactivity, separate control experiments were conducted where movement of animals was restricted by maintaining them in a smaller enclosure or made them to swim for varying duration, respectively.Citation2,Citation44 In another control group, REMS-deprived animals were allowed to recover from the lost REMS, the recovery group (REC). This experiment is carried out to confirm if the changes observed upon REMSD are reversible, particularly upon recovery of lost REMS.Citation44,Citation45 Nevertheless, an argument against this method is that the animals may suffer from isolation as they are kept alone in the experimental setup. However, these isolation-induced effects may be countered as the LPC animals are also maintained alone. Further, in some studies, the animals are acclimatized by keeping them isolated (alone) in their home cages prior to the start of experiment. Notwithstanding, multiple-platform method has been designed to overcome some of the drawbacks of the flowerpot method, which however, achieved limited success.

Hand arousal technique

This method was first used by Dement,Citation4 where a subject is awakened as and when one goes into sleep, NREMS or REMS, usually NREMS. To use this technique, one needs to record electrophysiological parameters defining sleep and waking. Based on such electrophysiological criteria if a subject is awakened at the onset of every NREMS, the subject is almost certainly deprived of total sleep (NREMS as well as REMS) as REMS does not appear unless there is some amount of NREMS. On the other hand, if the subject is awakened only at the onset of every REMS, although apparently one may be deprived mostly of REMS, practically it is difficult to continue for longer duration. This is because with increased duration of deprivation, REMS pressure builds up. This causes increased frequency of appearance of REMS leading to frequent awakening and depriving the subjects of total sleep.

To rule out nonspecific effects, another control subject/animal (yoked control) is awakened equal number of times (as that of the experimental subject) irrespective of the former’s sleep state. It was observed that the experimental subject may be selectively deprived of total sleep or REMS to a reasonable extent only if the studies are continued for a short-term deprivation (limited) period. The method is not suitable to achieve long-term (for more than several hours to a couple of days) deprivation, particularly for REMSD. This is because as the deprivation progresses, the NREMS and REMS pressure increases so much that the subject must be awakened frequently; also, the yoked control gets deprived of sleep. Besides, the method needs continuous monitoring, and therefore, if continued for longer time, the experimenter is also deprived of sleep (unless several researchers take turns, which in turn may add variability though). Use of modern computer-aided techniques has limited the human intervention; however, elaborate instrumentation and possible confounds due to instrumental use need careful planning. In this method, particularly for animal studies, electrodes need to be surgically implanted for recording the electrophysiological signals to objectively define and identify sleep and waking states.

Treadmill arousal method

In this method, the animals are maintained on a treadmill where due to continuous movement of the belt, the animals cannot sleep.Citation46 Ideally, the animals are deprived of total sleep (NREMS as well as REMS) and not exclusively of REMS since they always remain in motion. However, in practice, after spending a while on the treadmill, the animals often learn to run in the opposite direction to that of the movement of the treadmill belt and take a nap till it reached the other end of the treadmill. By this time, to avoid falling, the animal wakes up and runs to the other end of the belt and repeats the cycle. For how long the animal may sleep depends on the speed of the treadmill belt movement, which needs to be increased with passing time to continue deprivation. It has been reported that on an average the animals could enjoy almost 40% of the time spent on treadmill in NREMS.Citation47 In this method, the animals could neither be deprived exclusively of NREMS nor REMS. In a modification of this method, the speed of movement of the treadmill belt was regulated by the changes in the EEG so that whenever the animal under study entered REMS, the belt started rolling at varying speeds and the animal was awakened. This needed much elaborate arrangement or surgery of the animals and often faced difficulty in differentiating REMS from wakefulness only by the EEG in addition to depriving the animals of both NREMS and REMS. Also, during the experiment, free access to food and water is difficult, and appropriate control for activity needs to be designed.

Pendulum method

This technique was designed to avoid the movement restriction-associated possible stress experienced by the animals in other techniques.Citation48 An animal was kept in its home cage, which was put in a pendulum hanging from the roof and made to swing. Because of the swing, due to postural imbalance at the extremes of the swing arc, the animal was forced to remain awake to maintain balance. However, in this technique, the animal although experienced minimal REMS (0–2%), it lost significant NREMS; the animal experienced only 19–30% of NREMS during 72 h of deprivation in the pendulum. The advantage of this method is that no external intervention or elaborate instrumentation is needed. However, the disadvantages of this method are that experimental animals do not get free access to food and water, simultaneous deprivation of several animals is not possible, it is difficult to design specific controls and REMS- or NREMS-specific deprivation is difficult to achieve.

Multiple-platform method

To avoid the immobilization stress experienced by the rats in flowerpot method, van Hulzen and CoenenCitation49 developed this method in which experimental and control animals are put on platforms over water in respective tanks (as in flowerpot method) containing more than one platform (either small or large as the case may be) so that the animal could move freely from one platform to the other. This method was further modified, and more than one rat was kept for REMSD or control in water tanks containing many platformsCitation50 to overcome immobilization as well as social isolation during REMSD. In this method, care is to be taken so that the platforms are neither so close to each other or to the walls of the water tank so that the animals could get support and relax easily to enjoy REMS, nor they should be too far so that the animals cannot move easily from one platform to the other.

As the animals can move across platforms and more than one animal is present in one enclosure, the immobilization and isolation stresses, if at all, are likely to be reduced. However, in practice, we have found (unpublished data, 2018) that one animal jumps onto another platform occupied by another sleeping animal and wakes the latter; that is, all the rats in the multiple small- or large-platform group are sleep deprived (including REMS) to variable duration. Although we have not recorded electrophysiological sleep–wake in these animals (which is technically challenging though), we estimated the brain Na-K ATPase activity as an estimate of REMSD (manuscript under preparation). We have reportedCitation44 that on single-platform method, the enzyme activity increases in the experimental REMS-deprived rats (on small platform) but not in the LPC rats. However, we observed that the enzyme activity increases in the multiple large platform controls (MLPCs) also and remains comparable to that of the REMS-deprived small-platform experimental rat brain as well. Thus, the finding of the MLPCs questions the effectiveness of this method. Comparable behavior of MLPCs has been mentioned in other studies,Citation50 which may be the likely cause of differences in results in such study in addition to noncompliance with the ratio of body weight of the animals to the platform size.

Rotating disc-over-water method

In another modification, a computer-aided elaborate procedure for REMSD was designed.Citation51,Citation52 In this method, the experimental and control rats are simultaneously housed one on each side of a horizontally placed disk suspended over water. A vertical partition was fixed in a manner so that the disk is separated into two halves, although it could rotate unhindered. The arrangement is such that the rat on the disk would fall into the surrounding water if the disk is allowed complete rotation. The experimental and control rats are surgically implanted with electrodes for recording of electrophysiological sleep–wake. The signals are fed into a computer which gives signal to rotate the disk (platform) as and when the experimental rat goes into REMS or NREMS, as the design of experiment. As complete rotation of the disk would push the rat into the water, the animal wakes up to stop the rotation and prevents itself from going into the water below and in turn deprives itself of REMS or NREMS. Although the method overcomes the immobility stress, the control animal is awakened as and when the experimental animal goes into REMS or NREMS (as the case may be). Thus, the control animals must sleep when the experimental animal sleeps, which cannot be practically correct always. In practice, with increase in duration of deprivation period, the experimental rat frequently tends to sleep, and therefore, the control animal is also awakened frequently and is deprived of sleep; thus, the purpose is defeated. This method does not take care of isolation, it is also difficult to deprive many animals simultaneously and it needs elaborate arrangement.

