969
Views
58
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Unihemispheric sleep and asymmetrical sleep: behavioral, neurophysiological, and functional perspectives

Pages 221-238 | Published online: 12 Jul 2016

Abstract

Sleep is a behavior characterized by a typical body posture, both eyes’ closure, raised sensory threshold, distinctive electrographic signs, and a marked decrease of motor activity. In addition, sleep is a periodically necessary behavior and therefore, in the majority of animals, it involves the whole brain and body. However, certain marine mammals and species of birds show a different sleep behavior, in which one cerebral hemisphere sleeps while the other is awake. In dolphins, eared seals, and manatees, unihemispheric sleep allows them to have the benefits of sleep, breathing, thermoregulation, and vigilance. In birds, antipredation vigilance is the main function of unihemispheric sleep, but in domestic chicks, it is also associated with brain lateralization or dominance in the control of behavior. Compared to bihemispheric sleep, unihemispheric sleep would mean a reduction of the time spent sleeping and of the associated recovery processes. However, the behavior and health of aquatic mammals and birds does not seem at all impaired by the reduction of sleep. The neural mechanisms of unihemispheric sleep are unknown, but assuming that the neural structures involved in sleep in cetaceans, seals, and birds are similar to those of terrestrial mammals, it is suggested that they involve the interaction of structures of the hypothalamus, basal forebrain, and brain stem. The neural mechanisms promoting wakefulness dominate one side of the brain, while those promoting sleep predominates the other side. For cetaceans, unihemispheric sleep is the only way to sleep, while in seals and birds, unihemispheric sleep events are intermingled with bihemispheric and rapid eye movement sleep events. Electroencephalogram hemispheric asymmetries are also reported during bihemispheric sleep, at awakening, and at sleep onset, as well as being associated with a use-dependent process (local sleep).

Video abstract

Point your SmartPhone at the code above. If you have a QR code reader the video abstract will appear. Or use:

http://youtu.be/K4SOQwb_0uY

View correction statement:
Unihemispheric sleep and asymmetrical sleep: behavioral, neurophysiological, and functional perspectives [Corrigendum]

Introduction

Sleep was earlier defined as a periodically necessary behavior, with a rhythm relatively independent from the environment and with an almost complete interruption of sensory and motor functions.Citation1 However, it is now known that motor signals reach motoneurons, although their output is reduced during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep and actively inhibited during REM sleep. Sensory messages impinge the thalamus and the cortex, but the increase in sensory thresholds makes the subject progressively unresponsive to external stimuli as sleep deepens. Sleep is characterized by a reduction of motor activity (except REM as well as facial and digital twitches), the search for a sensorily isolated and protected place, and the assumption of characteristic body postures.

Sleep can be studied by observing and recording the body signs of sleep behavior, but to detect its physiological and psychological organization, it is indispensable to record electrographic signs: electroencephalogram (EEG), movements of eyes (electrooculogram [EOG]), muscle tone (electromyogram [EMG]), electrocardiogram (ECG), breathing frequency, and body temperature. These parameters allow distinguishing wakefulness from sleep, NREM from REM sleep, and the transitions from wakefulness to sleep and sleep to awakening.

In mammals, the EEG wave pattern changes as follows: during wakefulness, it consists of low-amplitude high-frequency waves (beta rhythm, desynchronized EEG); at onset of NREM sleep, a theta rhythm predominates (stage 1); thereafter, the EEG becomes progressively more synchronized at subsequent stages, and at stage 4, it is dominated by high-voltage and low-frequency waves (delta waves, slow-wave sleep [SWS], synchronized EEG); concurrently, the muscle tone progressively declines. In the course of NREM, the EEG pattern changes to low voltage and fast waves (desynchronized EEG), but sleep behavior persists, the muscle tone is actively inhibited, and bursts of saccadic eye movements (REM) are recorded in the EOG. In humans, each cycle of an NREM and REM episode lasts 90–100 minutes. During NREM, blood pressure, heart and respiration rates, and body temperature decrease. During REM sleep, blood pressure, heart rate, and respiration rate increase and become irregular, and there is a general activation of brain and autonomic system, accompanied by male penile erection and female clitoral engorgement. Thermoregulation is mainly abolished during REM sleep; therefore, thermoregulatory mechanisms cannot be brought into function. Sleep is not associated with a reduction of brain activity; by contrast, neurons remain fully active during sleep although they show a different discharge pattern. Overall, the role of sleep on the brain and body functions is connected to the patterns of EEG waves, dreaming, the change of discharge pattern of neurons, the synthesis and degree of release of brain chemical substances, synaptic homeostasis, and others.

A function attributed to sleep is the restoration of brain energy metabolism, consisting in the replenishment of brain glycogen stores.Citation2 The stores of glycogen in astrocytes are progressively depleted during waking, associated with a progressive propensity to sleep. During NREM sleep, the glycogen stores are progressively replenished and brain energy metabolism is restored; the process is less efficient during REM sleep. The depletion of glycogen during waking causes the accumulation of adenosine, which has been proposed as a putative somnogen able to trigger sleep onset and EEG synchronization.Citation1,Citation3,Citation5

A pulsatile release of some hormones during sleep may be associated with body restoration and homeostasis.Citation3Citation6 Cortisol level reaches a nadir during nocturnal sleep but slowly increases during the second half of the sleep period, and its acme is reached just before and at onset of awakening. The growth hormone peaks during the first half of the sleep period. Prolactin secretion peaks at the middle of the sleep period and decreases when awakenings occur during sleep. Pituitary thyroid-stimulating hormone secretion increases during nocturnal sleep, while the secretion of triiodothyronine and thyroxine decreases. It has been indicated that hypothalamic growth hormone-releasing hormone (GHRH) promotes sleep, while corticotropin-releasing hormone favors awakening and wakefulness. Finally, sleep deprivation causes a decrease in body sensitivity to insulin, a decrease of leptin (inhibits hunger), and an increase of gherlin (stimulates hunger).

Sleep keeps up the efficiency of the immune system, thus favoring health and the probability of survival.Citation7Citation10 An infectious illness is accompanied by an intense somnolence or sleepiness probably because compelling to rest and more sleep favor the processes of healing. Insufficient or inadequate sleep increases the susceptibility to viral infections (cold, flu) or the ability to be affected by infection spread. The cytokines interleukin (IL)-1 and IL-6, as well as the trophic factor tumor necrosis factor-1, act on the hypothalamic thermostat, triggering fever and NREM-SWS.

Sleep and thermoregulation are closely linked.Citation11Citation15 Animals select an appropriate environment for sleeping that facilitates minimal thermal loss and maximal thermal conservation. The preoptic area of anterior hypothalamus (POAH) shares the mechanisms of sleep regulation and thermoregulation. Mild heating of POAH reduces sleep latency and promotes NREM sleep and SWS, while its cooling causes opposite effects. During NREM sleep, there is a decrease of body and brain temperatures, caused by a reduction of metabolic rate, an increase of heat loss, and a decrease of heat production all aimed at energy conservation.

Memory encoding and consolidation are functions attributed to sleep.Citation16Citation19 Overall, memory consolidation is impaired in sleep-deprived subjects compared with nondeprived ones. However, for declarative memory consolidation, data are controversial because impairment or nonimpairment would depend on specific aspects of the memory task, such as the presence of emotional or neutral components. Motor learning performance and visual perceptual learning are significantly improved after sleep, but they are not after both an equivalent wake period or after sleep deprivation. Overall, SWS and REM sleep before training are indispensable for initial memory encoding, while posttraining SWS and REM sleep are crucial for memory consolidation and the associated neural plasticity.

Synaptic potentiation and strengthening is the neural correlate between sleep and memory consolidation. It has been suggested that the brain would become oversaturated in a short period of time if all the waking-activated synapses are reinforced by subsequent sleep, thereby causing a reduction of the capacity to acquire learning and memories. Furthermore, not all neural traces left by the waking experience should be relevant and have to be consolidated during sleep. It has been suggestedCitation20,Citation21 that the synapses involved in new significant memories are selectively strengthened during sleep, while synapses activated by nonsignificant experiences are selectively weakened. Selective synaptic weakening restores brain circuitry to a certain baseline functional level, fully predisposing the brain toward new learning and experiences during subsequent wakefulness.

Unihemispheric sleep

Electrographic patterns, behaviors, neural mechanisms, and functions have been described taking into account the idea that sleep involves the whole brain and the body at the same time. Hemispheric asymmetries of EEG wave patterns have been found during bihemispheric sleep (BS), as well as a higher amount of sleep in specific cortical areas associated with specific sensory or motor experiences during wakefulness (local sleep). Closure of both the eyes, symmetric body muscular hypotonia or atonia, and REMs are associated with BS. Some animals show a different sleep behavior because sleep is induced in only one hemisphere, while the other hemisphere remains awake or it is awakened, termed as unihemispheric sleep. In some aquatic mammals, sleep and wakefulness periods alternate between the hemispheres, and it is the only way of sleeping. In other animals (seals and birds), unihemispheric sleep is a transitory sleep event intermingled with BS. Unihemispheric sleep is currently associated with asymmetric eye closure/opening and particular body sleep postures. Electrographically, unihemispheric sleep consists of an EEG composed of waves of low frequency and high amplitude (slow waves, synchronized EEG) recorded in one hemisphere (sleeping) and an EEG composed of a mixture of high- and low-amplitude waves (intermediate state) or of high-frequency and low-amplitude waves (desynchronized EEG) in the other hemisphere (awake).Citation22,Citation23 Unihemispheric sleep in cetaceans and pinnipeds is named as unihemispheric slow-wave sleep or USWS, while unihemispheric sleep in birds is named as unihemispheric-monocular sleep (Un-Mo sleep). SWS in both hemispheres is named bihemispheric SWS (BSWS).

