926
Views
98
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Estimating adolescent sleep patterns: parent reports versus adolescent self-report surveys, sleep diaries, and actigraphy

, , , &
Pages 23-26 | Published online: 12 Feb 2013

Abstract

Background

In research and clinical contexts, parent reports are often used to gain information about the sleep patterns of their adolescents; however, the degree of concordance between parent reports and adolescent-derived measures is unclear. The present study compares parent estimates of adolescent sleep patterns with adolescent self-reports from surveys and sleep diaries, together with actigraphy.

Methods

A total of 308 adolescents (59% male) aged 13–17 years completed a school sleep habits survey during class time at school, followed by a 7-day sleep diary and wrist actigraphy. Parents completed the Sleep, Medical, Education and Family History Survey.

Results

Parents reported an idealized version of their adolescent’s sleep, estimating significantly earlier bedtimes on both school nights and weekends, significantly later wake times on weekends, and significantly more sleep than either the adolescent self-reported survey, sleep diary, or actigraphic estimates.

Conclusion

Parent reports indicate that the adolescent averages a near-optimal amount of sleep on school nights and a more than optimal amount of sleep on weekends. However, adolescent-derived averages indicate patterns of greater sleep restriction. These results illustrate the importance of using adolescent-derived estimates of sleep patterns in this age group and the importance of sleep education for both adolescents and their parents.

Introduction

Adolescent sleep patterns are measured in different ways. Many studies have relied upon survey data to estimate adolescent sleep patterns, either using parent report,Citation1Citation6 adolescent self-report surveys, or sleep diaries,Citation7 but fewer have utilized actigraphic estimates of sleep.Citation8 There is some uncertainty around the differences in results obtained by these different methods,Citation7,Citation9 and around parent reporting in particular.Citation10 One study found that parents of children aged 3–10 years overestimated their child’s sleep by an average of one hour and 53 minutes per night, compared with concurrently recorded actigraphic estimates.Citation10 Parents may be susceptible to socially desirable responding when estimating the sleep patterns of their child.Citation11 Further, parent reports of their adolescent’s sleep may align more closely with the time that adolescents spend in their bedroom at night, because parents are not aware of the length of time spent awake after lights out. Given the many biopsychosocial factors which predispose adolescents to later sleep-onset times (such as circadian phase delay, changes in accumulation of homeostatic sleep pressure, and increased academic and social responsibilities), plus the propensity for adolescents to access computers, mobile phones, and music players after bedtime, parents may not be in a position to provide accurate estimates of sleep.Citation12,Citation13 An exception to this may be wake times, as parents of adolescents become less involved in regulating bedtimes but more involved in waking them in the morning.Citation13

This is a concern that has substantial ramifications in both clinical and research settings. If parents are not able to describe their adolescent’s sleep patterns accurately, it calls into question previous research which has used this methodology. In addition, if parents do not have an accurate estimation of their teen’s sleep, especially if they overestimate their sleep, they may not be aware of problematic levels of restricted sleep and so may not provide the support required to address this. To address this concern, the present study compared adolescent survey, sleep diary, and actigraphic estimates of sleep and time in bed with parent-reported sleep. Consistent with the findings of Dayyat et alCitation10 among children aged 3–10 years, it is hypothesized that parent reports will significantly overestimate sleep duration compared with adolescent-derived estimates and actigraphy. It is also hypothesized that parents will significantly underestimate the lateness of adolescent bedtimes, but there will be no significant difference between parent and adolescent reports of wake times. The present study extends the previous work by including adolescent-reported survey and sleep diary data in addition to actigraphy.

Materials and methods

Participants

In total, 308 adolescents of mean age 15.6 ± 0.94 (range 13–17) years (59% male) with a parent or caregiver (77.2% mothers, 18.9% fathers, and 3.9% another family members/caregivers) participated in this study. Participants were drawn from eight high schools in South Australia and were in years 9, 10, and 11. Mean school start time was 8:32 am (standard deviation 0.28 hours). The study had no exclusion criteria, and adolescents were reimbursed for their time with a $40 gift voucher. The Flinders University Social and Behavioral Research Ethics Committee and the Department of Education and Children’s Service approved this study.