We have compared all the methods of deprivation in . The flowerpot method is by far most suited under laboratory conditions to study REMSD at least in animals. The method is very effective, inexpensive, procedurally simple, does not require continuous monitoring or human or surgical intervention and allows handling several animals simultaneously. However, many experimenters and critics are uncomfortable with this method citing perceived stress experienced by the animals, which needs elaborate discussion.

Table 1 Comparison of different methods of sleep deprivation

“Stress” vs. deprivation-induced specific effects

“Stress” has been coined to describe exposure of an organism to a stimulus that crossed a level, which may vary in intensity and duration. The challenge is to define sub-stressful and stressful stimulus. The response of the organism depends on the environmental factor(s), type of insult (stimulus) including its quality, intensity and frequency, as well as sensitivity, predisposition, adaptation and psychosomatic state of the organism. REMSD studies are not immune to these issues. Often, REMSD studies face criticism that the observed effects may be confounded due to experimental manipulation-induced associated stress, which, if any, certainly needs to be eliminated or ruled out from the observation.

As most of the physiological processes remain in a dynamic equilibrium, it is likely that the response(s) to various exposures would vary. Therefore, it is desirable to decipher if the observed change(s) is a primary or secondary response(s). For objective analysis, it is also important to bear in mind that the observation(s) should be free from subjective bias. For example, subjectively (being emotional) by observing the behavior, particularly of a higher animal, we may be prone to raise the issue of stress as opposed to ignoring responses of cells when exposed to comparable test factor. Ideally, a factor (stimulus) including stressor would be experienced at the structural, molecular and cellular levels; however, their expression(s) may be ignored due to lack of our appreciation, understanding and sensitivity. On the other hand, due to the lack of appropriate definition, a response need not necessarily be categorized as stress response until its molecular mechanism of action is deciphered. Interestingly, under specific conditions, both extremes, for example, overactivity and restricted movement, isolationCitation53 and overcrowding,Citation54 exposure to low and high oxygenation, carbon dioxide,Citation55 glucoseCitation56 and temperatureCitation57 and so on, may turn out to be stressful.

It needs to be distinguished if the exposure-induced effects (responses) are secondary to experimental manipulation or a direct response due to the exposure. While the former needs to be eliminated, for the rest a consistent, exclusive (marker) measurable factor is needed to define stress. Sometimes, the level of corticosterone has been taken as a marker for stress; however, it is known that corticosterone is necessary for growth and development of neurons as well as memory processing in the brainCitation58Citation61 in addition to many other functions. Similarly, elevated levels of NA, adrenal and thymus weight and sympathetic overactivity have been considered as stress response. Thus, if stress response means a compensatory physiological effect, can and should that be differentiated or eliminated, and if that is attempted to differentiate, the physiological processes might react and induce some other change(s), which the observer may or may not be taking note of.

Often, we ignore variations which are statistically insignificant. However, it is possible that there could be synergism among nonsignificant changes, while antagonism among significant changes, which may appear modulating a function and/or a behavior. The nonspecific responses due to experimental intervention may be eliminated by designing appropriate control experiments. Essentially, in principle, it is comparable to eliminate noise from embedded desirable specific response by overlapping stimulus-triggered responses often by averaging repeated observations; the more are the number of observations, the better and reliable would be the averaging results (findings and associated interpretation). Thus, possible stress, if at all (not only perceived), inflicted to the animal while conducting experimental loss of either or both NREMS and REMS, needs to be countered/eliminated by designing adequate and suitable control experiments. Alternatively, if the effects are due to (the so-called) “stressor”, it needs to be shown if the responses can be prevented by blocking the “stress/stressor”. In the absence of a marker, the term “stress” or “stress response” would remain in the domain of speculation and better be avoided. The best way forward will be to conduct adequate and multiple-control experiments to rule out nonspecific effects, to be as much as possible unbiased and objective in our observations.

As discussed earlier in this review, the flowerpot method allows designing appropriate control experiments to rule out various nonspecific effects; therefore, this method has been most extensively used for REMSD studies. However, citing stress, many critics favor multiple-platform method over flowerpot method (single platform). The critical point is whether appropriate controls have been carefully carried out while conducting either of the methods. In studies where multiple platforms have been used, often the critical ratio of animal weight to platform size has not been maintained.Citation50,Citation62,Citation63 If such ratio is not maintained, either the experimental animals are not REMS deprived or the large-platform animals are also deprived of the desired sleep. These could account for conflicting reports of differences in the levels of corticosterone, NA, weight (body, adrenal and thymus), lipid peroxidation and so on, upon deprivation by multiple-platform method carried out by different groups.Citation64Citation66

Also, in many studies,Citation62,Citation63 LPCs were missing, and post-REMSD recovery studies were not conducted, and therefore, one cannot be sure if the observed changes were specific to REMSD. Additionally, it is desirable to show that the changes observed after REMSD can be prevented by blocking a factor induced by REMSD, that is, a causal relationship needs to be shown, which has not been conducted in most studies.Citation63,Citation65 Using flowerpot method, studies from this laboratory have taken care of all the issues raised above and reported consistent findings.Citation67Citation69 It has also been shown that the observed changes were proportional to the duration of REMSD and the observed REMSD-induced changes initially were localized in localized (restricted) brain area and then diffused to other brain areas upon increasing the duration of REMSD.Citation70 In support of our contention, findings from a relatively recent well-controlled study further unequivocally reinforced the efficacy of the flowerpot method for REMSD studies.Citation71

As opposed to long-term REMSD by the flowerpot method, for short-term (4–6 h) deprivation, hand arousal may be used. However, in the latter, often there is total sleep deprivation, and it is debatable how much of the effect is due to deprivation as compared to due to other confounds (mentioned above). Also, it is debatable that if a few hours of deprivation induce some pathophysiological changes, what should be ideal control, and more importantly, whether normally body has compensatory effect for such small loss of REMS in particular. In such short deprivation experiments, it is not accounted for as to what proportion of total REMS is actually lost by the subjects. Also, it is not recorded normally how much REMS is present in control subjects during the clock time of conducting the study and if the subjects’ REMS is dependent on the clock time. Additionally, one does not account for if the deprived subjects experience rebound increase in REMS during the rest of the 24 h period after the short deprivation. Therefore, it is urgently needed to innovate and design appropriate experiment for short-term REMSD. Thus, it may safely be inferred that given the complexity, the flowerpot (single-platform) method with multiple-control experiments appears to be the best possibility for conducting at least REMSD. However, any innovation particularly to reduce number of control studies and maximizing desired deprivation alone in experimental subjects will be a welcome step. Until then we, the sleep researchers, would remain indebted to Michel Jouvet for inventing the flowerpot methodCitation42 and continue using it rather than selectively raising (biased) stress issue for some studies.