Unihemispheric sleep in cetaceans

Sleep behavior was studied in some cetaceans, including bottlenose dolphins, common porpoise, the Amazonian dolphin, pilot whale, and beluga whale. In the pilot whaleCitation24,Citation25 (a type of cetacean), was recorded an asymmetrical EEG pattern composed of high-amplitude, slow–frequency waves in one hemisphere and simultaneous slow-amplitude high-frequency waves in the other hemisphere, defined as an EEG pattern of “quiet wakefulness” (). Dolphins spent almost two thirds of day time in wakefulness showing a typical bilateral wake desynchronized EEG pattern and an USWS pattern in the remaining daily third. Several studies have confirmed and studied the USWS in cetaceans.Citation22,Citation26Citation31 USWS alternated between the hemispheres; in one single session, USWS occurred more in one hemisphere, while in another session, it was more frequent in the other one, but in the long run, both hemispheres benefited from an equal amount of sleep.Citation22,Citation32 BSWS (delta range) was never observed in dolphins, porpoises, and whales for more than a few seconds.Citation33 The use of a telemetry method facilitated the understanding that for one brief period of time (duration not reported by RidgwayCitation29), sleep-like EEG activity appeared in both hemispheres in bottlenose dolphins.Citation29

Figure 1 EEG recorded from occipital–parietal derivations.

Notes: (A) EEG in a bottlenose dolphin during USWS. (B) EEG of left hemisphere. (C) EEG of right hemisphere. Reprinted from Brain Research, Vol 134, Mukhametov LM, Supin AY, Polyakova IG. Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep pattern in dolphins, Pages no 581–584, Copyright © (1977) with permission from Elsevier.
Abbreviations: EEG, electroencephalogram; USWS, unihemispheric slow-wave sleep.
Figure 1 EEG recorded from occipital–parietal derivations.

In cetaceans, the correlation between eye closure/opening and USWS has been an undefined issue perhaps because these are not always correlated and/or due to the difficulty of detecting eye state during USWS while the animal swims or rests in a particular body position. It has been reported that dolphins sleep with one eye closed and one eye open in order to monitor the environment.Citation34,Citation35 An asymmetry state during USWS in bottlenose and Amazon dolphins was confirmed,Citation28,Citation36 but it was pointed out that one eye could be either open or closed when the contralateral hemisphere was in SWS. A subsequent study revealed that the eye contralateral to the hemisphere showing SWS is usually closed in dolphins.Citation37 Subsequently, in beluga and bottlenose dolphins, a closer association between USWS and eye state asymmetry has been confirmedCitation38: the eye contralateral to the sleeping hemisphere was mostly closed or in an intermediate state, while the eye contralateral to the waking hemisphere was almost always open or in an intermediate state. However, the stimulation of the open eye during an episode of USWS awakened the animal when the contralateral hemisphere was either awake or asleep, indicating that the hemisphere/open eye is in control of behavior.Citation36,Citation39 Bilateral eye opening indicated waking, while bilateral eye closure was rare (<2% of observation time). White whale also showed USWS not strictly associated with eye state asymmetry.Citation40 Thus, cetaceans’ eye state does not accurately reflect the sleep state because there are occasions (<1 minute) when they could be independent.Citation40

In cetaceans showing USWS, no EEG signs of REM sleep have been reported.Citation22,Citation27,Citation28,Citation33,Citation159 However, behavioral signs such as penile erection, eyelid movements, and muscle twitching, which are currently observed during terrestrial mammalian REM sleep, have been occasionally observed in cetaceans.Citation27,Citation33,Citation39,Citation41Citation43 On the one hand, these behavioral features are not considered sufficient indicators of REM sleep in terrestrial mammals, and on the other hand, they also occur during wakefulness in cetaceans.Citation33 Existing data do not support the presence or absence of REM sleep in dolphins, but some authorsCitation33 suggest that it could be present either in a modified form and/or as a short-lasting event that is therefore difficult to be detected. Subcortical structures and probably the whole half of the brain are involved in USWS because thalamic nuclei display a low-frequency electrical activity correlated with the ipsilateral sleeping cortex.Citation44

Bottlenose, Amazonian, and Pacific white-sided dolphins slowly swim or hover on the water surface when they are in USWS,Citation26,Citation32 while during constant swimming, they reach the surface and breathe but maintain the USWS condition. Porpoises do not hover but swim continuously and emerge to breathe. In addition, bottlenose dolphins have also been observed to resting and sleep at the bottom of the pool or tank with one or both eyes open, interrupted by surfacing to breathe. Cetaceans need to surface regularly for breathingCitation52; therefore, the movements and reflexes needed for such behavior are incompatible with BSWS and REM sleep, which show reduced or abolished muscular activity. Therefore, sleep is triggered in one hemisphere, leaving the other awake and in control of motor behavior.Citation28,Citation33 When they swam in groups, it was observed that the open eye was directed toward the center of the group while the closed eye faced away.Citation45,Citation46 Therefore, they maintained visual contact with the group. In fact, when the dolphins changed position within the group, the open/closed eye pattern was inverted.Citation45,Citation46

Unihemispheric sleep in seals (Pinnipedia)

Seals sleep in water and on land. EEG and sleep behavior have been studied in Otariidae (eared seals), in Phocidae (true seals), and in Sirenia (manatee). Eared seals (northern fur seals) show BSWS, bihemispheric REM sleep, as well as typical USWS in water and on land ().Citation26,Citation33,Citation47,Citation48 The degree of EEG asymmetry was not equal across the dorsal cerebral cortex, but it was the greatest in the occipital–lateral and parietal derivations and the smallest in the frontal–medial ones.Citation50

Figure 2 Electrographic recordings of the Cape fur seal.

Notes: (A) Wakefulness; (B) REM sleep; (C) left USWS; (D) right USWS; (E) BSWS. Reprinted with permission from Neuroscience Letters, Vol 143, Lyamin OI, Chetyrbok IS, Unilateral EEG activation during sleep in the Cape fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillus. Pages 263–266, Copyright © (1992) with permission from Elsevier.
Abbreviations: BSWS, bihemispheric slow-wave sleep; ECG, electrocardiogram; EEG, electroencephalogram; EMG, electromyogram; EOG, electrooculogram; LFO, EEG from left fronto-occipital cortex; LFP, left frontoparietal cortex; REM, rapid eye movement; RFO, right fronto-occipital cortex; RFP, EEG from right frontoparietal cortex; USWS, unihemispheric slow-wave sleep.
Figure 2 Electrographic recordings of the Cape fur seal.

On land, BSWS predominates over USWS, but in water, the amount of USWS increases: USWS percentage on land is 41.6%, while in water, it is 66%.Citation49 Seals revert to more USWS when water conditions render BSWS less convenient or feasible because of the need to breathe while sleeping. For the same reason, the amount of REM sleep in water is lower than that on land.Citation51

In water, fur seals adopt a particular body posture during USWS, which allows them to sleep and breathe: they lie on one side keeping one of the front flippers in water paddling continuously, while the other three flippers are kept in air, probably for lowering heat lossCitation52,Citation53; the nostrils are kept out of water for breathing (). The hemisphere contralateral to the moving flipper tends to be awake (desynchronized EEG), thereby allowing the motor commands for paddling, while the other hemisphere is less desynchronized or in SWS. As soon as the seal falls into REM sleep, the flipper movement ceases and the seal’s head sinks.Citation51 USWS is accompanied by unilateral eye closure.

Figure 3 Posture that northern fur seal assumes during USWS in water.

Notes: Seal lying on its left side; the right hemisphere is awake while the left one is sleeping. Reprinted with the permission of Natus Medical, Inc., Middleton, USA.
Abbreviation: USWS, unihemispheric slow-wave sleep.
Figure 3 Posture that northern fur seal assumes during USWS in water.

USWS has been also recorded in Cape fur seals while they were on land, thereby not linked to the need for breathing. The hemisphere contralateral to the open eye was more awake (EEG desynchronized) than the other hemisphere connected to the closed eye.Citation47 EEG in these seals was not recorded during USWS in water. During an episode of USWS on land, these eared seals adopt the same body posture as they do when they have USWS in water, thereby not linking it to breathing.Citation47 In a species of sea lions, the Stellar, USWS was also associated with reaching the surface during sleep for breathing, but, similar to dolphins, USWS has been recorded while they were swimming.Citation54 Sleep has been also studied in three other species of true seals.Citation28,Citation54Citation58 In these seals, EEG recordings revealed that they had only BSWS and REM sleep and that USWS was totally absent both in water and on land. In water, they adopt two sleep positions: 1) float at the surface and raise the head for breathing without arousing; 2) in deep water, hold the breath while sleeping bihemispherically and periodically surfacing for breathing. On land, they sleep and breathe. In different families of seals, different solutions have been worked out for the same need to breathe and sleep while they are in water. Manatees (species of the order Sirenia) display exclusive aquatic swimming in shallow waters. USWS has not been recorded in Caribbean manatees although the presence of USWS cannot be excluded. Amazonian manatees showed USWS, BSWS, and REM sleep and they remained motionless at the bottom of the water during all stages of sleep.Citation59 Walrus sleeps both in water and on land.Citation158 On land, walrus showed BSWS (both eyes closed), paradoxical sleep, and brief episodes of interhemispheric EEG asymmetry or USWS. The eye contralateral to the hemisphere showing SWS was closed, while the eye contralateral to the hemisphere with low-voltage waves was open. In water, sleep occurred mainly while the walrus was lying at the bottom of the pool and it awoke when it emerged to the surface for breathing. The amount of asymmetrical SWS decreased in water.Citation158

