494
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Spotlight on the Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) Test

ORCID Icon
Pages 73-81 | Published online: 01 Mar 2021

Abstract

The developmental eye movement (DEM) test is a practical and simple method for assessing and quantifying ocular motor skills in children. In this review, a summary of the literature relevant to the DEM test has been made, its psychometric properties and its pros and cons have also been considered. The DEM test provides clinicians with a simple method of measuring eye movement using a psychometric test. Over the years, many studies have enabled the identification of the strengthens and weaknesses of this test apart from outlining the psychometric properties. The validity of the test has been checked and expanded over time and studies have shown that the DEM test measures an aspect of eye movement related to reading, rather than purely parameters associated with eye-movement. Some reservations have emerged regarding the repeatability of the test because a degree of learning effect emerges over multiple sessions. Being aware of this point allows correct clinical application and interpretation of the test. Normative data in children were available for nine languages and countries. So far, DEM test could be applied clinically in each case when a rapid test of eye movement was required, such as in testing for vision-related visual problems.

Introduction

During the process of reading, the eyes change their position quickly in a series of saccades. These alternate with fixations in which the information is acquired. Several studies have demonstrated that learning disabled (LD) and generally poor readers may perform inadequate ocular movements,Citation1Citation3 producing more regression and fixation, and additional, although short, left to right saccades.Citation4

Figure 1 The three cards of the DEM test: A, B and C. The images came from the original test and they were presented in a low resolution only for representative purpose. Image courtesy from Bernell Corp. Citation5

Figure 1 The three cards of the DEM test: A, B and C. The images came from the original test and they were presented in a low resolution only for representative purpose. Image courtesy from Bernell Corp. Citation5

With these indications, clinicians require a practical and basic instrument to assess ocular movement skills during reading in a simple way as an alternative to eye-tracking instruments. The DEM test () is a practical paper-based psychometric test designed for the assessment of ocular movement in a reading-like condition.Citation5 This review outlines a summary of the recent literature, together with giving consideration to the classic studies that are the key to understanding its application, the psychometric properties, and pros and cons of the DEM test.

Eye Movement Testing

The examination of eye movements can be performed by using different techniques, adapted according to the age, the aims of evaluation and the time required.Citation6 Each method has its pros and cons. A simple summary of all the possible methods for ocular movement testing is presented in .

Table 1 An Outline of the Available Methods for Measuring Eye Movements

The history of the psychometric measurement of eye movements originates from the Pierce test,Citation7 which was composed of four cards, one pre-test and three test cards. In this test, a subject was required to perform saccades from a column of numbers on the left to one on the right, while naming the numbers. The pre-test and the first cards were used as a training phase. The execution time was measured, and it was compared to the norms supplied with the test.

The King Devick (KD)Citation8 and the NYSOA K-D tests,Citation9 are tests similar to that of Pierce, with subjects required to name the number quickly. The KD differs from the Pierce test, in that the numbers were arranged in a reading-like condition. The evolution of Pierce to KD is in the arrangement of the numbers, from left-to-right equal-space saccades to irregular and unpredictable horizontal saccades for reading.

However, in both of these tests, there is the problem of the influence of baseline naming in the total time of execution.Citation10 The overall performance could be slowed both by eye movements and/or the naming process. Consequently, the DEM test was developed because it was realized that naming deficit represents an important problem in dyslexic and learning disabled children.Citation11 The vertical time and the calculation of ratio, together with the four clinical response types, could differentiate naming problems from oculomotor disorders.Citation5,Citation12

The DEM Test

The Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test is a psychometric paper test for the assessment of eye movement performance in children. The quantification of eye movement is performed by naming numbers in a reading-like condition without sophisticated instruments. The task consists of naming numbers in a reading-like condition without using a complex instrument.Citation5,Citation12,Citation13