Contribution of REMSD studies to understand REMS regulation and functions

Neural regulation of REMS

It has been discussed earlier that transection, lesion, stimulation and pharmacological studies have contributed significantly to unravel the neural regulation of REMS. REMSD studies have complemented many of those studies to support and confirm regulation of REMS, particularly to simulate and/or extrapolate the REMSD-associated disordered states. For example, indeed upon REMSD, the REM-OFF neurons continue firing instead of cessation of activity during REMS, and REM-ON neurons tend to increase firing, possibly an attempt to compensate for REMSD;Citation25 other neurons did not show such trend. In support of the hypothesis that REMS maintains brain excitability,Citation72,Citation73 it has been shown by the dorsolateral pontine neurons that REMSD indeed reduced the auditory stimulation-induced evoked inhibitory responses.Citation74

Necessity of cessation of the LC-REM-OFF neurons for induction of REMS was confirmed by increased REMS in cold-inactivated LC.Citation75 Such studies led us to hypothesize that NA level should rise in the brain during REMSD, which has been shown later. The role of LC REM-OFF neurons and that of released NA from those neurons in inhibiting the REM-ON neurons resulting in non-appearance of REMS and inducing REMSD-induced changes was confirmed recently in in vivo experiments. In brief, it has been shown that if the NA synthesis in those neurons was prevented (downregulated by infusing si- and shRNA of TH), REMS was increased, which however was brought back to normal state if NA was micro-injected into the PPT, the site of REM-ON neurons. Further, if such rats where the NA synthesis was downregulated were deprived of REMS, the REMSD-associated changes in Na-K ATPaseCitation7 and changes in apoptosis-inducing factors were not observed.Citation7 In a series of complementary studies if the LC REM-OFF neurons were prevented to cease activity by electrical stimulation,Citation16 microinjection of GABA antagonistCitation19Citation21 or anti-ouabain antibodies,Citation22 REMS was reduced; that is, REMSD-like condition could be induced. Similarly, changes in the level of GABACitation68 and orexinCitation76 in the brain upon REMSD support their roles in REMS and REMS loss-associated disorders (dealt with later).

Behavioral changes

After the discovery of REMS, while evaluating its physiological significance, REMSD studies were carried out in humans by Dement.Citation4 Hand arousal was used to deprive humans of REMS; the subjects were physically awakened when during sleep they entered into state of “activated EEG”, the REMS. This was perhaps the first experimental study on humans to show that lost REMS needs to be compensated and suggested an indispensable role of REMS in maintaining normal human physiological processes.Citation4 Anxiety, irritability, increased appetite and inability to concentrate were among the other major behavioral symptoms recorded after 3–4 days of REMSD. Subjects almost uniformly showed confusion and symptoms of withdrawal.

In rats, REMSD increases body temperature in the first 24 h of deprivation; however, thereafter, hypothermia sets in, which if uncontrolled could lead to death.Citation5 Other studies reported increased fighting,Citation77 adrenocorticotropic hormone-induced yawning,Citation78 decreased nestingCitation79 and postural imbalance in animals; many of the effects tended to be reversed after recovery of lost REMS. Chronic REMS restriction led to impaired physical development and anxiety-like behavior in adolescent rats, which may be attributed to alterations in the levels of NA and serotonin in amygdala and hippocampus.Citation80 In a study to test the rate of shock avoidance, REMSD rats treated with D-amphetamine were unable to increase avoidance suggesting an impaired catecholaminergic system in REMSD rats. Catecholamine potentiating drugs like imipramine (monoamine reuptake blocker) or pargyline (suppresses monoamine degradation by monoamine oxidases [MAOs]) showed an increased retention in REMSD rats without having any effect on control rats after passive avoidance training. In some studies, effects of REMSD were found to be comparable to those of antidepressants.Citation81

Learning and memory

REMS has been suggested to affect hippocampal memory processing and consolidation,Citation82 while REMSD prevents the performance of learned tasks and impairs new learning processes;Citation83 in other studies, sleep deprivation has been shown to affect object recognition.Citation84 A brief exposure to 24 h REMSD drastically affected object recognition along with alterations in serotonin and NA levels in rat model of Parkinson’s disease.Citation85 Studies focusing on reconsolidation of memory in rats suggest that REMSD selectively disrupts consolidation, but not reconsolidation of novel object recognition.Citation86 REMSD decreased spatial memory in Morris water maze test, while it has been shown to impair reference but not working memory in radial arm maze test.Citation87 The effects have been shown to last for at least 2 weeks as manifested by a reduction in memory performance and by a persistently high apoptotic rate.Citation88 Studies using a combination of behavioral, pharmacological, slice culture and optogenetic techniques showed that NA is involved in threat learning.Citation89 In amygdala, NA acts through β-adrenergic receptors to enhance threat memory by facilitating the association between conditioned and aversive stimulus. Primary culture of rat cortical neurons showed that NA exerts neuroprotective effect against Aβ-induced neurotoxicity;Citation90 these findings may help explain the beneficial effect of REMS as well as lack of adverse effects of REMSD in some studies.Citation91,Citation92

In humans, REMSD showed impairment of face recognition. However, it could not be correlated in these subjects whether the task performance was due to loss of REMS duration or REMS density. In another study employing slow wave sleep (SWS) deprivation and REMSD in different subjects, it was observed that SWS was essential for explicit memory consolidation for visuospatial information, while both SWS deprivation and REMSD adversely affected explicit verbal recall of the tasks.Citation93 Notwithstanding, in some studies, REMSD could not affect emotional memory consolidation,Citation91,Citation92 which could be due to difference in experimental approaches and needs confirmation.

Modulation of neurotransmitter levels

Neurons exert desired effects by release of specific neurotransmitters. Also, the activities of neurons depend on the relative levels of different neurotransmitters acting on them. Isolated studies have shown that several neurotrans mitters like NA, GABA, ACh, orexin, serotonin, histamine, dopamine and glutamate regulate REMS. Initially, based on neuronal behavior, it was predicted that NA level should be elevated in the brain upon REMSD,Citation94 which has since been confirmed.Citation68,Citation95 Our recent study in rats has shown that after 96 h REMSD, NA increases in LC, PPT, cortex and frontal lobe, while it decreases in hippocampus.Citation68 In the same study, we have shown that GABA decreases in LC, PPT and cortex, while it increases in hippocampus after REMSD. During recovery control, the levels of both the neurotransmitters were comparable to free moving controls. The differences in the neurotransmitter levels observed could be due to the differences in the number of projection neurons, density of synapses of the projecting neurons, number of interneurons and firing rates of all these neurons. The role of GABA in REMS regulation was proposed in ratsCitation21 and cats,Citation96,Citation97 which has been confirmed by actual estimation of GABA level in different brain areas in the REMSD rats.Citation68 Similarly, REMSD-associated changes in brain acetylcholinesteraseCitation98 and orexinCitation76 confirm their role in REMS regulation. Orexin level increased significantly in LC, PPT and hypothalamus after REMSD; the maximum increase was observed in the LC.Citation76 This supports the dense projections of orexinergic neurons of posterior hypothalamus to LC. For NA-induced REMSD-associated chronic dysfunction, there should be sustained increase in NA synthesis in the brain upon REMSD. Indeed, it has been reported that the enzymeCitation68,Citation95 and mRNACitation95 of TH, the rate-limiting enzyme involved in the biosynthesis of NA, increase in the brain after REMSD in rats.