Unihemispheric sleep in birds

Ball et alCitation60Citation62 reported that gulls slept in bouts alternating between bilateral and unilateral eye closure, which was associated with the presence of Un-Mo sleep. In a screening study, asymmetric eye closure was used for assessing the presence of Un-Mo sleepCitation61,Citation62 and unilateral eye closure was found in some species of birds but not in others. Szymczak et alCitation63 recorded episodes of Un-Mo sleep (EEG slow-wave asymmetry) in the European blackbird but failed to observe a concurrent asymmetric eye closure. An absence of EEG asymmetry and of asymmetric eye closure was reported in pigeons.Citation64 Thereafter, events of Un-Mo sleep associated with unilateral eye closure were clearly found in pigeons.Citation65 Unilateral eye closure during sleep was also observed in Cape white-eye passerine but not in other passerine birds such as malachite sunbird and fan-tailed widowbird.Citation66 Rattenborg et alCitation23 suggested that Un-Mo sleep should be actually absent in some species of birds, but it may be also that Un-Mo sleep has passed unnoticed because there are difficulties in detecting changes in the EEG wave pattern and eye asymmetry concurrently.Citation64 In mallard ducks, the presence of Un-Mo sleep and unilateral eye closure was unmistakably established.Citation67,Citation68 It was also indicatedCitation23 that the “awake” hemisphere seems not to be fully awake but in an intermediate state or in a sort of quiet wakefulness that did not impede the mallard duck being behaviorally awakened as soon as a visual stimulus was presented to the open eye.Citation23

An interesting issue concerns the sleep of migrating birds during nonstop long-lasting flights. The general belief is that birds should sleep in flight,Citation69 although it has not been technically feasible to record the EEG. Assuming that the motor physiology of migrating birds is not different from that of nonmigrating ones, both BSWS and REM sleep should be unsuitable for flying and sleeping.Citation69 Therefore, either they do not sleep at all, dispensing with a large proportion of sleep, or they have only Un-Mo sleep during flight. Rattenborg et alCitation70 studied sleep in the white-crowned sparrow in captivity during migratory and non-migratory seasons. During the migratory season, the birds had only one third of the sleep time they had during nonmigratory seasons. Another migrant, the Swainson’s thrush, in captivity had a substantial reduction of night-time sleep during the migration season. The loss of nocturnal sleep is compensated during the day by an increase of drowsiness, total sleep time, daytime micronaps, and episodes of unilateral eye closure/Un-Mo sleep.Citation71,Citation72

Un-Mo sleep has been found only in some species of birds.Citation63 I may hypothesize for diverse reasons: 1) It was not studied or/and it was not detected. 2) Perhaps some birds do not show Un-Mo sleep because vigilance would be performed in different ways, ie, some birds of the group would supervise and they would be fully awake while others would be allowed to sleep. 3) Some birds do not have or they do not need Un-Mo sleep and antipredation vigilance because they are predators, for example, eagle and owl.

Unihemispheric sleep in domestic chicks

Domestic chick (Gallus gallus) is an interesting animal model for studying behavior, sleep, and particularly unihemispheric sleep: 1) domestic chicks have a hemispheric lateralization in the control of behavior;Citation73Citation75 2) hemispheric dominance changes during posthatching development;Citation73 3) chicks can be subjected to performance of specific tasks in which the control of learning is exerted by one or the other hemisphere;Citation76,Citation77 4) it is possible to influence the embryonic development in ovo and, thereby, the hemispheric lateralization.Citation78Citation80

Domestic chicks show a sleep/wakefulness cycle similar to that of mammals.Citation81Citation83 Wakefulness is characterized by an EEG with low amplitude and fast waves; an intermediate phase or drowsiness, with EEG composed of slow and fast waves; BSWS with symmetric EEG with high amplitude and slow waves; and “hypnotic sleep” with symmetric EEG with low amplitude and fast waves (); BSWS is interrupted by Un-Mo sleep episodes composed of EEG of high-amplitude slow waves in one hemisphere and the contralateral eye shut and fast waves of low amplitude in the other hemisphere and contralateral eye open ().Citation62,Citation83Citation88 During a recording session, domestic chicks showed several Un-Mo sleep events intermingled with BSWS ones. In , a typical event of Un-Mo sleep of nearly 10 seconds’ duration is shown. Chicks spent ∼1.5%–2% of total time sleeping in Un-Mo sleep.

Figure 4 Electrographic recordings in the domestic chick during sleep.

Notes: (A) Wakefulness; (B) intermediate phase; (C) bihemispheric slow-wave sleep; (D) paradoxical sleep; (E) unihemispheric slow-wave sleep. BF indicates bill forward sleep position, with right eye closed and left eye open. Panel (E) has been reprinted from Behavioural Brain Research, Vol 134, Bobbo D, Galvani F, Mascetti GG, Vallortigara G., Light exposure of the chick embryo influences monocular sleep. Pages no 447–466. Copyright © 2002 with permission from Elsevier.
Abbreviations: EEG, electroencephalogram; EMG, electromyogram; LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere.
Figure 4 Electrographic recordings in the domestic chick during sleep.

The Un-Mo sleep pattern of chicks is associated with the posthatching development of brain lateralization.Citation89 During the first week (including day 8), behavior is dominated by the left hemisphere, while the right hemisphere dominates during the second week.Citation73,Citation76,Citation90,Citation91 During the first week, imprinted chicks showed significantly more left Un-Mo sleep (right eye closure),Citation89 which was associated with the fixation of memories of imprinting in the left hemisphere.Citation89,Citation92,Citation93 A clear bias toward more right-oriented Un-Mo sleep (left eye closure) was recorded during the second week post-hatching, associated with a wake dominance of the right hemisphere.Citation86,Citation89

Hemispheric dominance and Un-Mo sleep are also influenced by the egg’s incubation.Citation78,Citation88,Citation95 Chicks hatched from eggs light-incubated (last 3 days of incubation) showed a significant posthatching bias toward more right Un-Mo sleep (left eye closed), while in chicks coming from eggs dark-incubated, the bias was toward more left USWS (right eye closed).Citation88,Citation95 In the egg, the right eye lies adjacent to the shell, and it is stimulated by penetrating light, while the left eye is positioned next to the dense mass of the body and not stimulated by light.Citation94 It was suggestedCitation88,Citation95 that light stimulated the developmental processes of the left hemisphere earlier than those in the right, causing both left hemisphere dominance and more left Un-Mo sleep (right eye closed); instead, the absence of light stimulation favors right hemisphere development processes. In other terms, right hemisphere dominance would be prewired in the embryo in the absence of any external modulator effect.Citation95

It is known that the left hemisphere dominates in tasks in which the selection of cues allows stimuli to be classified into categories, while the right hemisphere dominates in spatial analysis and in responses to novelty.Citation75,Citation77,Citation96Citation98 Chicks trained binocularly in a color discrimination task showed subsequently more left Un-Mo sleep (right eye closure), associated with a prevalent engagement of the left hemisphere in learning the task. Another group of chicks subjected to a spatial task showed subsequently more right Un-Mo sleep (left eye closure) related to a prevalent engagement of the right hemisphere in the task.Citation99Citation101

The functions of unihemispheric sleep

In cetaceans living in an aquatic environment, USWS provides the benefits of both sleep and survival. Concurrently, the open eye and the awake hemisphere allow cetaceans to monitor the environment for predators, particularly when they swim slowly or hover on the water surface. USWS allows them to surface regularly for breathing while sleeping.Citation26,Citation31,Citation49 With the open eye and awake hemisphere, cetaceans are able to control the conspecifics to maintain group coherence while swimming in groups.Citation45,Citation46 The awake hemisphere ensures efficient motor control over the tail and flippers for swimming or hovering. Living in water, cetaceans face a thermally challenging environment of high heat loss, and USWS allows them to ensure effective thermogenesis continually from muscular tone and activity, mainly from flipper and tail movements for the forward propulsion. In this context, USWS should be an adaptation to the thermal pressure in water. Pillay and MangerCitation102 reported that during USWS, the potential heat loss is compensated by the maintenance of muscle activity and tone.Citation103 In fact, the brain temperature in the awake hemisphere remained constant, while it has been found to be slightly lower in the sleeping one, in agreement with the thermoregulatory role of SWS.Citation11,Citation104,Citation105 Two additional features reinforce the role of USWS in maintaining effective thermoregulation. 1) The absence of REM sleep. It is known that REM is accompanied by muscular atonia and inefficient thermoregulation. 2) USWS meets the requirements of the homeostatic regulation of sleep. USWS deprivation causes an increase of the pressure to sleep in the deprived hemisphere and a SWS rebound only in the deprived hemisphere.Citation106

The function of USWS in eared seals is slightly different in water and on land. In water, during USWS, seals adopt a special body position at the surface, which allows them to breathe, to paddle with one front flipper, and keep the other three flippers in air probably to reduce the heat loss (thermoregulation). In addition, the open eye and contralateral awake hemisphere allow motor output to the paddling flipper, as well as controlling the surroundings and the approach of predators (sharks, killer whales). In the Steller sea lions, USWS has been recorded while they swim, allowing movement to the surface for breathing and thermoregulation.Citation47 On land, the USWS is not linked to breathing,Citation47 thermoregulation, and motor control but to a sentinel behavior. The open eye connected to the contralateral awake hemisphere looks away, enabling the control of the environment, coordination with the conspecifics, and detection of predators. Manatees remain motionless at the bottom during all stages of sleep; consequently, USWS may not be connected to the need for breathing, but it is perhaps only used to detect predators. In walruses,Citation158 the episodes of USWS and brief opening of one eye would facilitate monitoring of the environment to detect predators and aid in their high level of social activity.Citation158 In birds, Un-Mo sleep allows sleep as well as permitting an intermittent necessity for the control of the environment and antipredation. In European blackbirds and domestic chicks, episodes of Un-Mo sleep occur when the bird’s bill is pointing forward and parallel to the horizontal plane (BF or bill forward sleep position),Citation63,Citation89 which is fully adequate to visually observe the surrounding environment (). In Swainson’s thrush, unilateral eye opening during daytime sleep was associated with predator detection and control of environment while recovery processes proceeded.Citation71,Citation72 Rattenborg et alCitation67,Citation68 studied the amount of USWS associated with the amount of risk of predation in a group of mallard ducks arranged in a row. They found that ducks at both ends of the row showed 150% higher amount of Un-Mo sleep and associated eye opening than ducks located in central positions. The opened eye of these “sentinel ducks” was directed away from the group, enabling them to detect the approach of a predator. In addition, they quickly awakened (escape behavior) as soon as a threatening visual stimulus was applied to the open eye during an episode of Un-Mo sleep.Citation69 Consequently, BSWS time is reduced and the amount of Un-Mo sleep is increased when the risk of predation is high; meanwhile, when that risk is absent or unlikely, BSWS sleep is favored over Un-Mo sleep.Citation23 If migrating birds sleep during flight, Un-Mo sleep is the only way that allows efficient muscular activity for recovery and to keep flying. On the contrary, migrating birds drastically reduce sleep during flight, therefore, recovery sleep and Un-Mo sleep episodes were allowed when birds landed to perch and rest on land.Citation71,Citation72

Un-Mo sleep in domestic chicks provides sleep and recovery in the dominant hemisphere and, at the same time, it allows control of the environment (hen or imprinting object) and a probable antipredatory function. An object or a hand moved over the sleeping cage during an episode of Un-Mo sleep caused waking, an immediate startle response, and distress calls. Un-Mo sleep events are short-lasting; in , the event lasts ∼10 seconds, but chicks show several episodes of unihemispheric sleep during a recording session covering ∼1.5%–2% of sleep time. The question is: why should the events of Un-Mo sleep be less relevant because they have a short duration or they cover a reduced percentage of sleep time? Several important behavioral patterns occur with reduced duration, but they are of marked biological relevance (eg, antipredatory response, aggression, and copulation). Duration of 10 seconds for each event and for several events should be a sufficient amount of time for an efficacious control of environment and vigilance behavior.