The DEM test is composed of a pre-test and three test cards: A, B, and C. The pre-test card is composed of 12 horizontal numbers intended to assess the basic skills for completing the test. The first two cards (A and B) comprise two columns of 20 numbers for each card. Card C is composed of the same 80 numbers from the A and B cards but arranged in a horizontal pattern, similar to a reading text. The subjects tested were usually seated in a comfortable position with an inclined lectern positioned at 40 cm, and they were subsequently presented with the different cards. The subjects were required to read the numbers on the different cards as fast as possible. For the vertical cards A and B, the participants had to read aloud the two columns of numbers vertically, while for card C, the participants had to read aloud the same 80 numbers horizontally in a condition analogous to reading. The time of execution for all cards and the errors made for card C were recorded. Subsequently, two scores were calculated. The Vertical Time (VT) is calculated by adding the times for the two cards A and B. The Adjusted Horizontal Time (AHT) is the time for card C, adjusted for addition or omission errors. The Ratio score is calculated by dividing the adjusted horizontal time by the vertical time. The total number of errors is the sum of the different errors made for card C. The VT represents the time spent naming 80 numbers, while the AHT reflects the total time taken to name numbers plus the time to perform saccadic eye movements. The Ratio is the main measure used to evaluate ocular movement performance. Each score was successively compared with the specific language normative data for the detection of number naming or eye movements problems, or a combination of the two.

Application of DEM Test

Who Needs to Use the DEM Test and Why?

As mentioned earlier, compared to Pierce and KD, only the DEM test takes into account the naming skills and, for this reason, the target population of this test is children without, with, or with suspected learning disabilities aged between 6 and 13 years.Citation13 However, the age at which the test could be applied depends primarily on the existing norms. The different norms that are available in different languages have been limited in higher ages from 11Citation14,Citation15 to 18 years old.Citation16 Application outside these norms has been possible, but while being aware of the limitations of the evaluation.Citation13,Citation17 Taking into account the higher age available and the percentile scoring, it is also almost possible to detect the severe cases of naming and or oculomotor problems in adults.Citation18 Ratio score was demonstrated as a predictor of an oculomotor dysfunction through the complete life span.Citation17,Citation19 In fact, a modified version of the DEM test shows a mean ratio of about 1.04 (SD 0.10), for participants aged between 14 and 68 years, without differences by year groups.Citation20

Studies that investigate visual problems have shown that 62% of developmental dyslexia or learning disabled (DD/LD) children present oculomotor problems on application of the DEM test, compared to 15% of those whose development was typical.Citation21 In the neurological population with acquired brain injury (ABI) or traumatic brain injury (TBI), the DEM test has proven to be useful in the assessment of baseline behavior and or specific oculomotor treatment,Citation22 other than the screening capacity in a baseline clinical practice.Citation23 Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) showed mild atypical performance in the DEM test.Citation24

The DEM test has no formal restriction to its use, and it could be performed by different professionals involved in the care of learning-disabled children. The latest manual reports that the test could also be administered by classroom and special education teachers, reading specialists, and school psychologists rather than only by vision specialists.Citation13 However, the personnel applying the test need to be aware of its standardized administration, and detailed interpretation of the results could only be performed by professionals who have a complete understanding of the vision and cognitive status of the child.Citation21 In these cases, a multidisciplinary team is preferable for addressing the different clinical manifestations of LD problems.Citation25 However, the different legislation and professionals involved in various countries make a unique statement very difficult to perform.

What We Can and Cannot Do with the DEM Test?

Some procedures, such as gross observations of eye movements, have found a relationship with reading disability.Citation26,Citation27 Compared to the DEM test, the gross observation of eye movements is considered to have a different accuracy (see validity paragraph below). The NSUCO (Northeastern State University College of Optometry)Citation26 oculomotor test is a standardized method and scoring of observational eye movement testing (saccades and pursuit). A comparison between the NSUCO accuracy scale and the DEM test in a group of children did not find a significant relationship between these tests because they measure ocular movements in a different way and with different accuracy.Citation28 The DEM test is one that is designed for ocular movement in a specific reading-like condition and a simple exterior validity confirms this point. The DEM test was constructed with the aim of testing ocular movement in a reading-like condition in children, and the VT score takes into account the naming skill and allows the correct detection of problems. Based on the results of the four scores (VT, AHT, Ratio, and Errors), four possible classifications can be made using the cut-off score of the 30th or 15th percentiles.Citation13,Citation29Citation31 Normal VT, AHT, and Ratio (in percentiles) express the behavior of a normal subject. Low VT, low AHT, and normal Ratio express the behavior of a naming problem. Normal VT, low AHT, and low Ratio express an oculomotor disorder and, a low VT, a low AHT, and a low Ratio express comorbidity of naming and oculomotor problems. Despite the four types of problems that could be found being clear, a continuum evaluation of each subtest using percentile scores may be the best way to express this result.Citation30