Biochemical, cellular and molecular changes

Sleep is a behavioral phenomenon, which is affected by changes in various molecules in the brain; also, its disturbance affects almost every physiological process. Therefore, it is likely that REMS as well as REMSD must have biochemical and molecular correlates, which in turn would be responsible for REMSD-associated pathophysiological changes. REMSD has been shown to decrease both blood and brain potassium levels in rats.Citation99 Brain glycogen (both total and free) decreases after 72 h of REMSD.Citation100 Attempts to understand its effect on glucose metabolism revealed an increase in hexokinase activity (a rate-limiting enzyme in glycolysis) and a decrease in glucose 6-phosphatase activity after 4 days of REMSD in rat brain.Citation101 During REMSD, there is an increase in all the amino acid levels in the brain with the most significant changes occurring in the levels of serine, glycine, alanine, phenylalanine and leucine,Citation102Citation104 although no increase in protein synthesis has been observed.Citation105 Changes in brain ammonia and nitrogen metabolismCitation106 reflect an increased catabolism in the brain after REMSD.

We have mentioned above that REMSD elevated the level of NA, while it decreased GABA in the brain. Tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) and glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) are involved in synthesis of NA and GABA, respectively, while MAO-A degrades NA. Increase and decrease in the activities of the corresponding enzymes of those neurotransmitters, TH, MAO-ACitation70 and GAD, respectively, support and complement each other.Citation19,Citation31 Although ACh level is expected to decrease upon REMSD, its level has not yet been estimated after REMSD. A significant increase in acetylcholinesterase activity in the brain upon REMSDCitation70 supports cholinergic downregulation during REMSD, and thus, supports ACh role in REMS regulation. Several studies in ratsCitation107,Citation108 and in catsCitation109 have considered increased c-fos expression in some neurons as an evidence for neuronal overactivity due to sleep/REMS loss. It has also been shown that wake-induced c-fos expression in cortical neurons is regulated by NA-ergic inputs from LC.Citation110 However, as a critical analysis, it needs to be highlighted that c-fos is an immediate early gene, which is reported to be expressed under various conditions,Citation111Citation113 and hence, the expression may not be exclusively related to sleep/REMS loss. This contention may be supported by the fact that the increased level of c-fos expression reduces with the increase in sleep/REMS loss; that is, its level neither further increases nor remains sustained with continued REMSD.Citation114

Excitability is a fundamental property of the neurons, and Na-K ATPase is a key factor to maintain it. REMSD has been reported to alter brain excitabilityCitation77,Citation115 and increases Na-K ATPase activity as well as expression.Citation111Citation113 It has been proposed that REMSD-induced increased brain excitability is mediated by elevated NA, and thus, REMS maintains the housekeeping function of the brain.Citation67 To understand the REMSD-associated sustained effect on brain excitability, the molecular basis of NA-induced REMSD-associated increase in Na-K ATPase activity has been investigated, and the findings support the excitability hypothesis.Citation116 The role of Ca2+ in modulating Na-K ATPase activity and its expression has also been investigated.Citation117,Citation118 These changes are not nonspecific effects as there were reciprocal changes in neuronal and glial Na-K ATPase activity, and thus, help maintain homeostasisCitation119 in the brain. In other studies, REMSD has been shown to increase phosphorylated synapsin1, which supports REMSD-associated increased vesicular mobilization and neurotransmitter release.Citation120

Cytomorphological changes

It has been shown that REMSD-associated cytomorphological,Citation45,Citation121,Citation122 biochemicalCitation67 and apoptotic changes,Citation69 be it in the whole brain or in localized brain regions including LC and PPT, are mediated by elevated NA. In rats, REMSD altered neuronal perimeter, area and shape in REMS-regulating areas of brainCitation45 and induced neuronal apoptosis,Citation69 and the effects were mediated by NA. Interestingly, the changes differed depending on the functional correlates of the neurons; for example, adrenergic neurons which continue firing during REMSD increased in size, while cholinergic neurons decreased in size. These changes in neuronal cytomorphometry are likely to be the initial changes before the neurons undergo apoptosis leading to REMS loss-associated neurodegenerative disorders.

Effects on epigenetic and genetic levels

REMS is an evolutionarily conserved fundamental physiological process at least in higher animals. Therefore, it is not surprising that its loss, depending on the intensity and predisposition of the subject, affects most physiological processes in short as well as long term. The changes in the factors help explain the immediate and/or short-term effects; however, for sustained long-term effects, it is necessary that REMSD must affect at the epigenetic and genetic levels. Although this field needs serious systematic investigation, some attempts have been made in this direction. For example, continuous physical movement, which presumably caused total sleep loss in drosophila, showed changes in wide variety of gene expressions;Citation123 some of these changes are difficult to explain though. Some studies have been conducted in rats and mice. In a recent REMSD study, changes in chromatin assembly, methylation of genes involved in learning and memory, regulation of synaptic transmission, neuronal plasticity and neurohypophyseal hormone synthesis in rat brain have been reported,Citation124 which help explain REMSD-associated long-term behavioral and other changes described in this review.

Neurodegeneration and associated disorders

An increase in proapoptotic factors like Bcl2-associated death promoter protein, apoptotic protease activating factor-1, cyto chrome c, caspase9 and caspase3 was observed in 6-day-old REMSD rats.Citation69 The effects could be prevented by both intra-peritoneal injection of α1-adrenoceptor antagonist, prazosin, and local microinjection of TH siRNA in LC region of rat brain, suggesting an important role of NA in the induction of REMSD-associated apoptosis.Citation7

REMS loss-associated disorders

We have discussed above that experimental REMSD affects enzymes,Citation125 metabolites,Citation126 neurotransmitters, hormones, immune system including interleukins,Citation127,Citation128 epigenetic changesCitation129 and neuronal structural proteins leading to apoptosisCitation69,Citation121 in the brain. These would affect other somatic systems and form the basis to explain the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms for REMSD-associated pathological conditions including in humans. Indeed, REMS and REMSD affect almost all systems in the body. An irregularity has been observed in heart rate, breathing rate, cardiac output, cardiac pressure and arterial pressure as when the subject enters REMS.Citation130 Also, respiratory reflexes such as response to hypoxia diminish during REMS.Citation131,Citation132 In humans, mean blood pressure has been reported higher during REMS along with progressive decline in heart and respiratory rates.Citation133,Citation134 Both hemispheric and brainstem blood flow increase even higher than during wakefulness. Uncontrolled hypertensive patients have significantly reduced REMS, while REMSD contributes to hypertension and arthrosclerosis by altering blood pressure.Citation135 Altered REMS is considered a characteristic feature of several psychosomatic and cognitive disorders. In Parkinson’s disease, there is an increased latency to sleep and reduced REMS,Citation136 while reduced latency and increased REMS have been reported in several depressed patients.Citation137 As it is difficult to experimentally induce the disorders and study their molecular mechanism of action, we often depend on animal and cellular models to understand the mechanism of action(s).