Furthermore, the Un-Mo sleep pattern was unquestionably connected with posthatching standing bias of hemispheric dominanceCitation73,Citation89 and the dominance in the control of learning behavior. Un-Mo sleep prevailed in the hemisphere that dominated during wakefulness, favoring a recovery process and consolidation of memory.Citation99Citation101,Citation107Citation109 Because Un-Mo sleep episodes were intermingled with BSWS ones, the dominant hemisphere spent relatively more time sleeping (BSWS plus Un-Mo sleep) than the nondominant one. Concurrently, for the control of the environment, hen and antipredation chicks awoke the nondominant hemisphere and opened the contralateral eye.

Unihemispheric and bihemispheric sleep

A comparison between USWS and BSWS is important. The first issue stresses that in animals showing only USWS or in those having only episodes of USWS or of Un-Mo sleep, the time spent sleeping is unquestionably lower than that of animals having only BSWS and REM. At first, it seems reasonable to assume that USWS or Un-Mo sleep would be a less efficient way for accomplishing the benefits of sleep for brain and body functions. Cetaceans having only USWS, the amount of which is almost equally shared between both the hemispheres, have approximately half of the sleeping time or, conversely, they are nearly half sleep deprived. Although the awake hemisphere could be considered to be in a state of relaxed waking,Citation24,Citation25 the functions of sleep cannot be accomplished during relaxed wakefulness. USWS and the reduction of sleep was probably worked out when in the past, cetaceans returned to water and faced the necessity to breathe, swim/hover, and thermoregulate in a medium favoring heat loss. Consequently, they adopted SWS in one hemisphere at the time and suppressed REM sleep in order to meet the vital needs and, at the same time, to ensure an appropriate brain-and-body recovery process. At any rate, the functions attributed to sleep and behavior seem to be accomplished in dolphins because in captivity, they show good healthCitation27,Citation37 and they can be trained to learn and memorize very complex tasks.

In seals and birds, USWS episodes are intermingled with BSWS and REM sleep episodes; therefore, a certain reduction of the total time spent sleeping was also evident. The reduction of sleep is higher in the hemisphere that is more often awakened with respect to the other hemisphere that benefits with more sleep. In seals, the reduction of sleep was higher in water than on land because the amount of USWS in water was 66% of sleep time, while on land, it was 41.6%.Citation26,Citation49 Perhaps, the need for vigilance was more pressing in water; therefore, BSWS and REM sleep were curtailed and USWS was increased.

Birds also showed a reduction of the total time spent sleeping, which was more in one hemisphere than in the other, in favor of vigilance. The amount of vigilance required for risk of predation in animals decreases in parallel with the increase in group size and the position inside the group; edge animals spent approximately twice as much time in vigilance behavior than animals placed centrally.Citation110,Citation111 Rattenborg et alCitation23,Citation68 reported that mallard ducks reduced sleep in favor of increasing the likelihood of survival. Probably, in the wild, the role of “sentinel” mallard ducks would be assumed, day after day, by different birds; therefore, the reduction of sleep time in one performance can be compensated in the following sleep sessions.

In domestic chicks, the Un-Mo sleep pattern seems to be mainly related to hemispheric dominance.Citation89 On the one hand, the reduction of sleep was lower in the dominant hemisphere, allowing for recovery process and memory consolidation.Citation89,Citation99 On the other hand, the nondominant hemisphere showed a higher reduction of the time spent sleeping and had a greater role in the control of environment and vigilance.

The relationship between BSWS and Un-Mo sleep was studied in domestic chicks during recovery of sleep after a moderate sleep deprivation (8 hours).Citation112 During recovery, the total time spent sleeping and time spent in BSWS were both higher in deprived chicks than in nondeprived control ones, in accordance with previous studies in rats.Citation113Citation115 Moreover, the time spent in Un-Mo sleep was equal between deprived and nondeprived chicks, suggesting that even when sleep is impeded by a moderate extension of wakefulness, chicks do not give up to Un-Mo sleep, alertness, and vigilance. Instead, when sleep deprivation is longer and the propensity to sleep is higher, a reduction of Un-Mo sleep in favor of BSWS is preferred and, at the same time, a reduction of alertness and vigilance is also found.Citation116

Overall, although USWS curtails a certain amount of sleep, it has not been reported that the behavior and health of cetaceans, birds, and seals would be affected. It may be assumed that these animals benefit by the restoration of brain energy metabolism, efficiency of immune system, thermoregulation, and brain plasticity. Perhaps seals recover sleep when they leave water and rest on land. Migrating birds recover sleep during the daily rest on land, showing USWS episodes for antipredation vigilance, but their body efficiency is not affected. In fact, they can resume an efficient and long-lasting flight the next night.

Unihemispheric sleep and neural and neurochemical mechanisms of sleep

The neural mechanisms of USWS are unknown, and they could only be suggested on the basis of what is known about the sleep/waking cycle in terrestrial mammals and by assuming that both the neural structures and mechanisms involved are almost the same. Cetaceans were terrestrial mammals before becoming aquatic ones; therefore, it is presumed that they preserved the basic sleep/wake neural structures and mechanisms.

Asymmetric eye closure/opening is associated with unihemispheric sleep. An initial issue to clear up is whether unihemispheric sleep is the output of the activity of brain neurochemical circuits or it is only the consequence of the unilateral motor signals that trigger the eye opening and visual input causing the awakening of the contralateral hemisphere. In gulls, it has been insinuatedCitation23,Citation62 that adverse environmental conditions (ie, the direction of the sun and the wind) could determine the eye to be kept closed and the hemisphere to be asleep. However, the transition from BSWS to USWS in birds may occur in the absence of visual stimuli.Citation117 USWS has been recorded in unilaterally and bilaterally blinded chickens.Citation85,Citation118 USWS has been recorded in gulls kept in complete darkness.Citation62 Furthermore, eye opening during an episode of USWS in dolphins is mainly, but not always, correlated with the awakening of the contralateral hemisphere. RidgwayCitation29 reported that the EEG of dolphins remained desynchronized and symmetrical even when one eye was occluded. In birds with lateral eyes (domestic chick and pigeons), the optic fibers cross completely at the optic chiasm, but part of them cross the midline through the subcortical commissures.Citation119 Therefore, each hemisphere receives binocular visual projections, which is an anatomical–functional feature incompatible with sensory awaking in one hemisphere and sleeping in the other one. Therefore, the assumption that USWS is triggered by a unilateral visual input could be ruled out, although it is unquestionable that the visual input during an USWS episode would have a role in maintaining the contralateral hemisphere in the awakened state, allowing efficient control of the environment. Therefore, USWS may be brought about by endogenous brain mechanisms permitting both adequate brain-restoring processes and adaptative survival behavior.

The neural mechanisms of the waking–sleep cycle are based on the interaction among structures of the brain stem, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain. Sleep-promoting neurons are the gamma-aminobutyric acid-secreting (GABAergic) neurons of the basal forebrain, anterior hypothalamus (ventrolateral preoptic nucleus [VLPO]), midbrain, and pons. Arousal and waking maintenance are promoted by the activity of monoaminergic (MA-ergic) and cholinergic nuclei of posterior hypothalamus, basal forebrain, locus coeruleus, and raphe nuclei. Orexinergic neurons of the middle hypothalamus have the role of sustaining and regulating the activity of waking neural groups.Citation120,Citation121 In addition, the low frequency of discharge of noradrenergic and serotonergic neurons during NREM sleep would favor the decrease of muscular tone. Fuller et alCitation121 proposed a flip/flop model for the sleep/waking cycle, in which waking is maintained by the high activity of wake-promoting neural groups that activate the cortex and subcortical structures and, at the same time, inhibit sleep-promoting groups. Instead, sleep is triggered when sleep-promoting neurons actively inhibit arousal- and wakefulness-promoting ones.

It may be assumed that the same brain neurochemical groups regulate the sleep/wake cycle of cetaceans, but the mechanisms triggering and maintaining sleep would prevail on one side of the brain, while the mechanisms causing arousal and maintaining waking would prevail on the other side. In other words, there would be a sort of interhemispheric flip/flop mechanism shifting from one side of the brain to the other. Kedziora et al,Citation122 using the model of mammalian arousal ascending system, constructed a mathematical model of USWS. The model included the mutual interaction between waking-promoting MA-ergic, cholinergic, and orexinergic nuclei and the sleep-promoting VLPOs of the anterior hypothalamus regulated by circadian and homeostatic drives. The central structure of the model is the VLPO nucleus and its interhemispheric and intrahemispheric inhibitory connections ().

Figure 5 Model of unihemispheric sleep.