Measures

Adolescents completed a modified Sleep Habits SurveyCitation14 during class time at school. This survey contained items relating to bedtime, wake time, and total sleep time for both school nights and weekends. For the following week, they completed a sleep diary morning and night, which asked adolescents to record their daily bedtime, wake time, and sleep duration. Adolescents were asked to fill out the sleep diary just before they went to bed each night and as soon as they woke in the morning. Adolescents indicated exact times for all sleep variables.

In order to provide an objective measure of sleep, adolescents wore MicroMini Motionlogger activity monitors (Ambulatory Monitoring, Ardsley, NY, USA) on the nondominant wrist. Data were digitized in one-minute epochs using zero crossing mode, with a sensitivity of 0.05 g, and analyzed with Action W2 software using the Sadeh algorithm. Diary data were used to define the scoring interval for sleep according to the procedure outlined by Acebo et al.Citation15 While sleep onset is a biological state change that can be inferred from actigraphic movement data, bedtime is a behavioral phenomenon that cannot be captured by analysis of movement. Unfortunately, the activity monitors used were not equipped with an event marker function to capture bedtime. As such, data regarding bedtime were collected from self-reported and parent-reported surveys and sleep diaries only.

Parents completed the Sleep, Medical, Education and Family History Survey developed by Carskadon et al at the Sleep for Science research laboratory at Brown University (Carskadon, unpublished data, 1999). In addition to demographic items, this tool contains questions about the time when the adolescent went to bed and woke up on school (Sunday to Thursday) nights and weekends (Friday and Saturday nights).

Results

One-way repeated-measures analyses of variance with post hoc comparisons were utilized to compare average values of sleep parameters according to measurement mode, using the Statistical Program for the Social Sciences version 20 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). Analyses of variance were conducted separately for school nights and weekend nights for each sleep parameter. shows mean values for sleep variables according to measurement mode (either actigraphy, sleep diary, self-report survey, or parent report), together with mean differences between parent reports and each measure and the significance of any differences observed. Bonferroni adjustments were made to allow for multiple comparisons.

Table 1 Mean values for sleep parameters from parent report compared to wrist actigraphy, sleep diary, and self-report survey, together with mean (±se) differences

The results of this study demonstrate significant and substantial differences between the averages estimated by actigraphy, sleep diary, and self-report of the adolescents and those reported by parents. Parents reported that their adolescents went to bed 14–18 minutes earlier and obtained 35–45 minutes more sleep on school nights compared with adolescent-derived estimates. On weekends, parents reported that their adolescents went to bed 18–35 minutes earlier, and obtained 40–92 minutes more sleep per night than adolescent-derived estimates. Parent wake time estimates on school days were more similar to the reports derived from their adolescents, with only sleep diary estimates being a significant, but small, 10 minutes later than the parent report. Unlike wake times on school mornings, parent estimates of weekend sleep were more divergent, with parents estimating wake times occurring 47–51 minutes later than adolescent sleep diary and actigraphic estimates. The survey reported that weekend wake times were not significantly different from parent reports.

Discussion

How should we measure adolescent sleep?

To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to compare parent reports of adolescent sleep with adolescent self-report, sleep diary self-report, and actigraphy in typically developing adolescents. When comparing the different ways of collecting data on adolescent sleep, significant differences in results were obtained depending on the method used. Parents reported a much more idealized sleep pattern, estimating that their adolescent went to bed earlier and obtained more sleep on both school nights and on weekends than did the adolescent. These results are consistent with those found in younger children.Citation10 While there were some significant differences in school morning wake time estimates between parent reports and actigraphic and sleep diary estimates, these differences were small. Parents may be more accurate in estimating their teens’ school morning wake times compared with bedtimes because of the diminished parental involvement in regulating adolescent bedtimes and increased involvement in waking teens for school with increasing age. On weekends, the difference between parent reports and sleep diary and actigraphy was greater for wake time, with parents overestimating wake times by 48–50 minutes. This finding is unexpected, and inconsistent with previous findings.Citation10 It may be that parents are less involved in adolescent wake times on weekends when compared with school mornings, and so are not able to provide accurate wake time estimates.Citation16 In addition, adolescents are more likely to maintain quiet wakefulness in the morning without disturbing their parent/s than are younger children.