In young rats, 96 h REMSD leads to decrease in homocysteine, an amino acid that is considered an independent risk factor for cardiovascular disease and stress.Citation138 REMSD by flowerpot method induced significant elevation of blood pressure in rats with partial predisposition to developing hypertension.Citation139 Immune responses vary in relation to sleep conditions, while immune challenge alters sleep profile.Citation140 Sleep gets compromisedCitation128,Citation140,Citation141 in most infections and diseased conditions. The activity and number of natural killer cells and phagocytes decrease upon REMSD, which suggests weakened immune system. This explains why there is an increased tendency of acquiring infection, and lesions in foot, paws and gastric mucosa after total sleep deprivation and REMSD in experimental rats.Citation142 After REMSD, the level of NA increases, and NA is also known to modulate the immune system;Citation143 their relationship under REMS-deprived condition needs to be investigated.

In rats, KushidaCitation144 showed that REMSD decreased plasma thyroxine (T4) and increased the ratio of plasma T3 to T4. This may explain the deficiency in growth, development and maturation of children and animals having disturbed REMS;Citation145 however, their cause-and-effect relationship needs to be confirmed. The author also reported increased protein catabolism as evidenced by an increased level of plasma urea nitrogen. This may explain the mechanism of reduced urine pH, metabolic acidosis, altered kidney function, urinary tract damage and finally animal death particularly upon prolonged REMSD (16–54 days).Citation146

Summary and conclusion

REMS is an essential phenomenon which affects the overall physiology and brain functions including psychosomatic behavior. The deprivation studies have contributed immensely in understanding generation, maintenance, regulation and significance of REMS. We have collated the information gathered using the method to update our knowledge on REMS. As various methods have been used to induce REMSD, we have compared them. Because of its ease and convenience of use, minimum limitations and possibility of conducting various types of controls to rule out nonspecific effects, the flowerpot method is the method of choice for carrying out REMSD studies in animals (rats, mice and cats) across the globe. We conclude that most methods have some limitations, particularly those induced by nonspecific factors. Those nonspecific effects need to be countered and eliminated by conducting suitable and multiple-control experiments rather than ignored or overemphasized including under the category of perceived stress, if any.