Conversely, the DEM test cannot detect reading problems or other conditions for different reasons.Citation32 Even if naming numbers does express one part of language processing,Citation33 it does not represent the final process of reading, which is more complex.Citation34 The reading process could only be tested by direct reading of the sequence of words/non-words, sentences, and paragraphs. The different reading levels need to be adapted to the level of development and skill acquisition of the child. Specific reading tests were available for each specific step of developing reading, for example, three for each year from 2nd to 5th grade.Citation35 For example, the vertical numbers of the DEM test are similar to the subtest 1B of the DDE battery.Citation36 The DDE battery is an Italian test for the assessment of reading and it is aimed at assessing different aspects of reading skills. Since reading batteries are very different from each other and also language specific, other tests in particular languages could have included vertical number reading as a component of the examination. In summary, vertical number naming represents only one of the aspects of the multi-faceted process of reading.

A study that investigates the relationship between the diagnosis of dyslexia and the results of DEM found no significant relationship.Citation32 Not all dyslexic children (and even more learning disabled) are equal. Even though the diagnosis was the same (eg based on DSV IV-TR; DSM-V or ICD-10-CM), there are different cognitive skills profiles for every single case.Citation11 As a result, there are dyslexic children with normal ocular movement and those with poor ocular movement.Citation21,Citation27,Citation37Citation39 This point explains the lack of a strong relationship between these two aspects and, at the same time, underlines the need to test the ocular movements because they could be normal or poor in each dyslexic child. Another source of variability between children is the fact that not only pathological dyslexic children need testing and care but also poor readers. This is a problem that is also defined with reading difficulties: child below normal range but not severe enough to be diagnosed as having learning disabilities. Finally, specific LD is not represented only by dyslexia but also by dysgraphia, dysorthographia, and dyscalculia,Citation40,Citation41 making the heterogeneity between cases more widely present and easy to detect. DEM test was also used in testing the oculomotor behaviour of DCD children.Citation24

Psychometrical Properties of DEM Test

The clinical usefulness of specific tests needs to be determined not subjectively by clinicians but it should be based on results of scientific studies on validity, repeatability, normative data, and other measurement properties.Citation42,Citation43

Validity

Validity expresses the ability of the test to measure what it is supposed to measure.Citation44 There is no unique form of validity, and even recent studies have attempted to put the different aspects of validity into the construct of validity, going over the boundary between content and criterion.Citation45 This point could be obtained by using a discrete amount of evidence that represents a check of the construct.Citation46

The validity of the DEM test has been tested several times and in several ways over the years. Initially, the first DEM study reported four methods that were originally used to check validity: raw scores and chronological age, internal consistency, relationship with an achievement test, and the relationship with education level.Citation12 The first analysis showed that DEM scores improve with age, and this result was subsequently confirmed and evident in all normative studies.

The second aspect is internal consistency. This aspect is peculiar and requires a deeper analysis. Garzia et alCitation12 reported that most internal correlations were significant and the lack of significance between VT and Ratio is a sign of the independence of the two factors. Subsequently, a replication studyCitation28 confirmed the previous results using partial correlation (age removed). In addition, an exploratory factor analysis was also performed. Exploratory factor analysis is a statistical method used to identify the underlying relationships between measured variables.Citation47 The results support the structure of the test and the previous internal correlations, showing saturation to three main factors. The first factor is highly related to VT and AHT and represents the naming component. The second factor is related to AHT and Ratio and could represent the overall process of eye movement. The third factor is related only to Errors and represents this score.

The relationship with reading and/or achievement tests, as shown by Garzia et al,Citation12 is complex and it does not have a simple and direct explanation. The purpose of this relationship in terms of an indication of validity concerns the fact that LD children diagnosed with reading tests perform worst in the DEM test. A priori, this seems to be legitimate and different results show a trend in this direction.Citation12,Citation28,Citation38,Citation48 However, over time there is evidence that not all LD children present deficits in ocular movements,Citation21,Citation37 since they have a different cognitive profile.Citation11,Citation49 Therefore, the general comparison between LD and non-LD children in DEM results and their correlation does not perfectly represent an indication of validity. However, the original study reported a significant difference in all aspects of DEM between lower-achieving and TD children. A comparison between DD and TD children using different reading tests showed differences in the VT and AHT scores, but not in Ratio and Errors.Citation28 Taking into account the correlation between tests, the AHT showed a positive relationship (partial correlation, age removed) to a test of reading words. A large relationship was found between the time required to read a series of words and AHT (r = 0.65 p <0.01).Citation28 A subsequent study has confirmed this result.Citation50 VT and AHT are good predictors of academic performanceCitation12,Citation51,Citation52 and reading rate.Citation50,Citation53 Consequently, VT and, in particular, AHT itself can still be useful in clinical settings when a quick evaluation is required.Citation28,Citation48,Citation54

After taking into account the convergent and divergent validity with other tests, only a moderate relationship was found with AHT (r = 0.29) for comparison with a test of visual exploration.Citation28 Conversely, the relationship with the NSUCO oculomotor test (accuracy score) was low and not significant.Citation28 Both test eye movement but at two different levels.