Notes: In the shaded and clear areas are shown the structures of the two sides of the brain. Numbers 1 and 2 indicate the structures and circuits of the two sides of the brain, respectively. Solid arrows indicate excitatory inputs; inhibitory connections are indicated by open arrows. Reprinted from Journal of Theoretical Biology, Vol 314, Kedziora DJ, Abeysuriya RG, Phillips AJK, Robinson PA. Physiologically based quantitative modeling of unihemispheric sleep. Pages no 109–119. Copyright © (2012) with permission from Elsevier.
Abbreviations: ACh/Orx, acetylcholine/orexin inputs to MA-ergic; Cip and Cco, hypothesized ipsilateral and contralateral connections of VLPO, respectively; H and C, homeostatic and circadian drives, respectively; MA-ergic, monoaminergic nuclei of brain stem and posterior hypothalamus; VLPO, ventrolateral preoptic area.
Figure 5 Model of unihemispheric sleep.

The main outcome of the model is that USWS would be promoted by interhemispheric or contralateral inhibitory connections between the VLPO nuclei of both sides, while BSWS would be promoted by VLPO intrahemispheric or ipsilateral inhibitory connections to MA-ergic nuclei. The frequency of alternating USWS episodes would be determined by sleep homeostasis and the time constant, ie, the circadian regulation. Both would promote sleep in one hemisphere and waking in the other one.

This model applies for cetaceans’ sleep, while for seals and birds, it suggests that the alternation of BSWS, REM sleep, and USWS would depend on the strength of the contralateral inhibitory VLPO connections; when they are weak, BSWS is promoted, whereas when they become temporarily strong, an USWS episode would be produced.

Sleep-promoting neurons of the VLPO of anterior hypothalamus are also thermosensitive. An increase or a decrease of brain temperature causes an increase or a decrease of the firing rate of these neurons, respectively. Mild heating of this area reduces sleep latency, promotes NREM sleep, and SWS, while its cooling causes opposite effects.Citation33,Citation123 In cetaceans, another mechanism is brought into play.Citation33 If the awake hemisphere’s brain temperature becomes slightly warmerCitation33 because it is awake,Citation11 the firing rate of the VLPO neurons increases and SWS is promoted, whereas if brain temperature in the sleeping hemisphere declines, the firing rate of the thermosensitive neurons also declines, favoring arousal and waking. Data partly support this hypothesis because during USWS of dolphins, the brain temperature was effectively found to be lower in the sleeping hemisphere, but the temperature in the awake hemisphere remained constant.Citation33,Citation104,Citation105 The discharge from the locus coeruleus of dolphins was found to be higher in the awake side and lower in the sleeping one.Citation124 Continuous discharge of noradrenergic neurons and the secretion of noradrenaline would stimulate glial metabolic rate and heat production. During USWS, the noradrenergic discharge/metabolic rate is asymmetric, being higher in the awake hemisphere and lower in the sleeping one. It has been suggested that these metabolic/brain temperature hemispheric mismatchesCitation124 play a role in causing the interhemispheric alternation of SWS and wakefulness. In addition, continuous firing by the locus coeruleus as well as raphe nuclei during USWS would allow the maintenance of muscle tone in dolphins for emerging, swimming, and breathing. Finally, the absence of REM sleep seems fully justified because neuronal firing from the locus coeruleus and raphe nuclei ceases completely during REM sleep in terrestrial mammals.

It is more difficult to speculate about the neural mechanism of USWS in seals and birds. Assuming that they possess sleep neural structures similar to those of terrestrial mammals, when an USWS episode is going to be triggered interrupting BSWS, sleep-supporting mechanisms would be briefly inhibited in one hemisphere, causing its awakening/contralateral eye opening, while sleep mechanisms remain dominant in the other hemisphere. The mathematical modelCitation122 points out that an USWS episode would occur when contralateral inhibitory VLPO connections become temporarily strong. When they subsequently become weak, the ipsilateral VLPO inhibitory connections would predominate and BSWS would resume. An important point is the monoamine release during unihemispheric sleep of fur seals. Serotonin, norepinephrine, and histamine release is not lateralized during USWS in fur seals.Citation132,Citation160 However, acetylcholine release is lateralized during USWSCitation131 of fur seals, being greater in the hemisphere showing low-voltage EEG pattern (quiet waking).Citation131 Perhaps, the asymmetrical release of acetycholine plays a role in the temporary unihemispheric awakening in fur seals.

The sleep/waking mechanisms of the two sides of the brain yield a certain degree of autonomy, higher in cetaceans and lower in seals and birds. However, the coherency of sleep/waking cycles between the two sides would be ruled by an across-the-midline exchange of neural messages. In dolphins, the interhemispheric messages would impede BSWS and allow proper duration of sleep in one hemisphere before being switched to wakefulness. MukhametovCitation22,Citation32 reported that the number of episodes of USWS in dolphins is almost equally distributed between both sides of the brain if they were quantified after a certain number of consecutive sleep sessions. However, in each single session, the number of USWS episodes was higher on one side than on the other. Probably, the propensity to sleep would not be always equal between the hemispheres and if propensity is higher in one of them, the neural circuits promoting sleep would be kept active longer. In dolphins swimming in groups, when sleep propensity nadir is attained in one hemisphere after it is awakened, the other hemisphere goes to sleep and the dolphin changes position in the group, maintaining visual contact and group coherency.

The commissures are the neural structures mediating the midline crossing of neural signals. The corpus callosum should be the first attribution, but it is unusually small in cetaceansCitation33,Citation125 and it is almost nonexistent or nonfunctional in birds.Citation126 On the one hand, in split-brain cats and humans as well as in humans with callosal agenesis, sleep maintains its bihemispheric pattern.Citation127Citation129 On the other hand, sleep neural mechanisms are located mainly in the brainstem and hypothalamus, so that the subcortical commissures may have the key role of mediating the midline crossing of signals. Michel and Roffwarg,Citation130 after a complete longitudinal section of the brain stem of the cat, observed alternating and asynchronous appearances of SWS in the two hemispheres. Mammals having USWS showed a larger posterior commissure compared with those lacking this kind of sleep.Citation23 In cetaceans, the posterior commissure is extremely large and the EEG coherence in split-brain cats is disrupted after the additional sectioning of posterior commissure.Citation33,Citation127 In cetaceans, the posterior commissure contains more midline-crossed noradrenergic fibers arising from locus coeruleus.Citation23 The activity of fibers coming from the locus coeruleus of one side could maintain the cortical activation and waking on the opposite side. In addition, acetylcholine release in fur seals is also lateralized during USWS with a greater release in hemisphere showing desynchronized waking EEG.Citation131 However, the main questions are the following: how can locus coeruleus (cholinergic nuclei of one side in fur seals)Citation131 promote waking in the contralateral hemisphere and, at the same time, those fibers arising from the other side be ineffective in causing waking in the other hemisphere? Are they inhibited by sleep-promoting neurons such as contralateral VLPO ones? Perhaps the brain structures involved in the sleep/waking cycle interact differently in cetaceans, seals, and birds than in animals having only BSWS and REM sleep.

It has been widely suggested that the shift from wakefulness to sleep could be triggered by the progressive accumulation of putative endogenous somnogens during wakefulness. During sleep, their concentration declines along with the reduction of sleep propensity. Adenosine released by basal forebrainCitation133Citation135 would bilaterally activate GABAergic VLPO sleep-promoting neurons and other nuclei connected to VLPO.Citation136 Other chemicals such as melatonin, IL-1, GHRH, and others have also been proposed as putative bilaterally acting somnogens. Probably, these chemicals are also present in the brain of dolphins, seals, and birds; therefore, somnogens’ involvement in USWS and Un-Mo sleep cannot be excluded. However, their effect in cetaceans would be unilaterally transient and shifting sides. The action of somnogens on BSWS and USWS pattern of seals and birds would be bilateral but it would be unilaterally blocked, causing a transient ipsilateral hemispheric arousal. After a short period of time, the block would be removed and BSWS resumed.

Hemispheric asymmetries in bihemispheric sleep: local sleep

In humans, the asymmetries in the EEG pattern during sleep have been associated with different degrees of hemispheric involvement in sleep. Overall, different functions, physiology, and mentation attributed to REM and to NREM sleep would be connected to hemispheric functional lateralization.Citation137 This issue is quite controversial because some studies have found certain relationships such as the right hemisphere being mainly involved in REM sleep and the left hemisphere activation during NREM sleep; other studies were unable to detect any hemispheric dominance during sleep. Armitage et alCitation138 described EEG asymmetry in stage 2 of NREM and SWS, while the EEG pattern was symmetrical in REM sleep. An alpha-rhythm EEG asymmetry during REM sleep in frontal and temporal derivations has been reported,Citation139 probably related to emotional reactivity during dreaming. Sekimoto et alCitation140 reported a higher number of delta waves in the right hemisphere frontal and central derivations than in the left one, while no asymmetry was found in the other cortical derivations. This asymmetry was attributed to a higher activation of the right hemisphere during wakefulness. The shift of EEG hemispheric asymmetry in the anterior/posterior axis in right-handed humans was explored.Citation141 Left hemisphere predominance in the frequency of sleep spindles was found along the anterior posterior axis. In central–parietal derivations, there would be right hemisphere dominance in the delta/theta band during NREM sleep and a left hemisphere dominance in the delta/theta band during REM sleep.

The behavioral awakening occurred more frequently from NREM sleep first in the left hemisphere or first in the right hemisphere, but this hemispheric asymmetry was not found at awakening from REM sleep.Citation142 Bertini et al,Citation143 using a tactile recognition test at awakening, reported a left-hand/right hemisphere superiority in the performance at awakening from REM sleep, while no hand/hemisphere difference was observed at awakening from NREM sleep. Subjects tested in a cognitive task after awakening from NREM (stage 2) and from REM sleep showed right hemisphere superiority in the performances after awakening from REM sleep and left hemisphere superiority after awakening from NREM sleep.Citation144 Casagrande et alCitation145 showed that left hemisphere goes to sleep earlier than the right one. As far as sleep onset is concerned, it was found that right-handed subjects show shorter latencies in the left hemisphere than in the right one, which could be associated with left hemisphere dominance.Citation146 In fact, left-handed subjects do not show any significant sleep onset asymmetry.