The differences between parent and adolescent and actigraphic estimates of sleep are important, and show that adolescent sleep is likely to be much more restricted than their parents believe. In fact, parent reports reveal that they believe their teens are obtaining a near-optimal amount of sleep.Citation8 This is significant on two levels. The first important implication of these findings regards the utility of parent reports for sleep research. Previous studies that have utilized parent reports to estimate sleep are called into question. At the very least, it is likely that these reports overestimate sleep and underestimate bedtimes. The present study provides support for the use of adolescent reports in preference to parent reports of teen sleep. Secondly, it highlights the importance of educating parents about teen sleep. It may be that because teens are less visible than younger children after they go to bed, parents assume that they are asleep, when in fact they are likely to be awake. Particularly with the presence of a wide array of electronic media in the bedroom and the biological propensity toward phase delay and wakefulness in the evening, teens may quietly maintain wakefulness well into the night without parental awareness.Citation17,Citation18 Parents can have an important role in regulating the sleep patterns of adolescents, with associated improvements in daytime functioning, but are unlikely to act if they are not aware that their teens is not obtaining sufficient sleep.Citation19

A strength of this study is the range of sleep measures obtained, in particular sleep diary and actigraphy, in a relatively large sample of adolescents and parents. The present study highlights the importance of relying upon adolescent-derived estimates to obtain data on sleep patterns in this age group. In addition, parent education is indicated to enable parents to have a more accurate picture of their adolescents’ sleep.

Acknowledgment

This work was conducted at the Flinders University of South Australia and was funded by the Australian Research Council (grant DP0881261).

Disclosure

The authors declare no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • IglowsteinIJenniOGMolinariLLargoRHSleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trendsPediatrics200311130230712563055
  • GangwischJEBabissLAMalaspinaDTurnerJBZammitGKPosnerKEarlier parental set bedtimes as a protective factor against depression and suicidal ideationSleep2010339710620120626
  • SmaldoneAHonigJCByrneMWSleepless in America: inadequate sleep and relationships to health and wellbeing of our nations’ childrenPediatrics2007119529537
  • GregoryAMVan der EndeJWillisTAVerhulstFCParent-reported sleep problems during development and self-reported anxiety/depression, attention problems, and aggressive behavior later in lifeArch Pediatr Adolesc Med200816233033518391141
  • PirinenTKolhoKLSimolaPAshornMAronenETParent and self-report of sleep problems and daytime tiredness among adolescents with inflammatory bowel disease and their population based controlsSleep2010331487149521102990
  • LabergeLPetitDSimardCVitaroFTremblayREMontplasirJDevelopment of sleep patterns in early adolescenceJ Sleep Res200110596711285056
  • WolfsonARCarskadonMAAceboCEvidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescentsSleep20032621321612683482
  • GradisarMGardnerGDohntHRecent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: a review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleepSleep Med20101211011821257344
  • ShortMAGradisarMLackLWrightHCarskadonMAThe discrepancy between actigraphic and sleep diary measures of sleep in adolescentsSleep Med20121337838422437142
  • DayyatEASpruytKMolfeseDLGozalDSleep estimates in children: parental versus actigraphic assessmentsNat Sci Sleep20113115123
  • PaulsCAStemmlerGSubstance and bias in social desirability respondingPers Individ Dif200335263275
  • JenniOGAchermannPCarskadonMAHomeostatic sleep regulation in adolescentsSleep2005281446145416335485
  • National Sleep FoundationSleep in America Poll2006Washington, DC Available from: http://www.sleepfoundation.org/article/sleep-america-polls/2006-teens-and-sleepAccessed November 16, 2012
  • WolfsonARCarskadonMASleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescentsChild Dev1998698758879768476
  • AceboCSadehASeiferREstimating sleep patterns with activity monitoring in children and adolescents: how many nights are necessary for reliable measures?Sleep199922951039989370
  • ShortMAGradisarMLackLCWrightHRDohntHThe sleep patterns and well-being of Australian adolescentsJ Adolesc10192012 [Epub ahead of print.]
  • CainNGradisarMElectronic media use and sleep in school-age children and adolescents: a reviewSleep Med20101173574220673649
  • TaylorDJJenniOGAceboCCarskadonMASleep tendency during extended wakefulness: insights into adolescent sleep regulation and behaviorJ Sleep Res20051423924416120098
  • ShortMAGradisarMWrightHLackLCDohntHCarskadonMATime for bed: parent-set bedtimes associated with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescentsSleep20113479780021629368