The final step of validity seems to be reached in relating DEM to objective eye movement. At least four articles have reported this analysis. In the first study, the authors found no significant correlation between DEM test performance and specific eye movement parameters such as peak velocity, gain, latency, and corrective saccades. Conversely, they did find a significant correlation between reading and visual processing speed. They concluded that DEM may be useful and should retain its diagnostic role in the clinical practice.Citation48 However, they did not take into account the total number of saccades performed during the test, which is the most simple indication of the quality of eye movement.

The second study investigated the relationship between DEM results, eye movement parameters, and standardized reading achievement scores in normally-sighted children. The results showed that there was a positive and significant correlation between AHT and the number of fixations (0.41), and between AHT and span recognition (−0.42). In addition, it was found that there was a significant correlation between reading rate and VT (−0.41), and between AHT and reading rate (−0.55).Citation54 It is necessary to emphasise that eye movement recording was performed using standard text and not during the DEM test.

The third study specifically recorded eye movement during the execution of DEM in DD, DD age-equivalent, and TD children, other than in reading tests. The results showed a significant correlation between DEM C and reading test but, interestingly, a positive and significant relationship between mean duration of fixation and time of execution of DEM C in each group tested. The authors concluded that the DEM test is a useful tool to test eye movement abilities in dyslexic children. They also explained that the lack of a relationship with saccades is to be expected because the participants had not read any text.Citation38

The authors of the fourth study have performed deep eye tracking measurement and analysis during the execution of a digital version of DEM. The authors also added a test of visual processing speed. After analysis of each specific eye movement parameter, they concluded that the number, the amplitude, and direction of the saccades were related to DEM performance on the horizontal, but not to the vertical subtest. Conversely, the time spent on the execution of saccades was small compared to the time spent on fixations. The duration of fixation was positively correlated with performance on vertical and horizontal DEM subtest, and with visual processing speed. In summary, the DEM test is not a saccade test, but expresses a whole score of a set of eye movement parameters, together with visual verbal and visual processing speed skills.Citation55

Broadly speaking, DEM seems not only to be measuring saccades, but a general oculomotor behavior. This is because the underlying process seems not only to be linked to the saccades themselves, but also to fixations in which saccade preparation is made.Citation38,Citation48,Citation54,Citation55

There is a minor point in question regarding the (face) validity of the DEM test which is related to the lack of correction for errors in vertical time. This problem arises from a mistake in the first edition of the manual and relies on the difference between the reported scoresheet and the description given in the manual. This was clarified in the second version of the manual. In the first edition, there is space in the scoresheet to calculate and report the adjusted vertical time, even though elsewhere in the manual and in the associated article that was published it was highlighted that the adjustment of vertical time should be avoided because errors in this part of the test are very low. This has also been reflected in different interpretations between reported norms because some use unadjusted timeCitation5,Citation16,Citation30 whilst others use adjusted VT.Citation15,Citation56 However, there are few errors performed in VT and this only became relevant for very inaccurate participants with a severe naming problem.

For adults, a specific variation of DEM, the A-DEM was developed. The A-DEM was constructed with the aim of increasing the cognitive visuo-verbal retrieval demand.Citation20 However this is contrary to the basic principle of reducing the cognitive demand on the vertical test. Consequently, the primary goal of the original test was not achieved. Nevertheless, the results of A-DEM showed that some parameters including the Ratio were consistent between age groups. Finally, a panel conference concluded that the DEM was preferable to the A–DEM.Citation23

Norms

Since the DEM test is a language-specific test and there is a different learning curve for each country which depends on socio-demographic, political, and cultural differences, the normative data in the first years of school become important. Normative values are actually available for nine languages and countries: English,Citation5 two for Spanish,Citation14,Citation57 Cantonese,Citation15 Japanese,Citation56 Portuguese,Citation58 Italian,Citation30 Mandarin,Citation59 Latvian,Citation16 and FrenchCitation60 languages.