Acknowledgments

RM received DST-woman scientist postdoc fellowship. SK received CSIR fellowship while conducting her PhD. BNM received funding through research grants from UGC, DBT and JC Bose fellowship and institutional umbrella support grant under the following: DBT-BUILDER; DST-PURSE and -FIST; UGC-UPEII, -DRS and Resource Networking.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • O’DonnellCPKingEDSchwartzARSmithPLRobothamJLEffect of sleep deprivation on responses to airway obstruction in the sleeping dogJ Appl Physiol198577418111818
  • MendelsonWBGuthrieRDFrederickGWyattRJThe flower pot technique of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivationPharmacol Biochem Behav1974245535564371007
  • KushidaCABergmannBMRechtschaffenASleep deprivation in the rat: IV. Paradoxical sleep deprivationSleep198912122302928623
  • DementWThe effect of dream deprivationScience196013134151705170713815805
  • JaiswalMKMallickBNPrazosin modulates rapid eye movement sleep deprivation-induced changes in body temperature in ratsJ Sleep Res200918334935619552734
  • AserinskyEKleitmanNRegularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleepScience1953118306227327413089671
  • KhandayMASomarajanBIMehtaRMallickBNNoradrenaline from Locus Coeruleus neurons acts on Pedunculo-Pontine neurons to prevent REM sleep and induces its loss associated effects in ratseNeuro201636119
  • MallickBNSinghAKhandayMAActivation of inactivation process initiates rapid eye movement sleepProg Neurobiol201297325927622521402
  • McGintyDJDrucker-ColinRRSleep mechanisms: biology and control of REM sleepInt Rev Neurobiol1982233914366749739
  • SiegelJMPhylogeny and the function of REM sleepBehav Brain Res1995691–229347546315
  • SiegelJMTomaszewskiKSNienhuisRBehavioral states in the chronic medullary and midpontine catElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19866332742882419085
  • CarliGZanchettiAA study of pontine lesions suppressing deep sleep in the catArch Ital Biol196510347517885325945
  • HenleyKMorrisonARA re-evaluation of the effects of lesions of the pontine tegmentum and locus coeruleus on phenomena of paradoxical sleep in the catActa Neurobiol Exp (Wars)19743422152324368348
  • JonesBEElimination of paradoxical sleep by lesions of the pontine gigantocellular tegmental field in the catNeurosci Lett1979133285293231225
  • El MansariMSakaiKJouvetMUnitary characteristics of presumptive cholinergic tegmental neurons during the sleep-waking cycle in freely moving catsExp Brain Res19897635195292551709
  • SinghSMallickBNMild electrical stimulation of pontine tegmentum around locus coeruleus reduces rapid eye movement sleep in ratsNeurosci Res19962432272358815443
  • Aston-JonesGBloomFEActivity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycleJ Neurosci1981188768867346592
  • JacobsBLSingle unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons in behaving animalsProg Neurobiol19862721831943529238
  • KaurSPanchalMFaisalMMadanVNangiaPMallickBNLong term blocking of GABA-A receptor in locus coeruleus by bilateral microinfusion of picrotoxin reduced rapid eye movement sleep and increased brain Na-K ATPase activity in freely moving normally behaving ratsBehav Brain Res20041511–218519015084434
  • KaurSSaxenaRNMallickBNGABA in locus coeruleus regulates spontaneous rapid eye movement sleep by acting on GABAA receptors in freely moving ratsNeurosci Lett199722321051089089684
  • MallickBNKaurSSaxenaRNInteractions between cholinergic and GABAergic neurotransmitters in and around the locus coeruleus for the induction and maintenance of rapid eye movement sleep in ratsNeuroscience2001104246748511377848
  • JaiswalMKDvelaMLichtsteinDMallickBNEndogenous ouabain-like compounds in locus coeruleus modulate rapid eye movement sleep in ratsJ Sleep Res20101918319119878449
  • ThankachanSIslamFMallickBNRole of wake inducing brain stem area on rapid eye movement sleep regulation in freely moving catsBrain Res Bull2001551434911427336
  • MallickBNThankachanSIslamFInfluence of hypnogenic brain areas on wakefulness- and rapid-eye-movement sleep-related neurons in the brainstem of freely moving catsJ Neurosci Res200475113314214689456
  • MallickBNSiegelJMFahringerHChanges in pontine unit activity with REM sleep deprivationBrain Res19905151–294982357583
  • PalDMadanVMallickBNNeural mechanism of rapid eye movement sleep generation: cessation of locus coeruleus neurons is a necessityShenq Li Xue Bao2005574401413
  • PalDMallickBNNeural mechanism of rapid eye movement sleep generation with reference to REM-OFF neurons in locus coeruleusIndian J Med Res2007125672173917704548
  • KumarRBoseAMallickBNA mathematical model towards understanding the mechanism of neuronal regulation of wake-NREMS-REMS statesPLoS One201278e4205922905114
  • BerodAChatMPautLTappazMCatecholaminergic and GABAergic anatomical relationship in the rat substantia nigra, locus coeruleus, and hypothalamic median eminence: immunocytochemical visualization of biosynthetic enzymes on serial semithin plastic-embedded sectionsJ Histochem Cytochem19843212133113386150057
  • AliMJhaSKKaurSMallickBNRole of GABA-A receptor in the preoptic area in the regulation of sleep-wakefulness and rapid eye movement sleepNeurosci Res199933324525010211770
  • KaurSSaxenaRNMallickBNGABAergic neurons in prepositus hypoglossi regulate REM sleep by its action on locus coeruleus in freely moving ratsSynapse200142314115011746711
  • OertelWHTappazMLBerodAMugnainiETwo-color immunohistochemistry for dopamine and GABA neurons in rat substantia nigra and zona incertaBrain Res Bull198291–64634746129046
  • PalDMallickBNGABA in pedunculo pontine tegmentum regulates spontaneous rapid eye movement sleep by acting on GABA-A receptors in freely moving ratsNeurosci Lett2004365320020415246548
  • WeberFHoang DoJPChungSRegulation of REM and non-REM sleep by periaqueductal GABAergic neuronsNat Commun20189135429367602
  • SapinELaprayDBérodALocalization of the brainstem GABAergic neurons controlling paradoxical (REM) sleepPLoS One200941e427219169414
  • LeskuJARothTCAmlanerCJLimaSLA phylogenetic analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: the integration of anatomy, physiology, and ecologyAm Nat2006168444145317004217
  • SiegelJMClues to the functions of mammalian sleepNature200543770631264127116251951
  • MarksGAShafferyJPOksenbergASpecialeSGRoffwargHPA functional role for REM sleep in brain maturationBehav Brain Res1995691–2111
  • MirmiranMVan SomerenESymposium: normal and abnormal REM sleep regulation: the importance of REM sleep for brain maturationJ Sleep Res19932418819210607093
  • ManentJBRepresaANeurotransmitters and brain maturation: early paracrine actions of GABA and glutamate modulate neuronal migrationNeuroscientist200713326827917519369
  • SandersJDHappeHKBylundDBMurrinLCDifferential effects of neonatal norepinephrine lesions on immediate early gene expression in developing and adult rat brainNeuroscience2008157482183218938224
  • JouvetDVimontPDelormeFJouvetMEtude de la privation selective de la phase paradoxale de sommeil chez le Chat [Study of selective deprivation of the paradoxal sleep phase in the cat]C R Seances Soc Biol Fil1964158756759 French [with English abstract]14186938
  • YanikGRadulovackiMREM sleep deprivation up-regulates adenosine A1 receptorsBrain Res198740223623643030496
  • GulyaniSMallickBNEffect of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation on rat brain Na-K ATPase activityJ Sleep Res199321455010607070
  • MajumdarSMallickBNCytomorphometric changes in rat brain neurons after rapid eye movement sleep deprivationNeuroscience2005135367969016154283
  • Guzman-MarinRSuntsovaNBashirTNienhuisRSzymusiakRMcGintyDRapid eye movement sleep deprivation contributes to reduction of neurogenesis in the hippocampal dentate gyrus of the adult ratSleep200831216717518274263
  • FergusonJDementWThe effect of variations in total sleep time on the occurrence of rapid eye movement sleep in catsElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19672212104163480
  • van HulzenZJCoenenAMThe pendulum technique for paradoxical sleep deprivation in ratsPhysiol Behav19802568078117220622