In rats, at the onset of light period, when the sleep pressure is high, there was a left hemisphere predominance of low-frequency power (parietal derivations) in SWS and there was a shift to right hemisphere predominance when sleep pressure faded out.Citation147 After a period of sleep deprivation, the left hemisphere predominance was enhanced during sleep recovery. During REM sleep, there was right hemisphere dominance in the power of the theta band. Recently, the EEG recorded in rats over a period of 48 hours reported significant light/dark changes in hemisphere dominance in delta power during NREM and in the theta power during REM.Citation148

Hemispheric asymmetry has been also associated with a use-dependent process. In humans, Cajohem et alCitation149 found that a 2-hour light stimulation on the left visual hemifield caused an attenuation of the waking alfa EEG activity and a decrease of EEG delta activity during subsequent sleep in the right visual cortex, but no effect was recorded in the left visual cortex after right visual field stimulation.Citation149 However, total sleep and sleep efficiency did not change while comparing experimental and control subjects. The application of aversive stimuli during sleep caused higher changes of frontal alpha activity in left hemisphere than in the right one both in stage 2 of NREM and in REM sleep.Citation150 Kettler et alCitation151 showed that a right-hand vibration stimulation in right-handed subjects before sleep caused subsequently a higher delta band power in the left hemisphere than in the right one. By contrast, no changes after left-hand stimulation were reported. They proposed that the effect is caused by metabolic activation in the contralateral left somatosensory cortex in response to the task but they also attributed a key role to the standing left hemisphere dominance. However, Cottone et alCitation152 reported that vibratory stimulation on a given hand did not disrupt SWS delta band pattern in the contralateral hemisphere, suggesting that human brain can monitor the environment without compromising sleep wave pattern.

In rats, unilateral vibrissae stimulation and the handedness in a task caused an asymmetrical EEG pattern with higher power in the hemisphere contralateral to the side of tactile stimulation and of the preferred paw.Citation153,Citation154 This use-dependent pattern is called local or regional sleep, meaning that when one part of the brain is highly stimulated during wakefulness that part would subsequently have more sleep than other parts less or not at all activated. Huber et alCitation155 reported a significant increase of SWS intensity in the right parietal region after subjects were subjected to a task of motor rotation adaptation compared to controls submitted to a no-rotation task but under kinematically identical conditions. Subsequently, subjects subjected to an arm immobilization showed a decrease of the somatosensory- and motor-evoked potentials over the contralateral somatosensory cortex and during sleep, SWS was also significantly reducedCitation156 over the same cortical area. In domestic chicks, the USWS should be considered a kind of local sleep.Citation100,Citation101 In pigeons, the monocular visual stimulation during BS deprivation elicited an increase of SWS and the slope of slow waves only in the hyperpallium connected with the stimulated eye (visually processing region) during recovery but not in the nonvisual mesopallium.Citation157 It is suggested that local SWS increase is correlated with local synaptic plastic changes associated with the learning task and with improvement in the task performance after sleep. On the other hand, the reduction of SWS would be indicative of local synaptic depression.Citation156 When synaptic strength is reduced, local sleep is concurrently reduced. Therefore, cortical plasticity could be associated with local sleep regulation.

Conclusion

Sleep and wakefulness are opposite brain and body conditions that accomplish different but complementary functions. Sleep is a vulnerable state of almost-complete disconnection from environmental events aimed at a mandatory brain and body recovery, while wakefulness is characterized by intense and necessary interaction with the environment aimed at survival. In some animals, this conflict has been mitigated by adopting a behavioral strategy in which they attempt to survive and recover at the same time. In other terms, they sleep with half of the brain while remaining awake with the other half. For cetaceans, USWS and the removal of BSWS/REM sleep were the crucial solutions that they adopted when they decided to return to the aquatic environment. It is difficult to know what kind of sleep–waking behavior cetaceans had when they lived on land; did they show BWSW and REM as other terrestrial mammals? Or did they also show USWS events similar to seals? In the first case, adaptation to aquatic conditions would have been a hard endeavor and the influence of natural selection would have been strong. In the second case, the adaptation would have been easier, perhaps just lengthening of and making USWS exclusive. USWS for some species of birds and seals is a transient sleeping behavior, together with BSWS and REM, aimed at controlling the environment and avoiding predation. In domestic chicks and mallard ducks, Un-Mo sleep probably is a legacy from when they lived in the wild. Probably, USWS is present in wild or semiwild birds. In the novel “Le chateau des songes” (The Castle of Dreams), Michel Jouvet (1993) transcribed a diary of a 17-century French physician, naturalist, and lord of a castle called Hugues la Scieve, in which some observations about sleep of birds were reported: “[…]the birds with the neck bent and head hidden under wing, sometimes lying down or on only one leg […] we were surprised to note that swans and geese slept with only one eye”. Finally, I do not resist the temptation of expressing a sort of envy for those animals that have the capacity to disconnect their brain from the environment and send to rest one part of it. Often, this desire arose when I was forced to attend boring social events, conferences, or scientific lectures.