Are the norms for each language similar or different? Some comparisons that have been made with seven of them showed that some are different, particularly for the younger group and in the lower percentile rank.Citation15,Citation30,Citation58,Citation59 Coherently, each specific norm has to be used for each language. Conversely, other direct comparisons did not find differences.Citation14,Citation57,Citation60

In summary, since some specific norms were different and others appeared similar to the original American ones, those for DEM seem to be related to differences in languages, cultures, and education systems. Therefore, specific norms need to be used in order to avoid errors in the classification of results.

For adults, the situation is relatively complex. Adults are interesting because they have a higher risk of acquired brain injury with the need for an evaluation of eye movement,Citation19 other than for adult dyslexics who have special needs. Some studies have taken into account this age group but without recognizing normative data.Citation22 This is reflected in the requirement for adult norms for the DEM test.Citation17,Citation19,Citation23 However, the results of A-DEM showed that some parameters, including the Ratio, were constant between age groups. Based on this information, higher tier values could be used in the absence of specific norms.

Repeatability

The two terms reliability and repeatability have been used in the same way in the case of the DEM test, even though they themselves express different concepts.Citation61 In test-retest studies, ICC or others correlations were used for repeatability. In combination with this approach, the agreement analyses and Bland Altman plot and calculation were used and the mean bias between sessions compared.

Garzia et alCitation12 showed that the DEM test presents the following correlation coefficients: (Pearson r): r = 0.89, p <0.001; r = 0.86, p <0.01; r = 0.57 p <0.05 and r = 0.07 n.s. respectively for VT, AHT, Ratio, and errors. These data showed that the DEM test has good test-retest reliability for vertical and horizontal time, but medium for Ratio, and low for Errors. A subsequent study has substantially replicated these results.Citation62 Lower results were found by Rouse et alCitation63 in testing 3rd-grade children. It was found that VT and AHT presented fair to good repeatability, whereas the Ratio score was poor. Limits of agreement were added. Tassinari and DeLandCitation29 found that the correlation coefficients were higher than those previously found. They added the comparison of the pass-fail classification and good agreement was found between test and retest.

Orlansky et alCitation31 performed a comprehensive evaluation of reliability. Subjects were tested three times in two sessions using parallel versions of the DEM test. Correlation coefficients showed fair to good correlation for the VT and AHT scores and poor results for Ratio and Error scores. Interestingly, they found general improvements in performance in all test scores in the retest session. Coherently, they suggested being careful in the use of DEM results for diagnosis or the assessment of treatment results.

Taking into account all previous observations, Facchin and MaffiolettiCitation28 performed another test-retest comparison, using the clinical version of the test, and not the parallel form of DEM used in the previously cited study.Citation31 They found high values for VT and AHT, medium to high for Ratio, and medium for Errors. Conversely, they confirmed the presence of a significant improvement in performance (mean bias) in the second session for VT, AHT, and Ratio and provided the minimum change in percentile for a significant result. However, the so-called learning effect (an improvement in the second session) was lower than was previously found.

In summary, the DEM test shows high correlation values but conversely poor agreement between sessions due to an improvement of performance over session. Even though this learning effect appears clear in different studies, the second edition of the manual gives a solution. Richman suggests a retest of the vertical and/or horizontal part in the case of a low score.

This problem of agreement affects not only repeatability itself but also normative data. The suggestion of Richman is clinically correct, but it is necessary to take into account the fact that normative data were built for the first administration. The last clinical suggestion is to take into account that this limitation and interpretation of results (and improvement after session or therapy) has to be performed with prior knowledge of this problem of agreement.

This learning-effect problem also affects the King Devick test because it is very similar.Citation64 The repeatability and agreement of the KD test show the same general behavior. Correlations (ICC) were high, but there is a significant improvement in performance in the second administration (reduced time of execution). The authors concluded that learning effects occur for the familiarity with the test. They recommended limiting the comparison of the results that should be made with the individual baseline performance, rather than with normative data.

Final Remarks

The selection of an appropriate instrument must take into account its strengthens and limitations and also with awareness these points.Citation42

Different instruments have been developed for a specific purpose, but this could evolve with time and experience. The DEM test has demonstrated its use in developmental-age applications, compared to King Devick which has been used in stroke, sport, and concussion.Citation65Citation67 Nevertheless, since research and application cannot be limited, these differences in clinical use have become less significant,Citation22,Citation68 and DEM and KD could coexist in the same field of application.