  • van HulzenZJCoenenAMParadoxical sleep deprivation and locomotor activity in ratsPhysiol Behav19812747417447323178
  • MachadoRBHipólideDCBenedito-SilvaAATufikSSleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique: quantification of sleep loss and recoveryBrain Res200410041–2455115033418
  • RechtschaffenABergmannBMSleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water methodBehav Brain Res1995691–255637546318
  • BergmannBMKushidaCAEversonCAGillilandMAObermeyerWRechtschaffenASleep deprivation in the rat: II. MethodologySleep19891215122928625
  • HawkleyLCColeSWCapitanioJPNormanGJCacioppoJTEffects of social isolation on glucocorticoid regulation in social mammalsHorm Behav201262331432322663934
  • UarquinDGMeyerJSCardenasFPRojasMJEffect of overcrowding on hair corticosterone concentrations in juvenile male Wistar ratsJ Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci201655674975527931312
  • ConleeKMStephensMLRowanANKingLACarbon dioxide for euthanasia: concerns regarding pain and distress, with special reference to mice and ratsLab Anim200539213716115901358
  • TorresILGamaroGDSilveira-CuccoSNEffect of acute and repeated restraint stress on glucose oxidation to CO2 in hippocampal and cerebral cortex slicesBraz J Med Biol Res200134111111611151035
  • JimenoBHauMVerhulstSStrong association between corticosterone levels and temperature-dependent metabolic rate in individual zebra finchesJ Exp Biol2017220234426443129051225
  • BarsegyanAMackenzieSMKuroseBDMcGaughJLRoozendaalBGlucocorticoids in the prefrontal cortex enhance memory consolidation and impair working memory by a common neural mechanismProc Natl Acad Sci U S A201010738166551666020810923
  • ChatterjeeSSikdarSKCorticosterone targets distinct steps of synaptic transmission via concentration specific activation of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptorsJ Neurochem2014128447649024117520
  • KulaJGugulaABlasiakADiverse action of repeated corticosterone treatment on synaptic transmission, neuronal plasticity, and morphology in superficial and deep layers of the rat motor cortexPflugers Arch2017469111519153228748319
  • SandiCRoseSPCorticosterone enhances long-term retention in one-day-old chicks trained in a weak passive avoidance learning paradigmBrain Res199464711061128069692
  • MachadoRBTufikSSucheckiDRole of corticosterone on sleep homeostasis induced by REM sleep deprivation in ratsPLoS One201385e6352023667630
  • SucheckiDTufikSSocial stability attenuates the stress in the modified multiple platform method for paradoxical sleep deprivation in the ratPhysiol Behav200068330931610716540
  • MathangiDCShyamalaRSubhashiniASEffect of REM sleep deprivation on the antioxidant status in the brain of Wistar ratsAnn Neurosci201219416116425205991
  • SucheckiDLoboLLHipólideDCTufikSIncreased ACTH and corticosterone secretion induced by different methods of paradoxical sleep deprivationJ Sleep Res1998742762819844854
  • SüerCDoluNArtisASSahinLYilmazACetinAThe effects of long-term sleep deprivation on the long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus and brain oxidation status in ratsNeurosci Res2011701717721256900
  • MallickBNSinghAREM sleep loss increases brain excitability: role of noradrenaline and its mechanism of actionSleep Med Rev201115316517821482157
  • MehtaRSinghSKhandayMAMallickBNReciprocal changes in noradrenaline and GABA levels in discrete brain regions upon rapid eye movement sleep deprivation in ratsNeurochem Int201710819019828427932
  • SomarajanBIKhandayMAMallickBNRapid eye movement sleep deprivation induces neuronal apoptosis by noradrenaline acting on alpha1 adrenoceptor and by triggering mitochondrial intrinsic pathwayFront Neurol201672527014180
  • ThakkarMMallickBNEffect of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation on rat brain monoamine oxidasesNeuroscience19935536776838413930
  • McDermottCMLaHosteGJChenCMustoABazanNGMageeJCSleep deprivation causes behavioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neuronsJ Neurosci200323299687969514573548
  • MallickBNThakkarMGulyaniSRapid eye movement sleep deprivation induced alteration in neuronal excitability: possible role of norepinephrineMallickBNSinghREnvironment and PhysiologyNew DelhiNarosa Publishing House1994196203
  • MallickBNAdyaHVAThankachanSREM sleep deprivation alters factors affecting neuronal excitability: role of norepinephrine and its possible mechanism of actionMallickBNInoueSRapid Eye Movement SleepMarcel Dekker IncNew York, USA1999338354
  • MallickBNFahringerHMWuMFSiegelJMREM sleep deprivation reduces auditory evoked inhibition of dorsolateral pontine neuronsBrain Res199155223333371913194
  • CespuglioRGomezMEFaradjiHJouvetMAlterations in the sleep-waking cycle induced by cooling of the locus coeruleus areaElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19825455705786181980
  • MehtaRKhandayMAMallickBNREM sleep loss associated changes in orexin-A levels in discrete brain areas in ratsNeurosci Lett2015590626725637698
  • MordenBConnerRMitchellGDementWLevineSEffects of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation on shock-induced fightingPhysiol Behav196833425432
  • LoboLLNeumannBGEidmanDSTufikSEffects of REM sleep deprivation of ACTH-induced yawningPharmacology19904031741782159158
  • Canal-FrederickGQuinnCPGillinJCWyattRJDeprivation of rapid eye movement sleep and nesting behavior in ratsPhysiol Behav1977182341344194271
  • da Silva Rocha-LopesJMachadoRBSucheckiDChronic REM sleep restriction in juvenile male rats induces anxiety-like behavior and alters monoamine systems in the amygdala and hippocampusMol Neurobiol Epub2017428
  • MaudhuitCJolasTChastanetMHamonMAdrienJReduced inhibitory potency of serotonin reuptake blockers on central serotoninergic neurons in rats selectively deprived of rapid eye movement sleepBiol Psychiatry19964010100010078915559
  • GivensBSOltonDSCholinergic and GABAergic modulation of medial septal area: effect on working memoryBehav Neurosci199010468498552178347
  • PearlmanCBeckerMBrief posttrial REM sleep deprivation impairs discrimination learning in ratsPhysiol Psychol197314373376
  • PalchykovaSWinsky-SommererRMeerloPDürrRToblerISleep deprivation impairs object recognition in miceNeurobiol Learn Mem200685326327116423541
  • dos SantosVVSantosDBLachGNeuropeptide Y (NPY) prevents depressive-like behavior, spatial memory deficits and oxidative stress following amyloid-β (Abeta(1-40)) administration in miceBehav Brain Res201324410711123396168
  • ChenLTianSKeJRapid eye movement sleep deprivation disrupts consolidation but not reconsolidation of novel object recognition memory in ratsNeurosci Lett2014563121624472565
  • SmithCTConwayJMRoseGMBrief paradoxical sleep deprivation impairs reference, but not working, memory in the radial arm maze taskNeurobiol Learn Mem19986922112179619997
  • Soto-RodriguezSLopez-ArmasGLuquinSRapid eye movement sleep deprivation produces long-term detrimental effects in spatial memory and modifies the cellular composition of the subgranular zoneFront Cell Neurosci20161013211326858601
  • SearsRMFinkAEWigestrandMBFarbCRde LeceaLLeDouxJEOrexin/hypocretin system modulates amygdala-dependent threat learning through the locus coeruleusProc Natl Acad Sci U S A201311050202602026524277819
  • MadrigalJLKalininSRichardsonJCFeinsteinDLNeuroprotective actions of noradrenaline: effects on glutathione synthesis and activation of peroxisome proliferator activated receptor deltaJ Neurochem200710352092210117854349
  • MorgenthalerJWiesnerCDHinzeKAbelsLCPrehn-KristensenAGöderRSelective REM-sleep deprivation does not diminish emotional memory consolidation in young healthy subjectsPLoS One201492e8984924587073
  • WiesnerCDPulstJKrauseFThe effect of selective REM-sleep deprivation on the consolidation and affective evaluation of emotional memoriesNeurobiol Learn Mem201512213114125708092
  • CaseySJSolomonsLCSteierJSlow wave and REM sleep deprivation effects on explicit and implicit memory during sleepNeuropsychology201630893194527797541