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Mr Francesco Battisti for both technical assistance and advice in the editing of the Video Abstract.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • PieronHLe Probléme Physiologique du SomneilParisMasson1913
  • BenningtonJHHellerHCRestoration of brain energy metabolism as a function of sleep (1995)Prog Neurobiol1995453473607624482
  • LeweyAJWherTAGoodwinFKNewsomeDMarleySPLight suppresses melatonin secretion in humansScience1980210126712697434030
  • SassinJFParkerDCMaceJWGotlinRWJohnsonLCRossmanLGHuman growth hormone release: relation to slow wave sleep and sleep-waking cycleScience19691655135154307378
  • SpiegelKLeproultRVan CauterEImpact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine functionsLancet19993541435143910543671
  • MullingtonJMEndocrine function during sleep and sleep deprivationStickgoldRWalkerMThe Neuroscience of SleepPhiladelphia, PAElsevier Inc2009209212
  • MullingtonJMImmune function during sleep and sleep deprivationStickgoldRWalkerMThe Neuroscience of SleepPhiladelphia, PAElsevier Inc2009213217
  • ObalFJKruegerJMBiochemical regulation of sleepFront Biosci20038520550
  • TothLATolleyEAKruegerJMSleep as a prognostic indicator during infectious disease in rabbitsProc Soc Exp Biol Med19932031791928502660
  • RenegarKBFloydRAKruegerJMEffects of short-term sleep deprivation on murine immunity to influenza virus in young adult and senescent miceSleep1998212412489595602
  • McGintyDSzymusiakRKeeping cool: a hypothesis about the mechanisms and functions of slow-wave sleepTrends Neurosci1990134804871703678
  • GlotzbachSFHellerHCCentral nervous regulation of body temperature during sleepScience1976194537539973138
  • ParmeggianiPLRabiniCSleep and environmental temperatureArch Ital Biol19701083693875493491
  • BachVTelliezFLibertJ-PInteraction between sleep and thermo regulation in adults and neonatesSleep Med Rev2002648149212505480
  • SzymusiakRThermoregulation during sleep and sleep deprivationStickgoldRWalkerMThe Neuroscience of SleepPhiladelphia, PAElsevier Inc2009218222
  • WalkerMPSleep dependent memory processingStickgoldRWalkerMPThe Neuroscience of SleepPhiladelphia, PAElsevier Inc2009230240
  • WalkerMPStickgoldRSleep, memory and plasticityAnn Rev Psychol20061013916616318592
  • WalkerMPStickgoldRSleep dependent learning and memory consolidationNeuron20044412113315450165
  • StickgoldRWalkerMPMemory consolidation and reconsolidation: what is the role of sleep?Trends Neurosci20052840841515979164
  • TononiGCirelliCSleep and synaptic homestasis: a hypothesisBrain Res Bull200367214315014638388
  • TononiGCirelliCSleep function and synaptic homeostasisSleep Med Rev200610496216376591
  • MukhametovLMSupinAYPolyakovaIGInterhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep pattern in dolphinsBrain Res1977134581584902119
  • RattenborgNCAmlanerCJLimaSLBehavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perpectives of unihemispheric sleepNeurosci Biobehav Rev20002481784211118608
  • ShurleyJTSerafetinidesEABrooksREElsnerRKenneyDWSleep in Cetaceans: I. The pilot whale, Globicephala scammoniPsychophysiology19696230
  • SerafetinidesEAShurleyJTBrooksREElectroencephalogram of the pilot whale (Globocephala scammoni), in wakefulness and sleep: lateralization aspectsInt J Psychobiol19722129135
  • MukhametovLMUnihemispheric slow wave sleep in the brain of dolphins and sealsInoueSBorbelyAAEndogenous Sleep Subtances and Sleep RegulationNew York, NYJapan Societies Press19856775
  • MukhametovLMOleksenkoAIPoliakovaIGQuantitative characteristics of the electrocorticographic sleep stages in bottle-nosed dolphinNeurofisioloziya198820532538 [Article in Russian] [Abstract]
  • MukhametovLMSleep in marine mammalsExp Brain Res19848227238
  • RidgwaySHAsymmetry and symmetry in brain waves from dolphin right left and right hemispheres: some observations after anesthesia, during quiescent hanging behaviour and during visual obstructionBrain Behav Evol20026026527412476053
  • HowardRSFinneranJJRidgwaySHBispectral index of unihemispheric effects in dolphinsAnesth Analg200610362666316931672
  • LiaminOMukhametovLMSleep in cetaceansZh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova2013636174 [Article in Russian] [Abstract]23697223
  • MukhametovLMUnihemispheric slow-wave sleep in the Amazonian dolphin, Inia geoffrensisNeurosci Lett1987791281323670722
  • LyaminOIMangerPRRidgwaySAMukhametovLMSiegelJMCetacean sleep: an unusual form of mammalian sleepNeurosci Biobehav Rev2008321451148418602158
  • LillyJCComment on cerebral dominance in the dolphinMountcastleVBInterhemispheric Relations and Cerebral DominanceBaltimoreThe Johns Hopkins Press1962112114
  • LillyJCAnimals in aquatic environments: adaptation of mammals to the oceanDillDBAdolphEFWiberCGHandbook of Physiology: Adaptation to EnvironmentWashington, DCAmerican Physiological Society1964741747
  • SupinAYMukhametovLMLadyginaTFPopovVVMassAMPoliakovaIGElectrophysiological Studies of the Brain of DolphinsMoscowNauka1978 Cited by Mukhametov et al., 1984
  • MukhametovLMOleksenkoAIPolyakovaIGThe Black Sea bottle-nose dolphin: the structure of sleepSokolovVERomanenkoVEThe Black Sea Bottlenose DolphinMoscowNauka1997492512 Cited by Lyamin et al., 2008
  • LyaminOIMukhametovLMSiegelJMRelationship between sleep and eye state in Cetaceans and PinnipedsArch Ital Biol200414255756815493557
  • OleksenkoAILyaminOIRest and active states in Amazonian dolphins (Inia geoffrensis)J Sleep Res19943suppl 1318
  • LyaminOLMukhametovLMSiegelJMNazarenkoEAPolyakovaIGShpakOVUnihemispheric slow wave sleep and state of the eyes in a white whaleBehav Brain Res200212912512911809503
  • FlanaganWFNocturnal behaviour of captive small cetaceans. I: the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)Sleep Res1997384
  • FlanaganWFNocturnal behaviour of captive small cetaceans. II: the beluga whale, Delphinapterus leucasSleep Res1997385
  • ShpakOVLyaminOLMangerPRSiegelJMMukhametovLMRest and activity states in Commerson’s dolphin (Cephalorhynchus commersonii)Zh Evol Biokhim Fiziol20094597104 Article in Russian19370995
  • SupinYAMukhametovLMSome mechanisms of the unihemispheric slow wave sleep in dolphinsSokolovVEElectrophysiology of Sensory Systems in Marine MammalsMoscowNauka1986188207 Cited by Rattenborg et al., 2000
  • GoleyPDBehavioral aspects of sleep in Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens, Gill 1865)Mar Mam Sci1999151041064
  • GnoneGBenoldiCBonsignoriBFognamiPObservations on rest behaviours in captive bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)Aquat Mam2001272933
  • LyaminOIChetyrbokISUnilateral EEG activation during sleep in the Cape fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillusNeurosci Lett19921432632661436677
  • LyaminOILapierreJLKosenkoPOMukhametovLMSiegelJMElectroencephalogram asymmetry and spectral power during sleep in northern fur sealJ Sleep Res20081715416518482104
  • MukhametovLMLyaminOIPoliakovaIGInterhemispheric asynchrony of the sleep EEG in northern fur sealsExperientia198541103410354018223
  • LyaminOIPavlovaIFKosenkoPOMukhametovLMSiegelJMRegional differences in cortical electroencephalogram (EEG) slow wave activity and interhemispheric EEG asymmetry in the fur sealJ Sleep Res20122160361122676149
  • LyaminOIOleksenkoAIMukhametovLMParadoxical sleep in northern fur seals in water and on landJ Sleep Res19963suppl 1259
  • MukhametovLMSupinAYLyaminOIInterhemispheric asymmetry of the EEG during sleep in marine mammalsOnaniTNeurobiology of Sleep-Wakefulness CycleTbilisiMatsniereba1988147159 Cited by Rattenborg et al., 2002
  • LyaminOIThe ontogeny development of the interhemispheric asymmetry of the EEG during slow wave sleep in northern fur sealsJ High Nerve Act198737157159 Abstract
  • LyaminOICetyrbockISInterhemispheric asymmetry of the electrocorticogram during slow wave sleep in pups of Stellar’s sea lionsPhysiology of Marine Animals. All-Union ConferenceApatiti198999 Cited by Lyamin OI, Cetyrbock IS, 1992
  • LyaminOIOleksenkoAIPolyakovaIGSleep and wakefulness in pups of arp seal Phagophilus groenlandicaJ High Nerve Act19893910611069 [Article in Russian] [Abstract]
  • LyaminOISleep in the harp seal (Pagophilus groenlandica). Comparison of sleep on land and in waterJ Sleep Res1993217017410607090
  • CastelliniMAMilsomWKBergerRJPatterns of respiration and heart rate during wakefulness and sleep in elephant seal pupsAm J Physiol1994266R863R8698160882
  • MilsomWKCastelliniMAHarrisMEffects of hypoxia and hypercadmia on patterns of sleep-associated apnea in elephant seal pupsAm J Physiol1996271R1017R10248897995
  • MukhametovLMLyaminOIChetyrbokISVassilyevAADiazRPSleep in an Amazonian manatee, Trichechus inunguisExperientia1992484174191582500
  • BallNJShafferyJPOppMRCarterRLAmlanerCJAsynchronous eye-closure of birdsSleep Res19851487
  • BallNJWeaveGEAmlanerCJThe incidence of hemispheric sleep in birdsSleep Res19861558
  • BallNJAmlanerCJShafferyJPOppMRAsynchronous eye closure and unihemispheric quiet sleep of birdsKoellaWPObalFSchulzHVisserPSleep 86New York, NYGustav Fischer1988151153
  • SzymczakJTKaiserWHelbHWBeszczynskaBA study of sleep in the European blackbirdPhysiol Behav199660111511208884941
  • ToblerIBorbelyAASleep and EEG spectra in the pigeon (Columba livia) under baseline conditions and after sleep deprivationJ Comp Physiol A1988163729738
  • RattenborgNCUnilateral eye closure and interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during sleep in the pigeon (Columba livia)Brain Behav Evol20015832333212016351
  • WellmannAEDownsCTA behavioural study of sleep patterns in the malachite sunbird, Cape white-eye and fan-tailed widowbirdAnim Behav2009776166
  • RattenborgNCAmlanerCJLimaSLPeriod amplitude analysis of avian unihemispheric quiet sleepSleep199922S73
  • RattenborgNCLimaSLAmlanerCJFacultative control of avian unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predationBehav Brain Res199910516317210563490
  • RattenborgNCDo birds sleep in flight?Naturvissenschaften200693413425
  • RattenborgNCMandtBHObermeyerWHMigratory sleeplessness in the white-crowned sparrow (Zenotrichia leucophrys gambelii)PLoS Biol20042924936
  • FuchsTHaneyAJechuraJMooreFRBingmanVPDaytime naps in night-migrating birds: behavioural adaptation to seasonal sleep deprivation in the Swainson’s thrush (Catharus ustulatus)Anim Behav200672951958
  • FuchsTMauryDMooreFRBingmanVPDaytime micro-naps in a nocturnal migrant: an EEG analysisBiol Lett20095778018990656
  • AndrewRJBehavioural development and lateralizationRogersLJAndrewRJComparative Vertebrate LateralizationCambridgeCambridge University Press2002157205
  • RogersLJAndrewRJComparative Vertebrate LateralizationCambridgeCambridge University Press2002
  • VallortigaraGRogersLJSurvival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralizationBehav Brain Sci20052857558916209828
  • VallortigaraGZanforlinMCailottoMRight-left asymmetry in position learning of male chicksBehav Brain Res1988271891913358855
  • VallortigaraGRegolinLBortolomiolGTommasiLLateral symmetries in eye use due to preference during visual discrimination learning in eye in chicksBehav Brain Res1996741351438851922
  • RogersLJLight experience and asymmetry of brain functionNature19822972232257200573
  • RogersLJDevelopment of lateralizationAndrewRJNeural and Behavioural Plasticity: The Use of Domestic Chick as a ModelOxfordOxford University Press1991507535