  • MallickBNMajumdarSFaisalMYadavVMadanVPalDRole of norepinephrine in the regulation of rapid eye movement sleepJ Biosci200227553955112381879
  • Porkka-HeiskanenTSmithSETairaTNoradrenergic activity in rat brain during rapid eye movement sleep deprivation and rebound sleepAm J Physiol19952686 Pt 2R1456R14637611522
  • NitzDSiegelJGABA release in the dorsal raphe nucleus: role in the control of REM sleepAm J Physiol19972731 Pt 2R451R4559249585
  • NitzDSiegelJMGABA release in posterior hypothalamus across sleep-wake cycleAm J Physiol19962716 Pt 2R1707R17128997373
  • ThakkarMMallickBNEffect of REM sleep deprivation on rat brain acetylcholinesterasePharmacol Biochem Behav19913912112141924507
  • HeinerLGodinYMarkJMandelPElectrolyte content of brain and blood after deprivation of paradoxical sleepJ Neurochem19681521501515637725
  • KaradzicVMrsuljaBDeprivation of paradoxical sleep and brain glycogenJ Neurochem196916129344304854
  • ThakkarMMallickBNRapid eye movement sleep-deprivation-induced changes in glucose metabolic enzymes in rat brainSleep19931686916948165381
  • BettendorffLSallanon-MoulinMTouretMWinsPMargineanuISchoffenielsEParadoxical sleep deprivation increases the content of glutamate and glutamine in rat cerebral cortexSleep199619165718650466
  • DavisJMHimwichWAStoutMCerebral amino acids during deprivation of paradoxical sleepBiol Psychiatry1969143873904312858
  • MicićDKaradzićVRakicLMChanges of gamma-aminobutyric acid, glutamic acid and aspartic acid in various brain structures of cats deprived of paradoxical sleepNature196721550971691706058284
  • BobillierPSakaiFSeguinSJouvetMDeprivation of paradoxical sleep and in vitro cerebral protein synthesis in the ratLife Sci1971102313491357
  • DostálováKNavrátilJSklenovskýANitrogen metabolism in the CNS after paradoxical sleep deprivation in ratsAct Nerv Super19782015758
  • CirelliCPompeianoMTononiGSleep deprivation and c-fos expression in the rat brainJ Sleep Res1995429210610607147
  • PompeianoMCirelliCTononiGEffects of sleep deprivation on Fos-like immunoreactivity in the rat brainArch Ital Biol199213043253351489251
  • ShiromaniPJMalikMWinstonSMcCarleyRWTime course of Fos-like immunoreactivity associated with cholinergically induced REM sleepJ Neurosci1995155 Pt 1350035087751925
  • CirelliCPompeianoMTononiGNeuronal gene expression in the waking state: a role for the locus coeruleusScience19962745290121112158895474
  • BullittEExpression of C-fos-like protein as a marker for neuronal activity following noxious stimulation in the ratJ Comp Neurol199029645175302113539
  • HoffmanGESmithMSVerbalisJGc-Fos and related immediate early gene products as markers of activity in neuroendocrine systemsFront Neuroendocrinol19931431732138349003
  • SmeyneRJVendrellMHaywardMContinuous c-fos expression precedes programmed cell death in vivoNature199336364251661698483500
  • Merchant-NancyHVazquezJAguilar-RobleroRDrucker-ColinRc-fos proto-oncogene changes in relation to REM sleep durationBrain Res199257923423461628220
  • Rosales-LagardeAArmonyJLDel Rio-PortillaYTrejo-MartinezDCondeRCorsi-CabreraMEnhanced emotional reactivity after selective REM sleep deprivation in humans: an fMRI studyFront Behav Neurosci201262511322279431
  • AmarMMallickBNRapid eye movement sleep deprivation associated increase in Na-K ATPase activity in the rat brain is due to noradrenaline induced α1-adrenoceptor mediated increased α-subunit of the enzymeNeurochem Res20154081747175726148532
  • MallickBNAdyaHVFaisalMNorepinephrine-stimulated increase in Na-K ATPase activity in the rat brain is mediated through alpha1A-adrenoceptor possibly by dephosphorylation of the enzymeJ Neurochem20007441574157810737614
  • MallickBNAdyaHVNorepinephrine induced alpha-adrenoceptor mediated increase in rat brain Na-K ATPase activity is dependent on calcium ionNeurochem Int199934649950710402225
  • BaskeyGSinghASharmaRMallickBNREM sleep deprivation-induced noradrenaline stimulates neuronal and inhibits glial Na-K ATPase in rat brain: in vivo and in vitro studiesNeurochem Int2009541657119013490
  • SinghSAmarMMallickBNRapid eye movement sleep deprivation modulates synapsin I expression in rat brainNeurosci Lett20125201626622609569
  • BiswasSMishraPMallickBNIncreased apoptosis in rat brain after rapid eye movement sleep lossNeuroscience2006142231533116887278
  • RanjanABiswasSMallickBNCytomorphometric changes in the dorsal raphe neurons after rapid eye movement sleep deprivation are mediated by noradrenalin in ratsBehav Brain Funct20106621820047681
  • CirelliCBusheyDSleep and wakefulness in Drosophila melanogasterAnn N Y Acad Sci2008112932332918591491
  • NarwadeSCMallickBNDeobagkarDDTranscriptome analysis reveals altered expression of memory and neurotransmission associated genes in the REM sleep deprived rat brainFront Mol Neurosci2017106728367113
  • BowersMBJrHartmannELFreedmanDXSleep deprivation and brain acetylcholineScience19661533742141614175917781
  • SpiegelKLeproultRVan CauterEImpact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine functionLancet199935491881435143910543671
  • IrwinMThompsonJMillerCGillinJCZieglerMEffects of sleep and sleep deprivation on catecholamine and interleukin-2 levels in humans: clinical implicationsJ Clin Endocrinol Metab19998461979198510372697
  • RedwineLHaugerRLGillinJCIrwinMEffects of sleep and sleep deprivation on interleukin-6, growth hormone, cortisol, and melatonin levels in humansJ Clin Endocrinol Metab200085103597360311061508
  • MehtaRSinghABókkonINath MallickBREM sleep and its loss-associated epigenetic regulation with reference to noradrenaline in particularCurr Neuropharmacol2016141284026813120
  • FletcherECGrayBALevinDCNonapneic mechanisms of arterial oxygen desaturation during rapid-eye-movement sleepJ Appl Physiol Respir Environ Exerc Physiol19835436326396841208
  • CatterallJRCalverleyPMMacNeeWMechanism of transient nocturnal hypoxemia in hypoxic chronic bronchitis and emphysemaJ Appl Physiol (1985)1985596169817034077777
  • SchaefferJIHaddadGGCardiovascular response to hypoxia in puppies during natural sleepPediatr Res198418344345
  • SnyderFHobsonJAGoldfrankFBlood pressure changes during human sleepScience196314235971313131414074846
  • SnyderFHobsonJAMorrisonDFGoldfrankFChanges in respiration, heart rate, and systolic blood pressure in human sleepJ Appl Physiol19641941742214174589
  • AndersenMLMartinsPJD’AlmeidaVSantosRFBignottoMTufikSEffects of paradoxica l sleep deprivation on blood parameters associated with cardiovascular risk in aged ratsExp Gerontol200439581782415130676
  • PoryazovaRGZacharievZIREM sleep behavior disorder in patients with Parkinson’s diseaseFolia Med (Plovdiv)2005471510
  • PalaginiLBaglioniCCiapparelliAGemignaniARiemannDREM sleep dysregulation in depression: state of the artSleep Med Rev201317537739023391633
  • de OliveiraACD’AlmeidaVHipólideDCNobregaJNTufikSSleep deprivation reduces total plasma homocysteine levels in ratsCan J Physiol Pharmacol200280319319711991229
  • NevesFAMarsonOBaumgratzRPRapid eye movement sleep deprivation and hypertension. Genetic influenceHypertension1992192 SupplII202II2061735579
  • OppMRSleeping to fuel the immune system: mammalian sleep and resistance to parasitesBMC Evol Biol2009981019134176
  • RuizFSAndersenMLMartinsRCZagerALopesJDTufikSImmune alterations after selective rapid eye movement or total sleep deprivation in healthy male volunteersInnate Immun2012181445421088046
  • RechtschaffenABergmannBMEversonCAKushidaCAGillilandMASleep deprivation in the rat: X. Integration and discussion of the findingsSleep198912168872648533
  • KohmAPSandersVMNorepinephrine: a messenger from the brain to the immune systemImmunol Today2000211153954211094255
  • KushidaCASleep Deprivation: Clinical Issues, Pharmacology, and Sleep Loss EffectsNew YorkMarcel Dekker2005
  • ToppilaJAsikainenMAlankoLTurekFWStenbergDPorkka-HeiskanenTThe effect of REM sleep deprivation on somatostatin and growth hormone-releasing hormone gene expression in the rat hypothalamusJ Sleep Res1996521151228795812
  • ThakkarMMMcCarleyRWPhysiological and neurophysiological changesKushidaCASleep Deprivation: Basic Science, Physiology, and BehaviorNew YorkMarcel Dekker2005265292