  • ChiandrettiCVallortigaraGEffects of embryonic light stimulation on the ability to discriminate left from right in the domestic chickBehav Brain Res200919824024619059286
  • KleinMMichelFJouvetMEtude polygraphique du somneil chez les oiseauxCR Soc Biol196415890103
  • GoodmanIJThe study of sleep in birdsGoodmanIGScheinMWBirds, Brain and BehaviourNew York, NYAcademic Press1974133152
  • OokawaTTakagiKElectroencephalograms of free behavioural chicks at various developmental agesJap J Physiol19681887995302356
  • SpoonerCObservations on the Use of Chick in the Pharmacological Investigation of Central Nervous System [PhD dissertation]UCLALos Angeles, CA1964
  • OokawaTElectroencephalograms recorded from telencephalon of blinded chickens during behavioural sleep and wakefulnessPoult Sci1971507317365569755
  • RogersLJChaffeyGLateralized patterns of sleep activity in the developing chick brainProc Aust Neurosci Soc1994595
  • van LuijtelaarELvan der GrintenCPMBlokhuisHJCoenenAMLSleep in the domestic hen (Gallus domesticus)Phisiol Behav198741409414
  • BobboDGalvaniFMascettiGGVallortigaraGLight exposure of chick embryo influences monocular sleepBehav Brain Res200213444746612191832
  • MascettiGGRuggerMVallortigaraGVisual lateralization and monocular sleep in the domestic chickCogn Brain Res19997451463
  • AndrewRJThe development of visual lateralization in the domestic chickBehav Brain Res1988292012093048306
  • RashidNAndrewRJRight hemisphere advantages for topographical orientation in the domestic chickNeuropsychologia1989279379482771032
  • HornGNeural basis of recognition memory investigated through an analysis of imprintingPhilos Trans R Soc London B19903291331421978359
  • McCabeBJHemispheric asymmetry of learning-induced changesAndrewRJNeural and Behavioural Plasticity: The Use of the Domestic Chick as a ModelOxfordOxford University Press1991262276
  • KovachJKJSpatial orientation of the chick embryo during the last five days of incubationComp Physiol Psychol196866283288
  • MascettiGGVallortigaraGWhy do birds sleep with one eye open? Light exposure of chick embryo as a determinant of monocular sleepCurr Biol20011197197411448774
  • GastonKEGastonMGUnilateral memory after binocular discrimination training: left hemisphere dominance in the chick?Brain Res19843031901936733524
  • VallortigaraGComparative neuropsychology of the dual brain: a stroll through left and right animal’ perceptual worldsBrain Lang20007318921910856174
  • VallortigaraGAndrewRJLateralization of responses to change of in the model partner by chicksAnim Behav199141187194
  • MascettiGGRuggerMVallortigaraGBobboDMonocular-unihemispheric sleep and visual discrimination learning in the domestic chickExp Brain Res2007176708416874518
  • NeliniCBobboDMascettiGGLocal sleep: spatial learning task enhances sleep in right hemisphere of domestic chick (Gallus gallus)Exp Brain Res201020519520420625703
  • NeliniCBobboDMascettiGGMonocular learning of a spatial task enhances sleep in the right hemisphere of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus)Exp Brain Res201221838138822349558
  • PillayPMangerPRTesting thermogenesis as a basis for the evolution of cetaceans sleep phenomenologyJ Sleep Res20041335335815560770
  • MangerPRRidgwaySHSiegelJMThe locus coeruleus complex of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) as revealed by tyrosine hydroxilase immunohistochemistryJ Sleep Res20031214915512753352
  • KovalzonVMBrain temperature variations and ECoG in free-swimming bottlenose dolphinKoellaWPLevinPSleep 1976: Third European Congress of Sleep ResearchMontpellier, BaselKarger1977239241
  • KovalzonVMMukhametovLMTemperature variations in the brain corresponding to unihemispheric slow wave sleep in dolphinsJ Evol Biochem Physiol198218307309
  • OleksenkoAIMukhametovLMPolyakovaIGSupinAYKovalzonVMUnihemispheric sleep deprivation in bottlenose dolphinsJ Sleep Res19921404410607024
  • HorneJAWhy We Sleep? The Functions of Sleep in Humans and AnimalsNew York, NYOxford University Press1988
  • HorneJAMinardASleep and sleepiness following a behaviourally active dayErgonomics1985285675754018009
  • HorneJAWelmsleyBDaytime visual load and the effects upon human sleepPsychophysiology197613115120176675
  • BednekoffPARitterRVigilance in Nxai Pan springbok, Antidorcas marsupialisBehaviour1994129111
  • ElgarMAPredator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical evidenceBiol Rev19896413332655726
  • BobboDNeliniCMascettiGGBinocular and monocular/unihemispheric sleep in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) after a moderate sleep deprivationExp Brain Res200818542142717940755
  • BorbelyANeuhausHUSleep deprivation: effects on sleep and EEG in the ratJ Comp Physiol19791337187
  • BorbelyAAToblerIHanagasiogluMEffects of sleep deprivation on sleep and EEG spectra in the ratBehav Brain Res1984141711826525241
  • ToblerIScherschlichtRSleep and EEG slow-wave activity in the domestic catBehav Brain Res1990371091182322413
  • BoeremaASRiedstraBStrijkstraAMDecrease in monocular sleep after sleep deprivation in domestic chickenBehaviour200314014151420
  • RogersLJThe Development of Brain and Behaviour in the ChickenWallingfordCAB International1995
  • TaraoMOokawaTOn the encephalogram in the unilateral optic enucleated chickPoult Sci196948151615175824208
  • RogersLJBehavioural, structural and neurochemical asymmetries in the avian brain: a model system for studying visual development and processingNeuroci Biobehav Rev199620487503
  • SaperCBChouTCScammellTEThe sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulnessTrend Neurosci20012472673111718878
  • FullerPMGooleyJJSaperCBNeurobiology of sleep-wake cycle: sleep architecture, circadian regulation and regulatory feedbackSleep200621482493
  • KedzioraDJAbeysuriyaRGPhillipsAJKRobinsonPAPhysiologically based quantitative modelling of unihemispheric sleepJ Theor Biol201231410911922960411
  • SzymusiakRSteiningerTAlamNMcGintyDPreoptic area sleep-regulating mechanismsArch Ital Biol2001139779211256189
  • RidgwaySHHouserDFinneranJJFunctional imaging of dolphin brain metabolism and blood flowJ Exp Biol20062092902291016857874
  • TapleyRGRidgwaySHCorpus callosum size in delphinid CetaceansBrain Behav Evol1994441561657987664
  • CuenodMCommissural pathways in interhemispheric transfer of visual information in the pigeonSchmittFOWordenFGThe Neuroscience Study ProgramCambridge, MAMIT Press19742129
  • BerlucchiGElectroencephalographic studies in spli-barin catsElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophisiol199620348356
  • MontplaisirJNielesenTCoteJBoivinDRouleauILapierreGInterhemispheric EEG coherence before and after partial callosotomyClin Electroencephalogr19902142472297948
  • NielsenTMountplasirJLassondeMDecreased Interhemispheric coherence during sleep in agenesis of corpus callosumEur Neurol1993331731768467828
  • MichelFRoffwargHPChronic split brainstem preparation: effect on sleep-waking cycleExperientia1967231261286032104
  • LapierreJLKosenkoPOLyaminOIKodamaTMukhametovLMSiegelJMCortical acetylcholine release is lateralized during asymmetrical slow-wave sleep in northern fur sealsJ Neurosci200727119991200617978041
  • LapierreJKosenkoPOKodamaTSymmetrical serotonin release during asymmetrical slow-wave sleep: implications for the neurochemistry of wake-sleep statesJ Neurosci2013332555256123392683
  • Porkka-HeiskanenTStreckerREThakkarMBjorkumAAGreeneRWMcCarleyRWAdenosine: a mediator of sleep inducing effects of prolonged wakefulnessScience1997276126512689157887
  • Porkka-HeiskanenTAlankoLKalinchuckAStenbergDAdenosine and sleepSleep Med Rev2002632133212531135
  • SchwierinBBorbélyAAToblerIEffects of N6-cyclopentyladenosine and caffeine on sleep regulation in the ratEur J Pharmacol19963001631718739204
  • MarairtySRainnieDMcCarleyRGreeneRDisinhibition of ventrolateral preoptic area sleep-active neurons by adenosine: a new mechanism for sleep promotionNeuroscience200412345145714698752
  • GolsteinLStoltzfusNWGardockJFChanges in interhemispheric amplitude relationships in the EEG during sleepPhysiol Behav197288118154339962
  • ArmitageRHoffmannRLoewyDMoffittAVariations in period-analysed EEG asymmetry in REM and NREMPsychophysiology1989263293362756082
  • BencaRMObermeyerWHLarsonCLEEG alpha power and alfa power asymmetry in sleep and wakefulnessPsychophysiology19993643043610432792
  • SekimotoMKatoMKajimuraNWatanabeTTakahashiKOkumaTAsymmetric interhemispheric delta waves during all-night sleep in humansClin Neurophysiol200011192492810802465
  • RothCAchermannPBorbelyAAFrequency and state specific hemispheric asymmetries in the human sleep EEGNeurosci Lett199927113914210507688
  • IkedaKMorotomiTAsymmetries of behavioural awakenings during nocturnal sleep in humansJpn J Physiol Psichol Psichophysiol1994123944
  • BertiniMViolaniCZoccolottiPAntonelliADi StefanoLRight cerebral activation in REM sleep: evidence from a unilateral tactile recognition testPsichophysiology198421418423
  • GordonHVFroomanBLaviePShift in cognitive asymmetries between wakings from REM and NREM sleepNeuropsichologia19822099103
  • CasagrandeMViolaniCDe GennaroLBraibantiPBertiniMWhich hemisphere falls asleep first?Neuropsichologia199533815822
  • CasagrandeMBertiniMLaterality of sleep onset process: which hemisphere goes to sleep first?Biol Psychol200877768017996353
  • WyazovskiyVBorbelyAAToblerIInterhemispheric sleep EEG asymmetry in the rat is enhanced by sleep deprivationJ Neurophysiol2002882280228612424269
  • GeneLEstebanSGonzalezJAsymmetric sleep in ratsLaterality20121711721557129
  • CajohemCDi BiaseRImaiMInterhemispheric EEG asymmetries during unilateral bright light exposure and subsequent sleep in humansAm J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol2008294R1053R106018216135
  • FloESteineIBlagstadTGronliJPallesenSPortasCMTransient changes in frontal alpha asymmetry as a measure of emotional and physical distress during sleepBrain Res2011136723424920888803
  • KettlerHDijkDJBorbelyAEffect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation prior to sleep on the sleep EEG in humansJ Sleep Res1994315916410607121
  • CottoneLAAdamoDSquiresNKThe effect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation on hemispheric asymmetries during slow wave sleepSleep200427636814998239
  • WyazovskiyVBorbelyAAToblerIUnilateral vibrissae stimulation during waking induces interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during subsequent sleep in the ratJ Sleep Res2000936737111123523
  • WyazovskiyVToblerIHandedness leads to interhemispheric EEG asymmetry during sleep in the ratJ Neurophysiol20089996997518077659
  • HuberRGhilardiMFMassiminiMTononiGLocal sleep and learningNature2004430788115184907
  • HuberRGhilardiMFMassiminiMArm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decrease sleep slow wave activityNat Neurosci200691169117616936722
  • LeskuJVyssotskiAMartinez-GonzalezDWilzeckCRattenborgNCLocal sleep homeostasis in the avian brain: convergence of sleep function in mammals and birds?Proc Biol Sci20112782419242821208955
  • LyaminOIKosenkoPOVyssotskiALLapierreJLSiegelJMMukhametovLMStudy of sleep in a WalrusDokl Biol Sci201244418819122760621
  • MukhametovLMThe absence of paradoxical sleep in dolphinsKoellaWPObalFSchulzHVisserPSleep’86Stuttgart, New York, NYGustav Springer Verlag1988154156
  • LyaminOILapierreJLKosenkoPOMonoamine release during unihemispheric sleep and unihemispheric waking in the fur sealSleep Epub20151222