Future Direction

Based on these considerations, the future use of the DEM test could involve stroke and other neurological patients.Citation69,Citation70 For these reasons, there is a requirement for specific adult norms and the investigation of the application and usefulness in these specific deficits.Citation71 In longer term, taking into account the Scheiman and Rouse three-component model of vision,Citation72,Citation73 the DEM test could not only establish its position but also become part of a wide psychometric battery of eye movement tests together with other tests, such as NSUCO, Groffman visual tracingCitation74 and others.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • Lefton LA, Lahey BB, Stagg DI. Eye movements in reading disabled and normal children: a study of systems and strategies. J Learn Disabil. 1978;11(9):549–558.
  • Pavlidis GT. The dyslexics erratic eye movements: case studies. Dyslexia Rev. 1978;1(1):22–28.
  • Pavlidis GT. Eye movements in dyslexia: their diagnostic significance. J Learn Disabil. 1985;18(1):42–50. doi:10.1177/002221948501800109
  • Jacobson JZ, Dodwell PC. Saccadic eye movements during reading. Brain Lang. 1979;8(3):303–314. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(79)90058-0
  • Richman JE, Garzia RP. Developmental Eye Movement Test, Examiners Booklet, Version 1. South Bend: Bernell Corp; 1987.
  • Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The Neurology of Eye Movements. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press, USA; 1999. Available from: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=nlebk&AN=283124&site=ehost-live&scope=site. Accessed February 11 2021.
  • Pierce J. Pierce Saccade Test. Bloomington: Cook inc; 1972.
  • King AT. The Proposed King-Devick Test and Its Relation to the Pierce Saccade and Reading Level. Chicago, IL: Available from the Carl Shepherd Memorial Library; 1976.
  • Lieberman S, Cohen AH, Rubin J. NYSOA KD test. J Am Optom Assoc. 1983;54(7):631.
  • Richman JE, Walker AJ, Garzia RP. The impact of automatic digit naming ability on a clinical test of eye movement functioning. J Am Optom Assoc. 1983;54(7):617.
  • Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, et al. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126(4):841–865. doi:10.1093/brain/awg076
  • Garzia RP, Richman JE, Nicholson SB, Gaines CS. A new visual-verbal saccade test: the development eye movement test (DEM). J Am Optom Assoc. 1990;61:124–135.
  • Richman JE. Developmental Eye Movement Test, Examiner’s Manual, Version 2.0. South Bend: Bernell Corp; 2009.
  • Fernandez-Velazquez FJ, Fernandez-Fidalgo MJ. Do DEM test scores change with respect to the language? Norms for Spanish-speaking population. Optom Vis Sci. 1995;72:902–906. doi:10.1097/00006324-199512000-00009
  • Pang PC, Lam CS, Woo GC. The Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test and Cantonese-speaking children in Hong Kong SAR, China. Clin Exp Optom. 2010;93:213–223. doi:10.1111/j.1444-0938.2010.00470.x
  • Serdjukova J, Ekimane L, Valeinis J, Skilters J, Krumina G. How strong and weak readers perform on the Developmental Eye Movement test (DEM): norms for Latvian school-aged children. Read Writ. 2016;1–20. doi:10.1007/s11145-016-9671-7
  • Powell JM, Birk K, Cummings EH, Ciol MA. The need for adult norms of the Developmental Eye Movement Test (DEM). J Behav Optom. 2005;16(2).
  • Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT. Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. Exp Brain Res. 1998;121(4):391–400. doi:10.1007/s002210050473
  • Powell JM, Fan M-Y, Kiltz PJ, Bergman AT, Richman JA. Comparison of the Developmental Eye Movement Test (DEM) and a Modified Version of the Adult Developmental Eye Movement Test (A-DEM) with Older Adults. J Behav Optom. 2006.
  • Sampredo A, Richman JE, Pardo MS. The adult developmental eye movement test (A-DEM): a tool for saccadic evaluation in adults. J Behav Optom. 2003;4(1):l–5.
  • Raghuram A, Gowrisankaran S, Swanson E, Zurakowski D, Hunter DG, Waber DP. Frequency of visual deficits in children with developmental dyslexia. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018;136(10):1089–1095. doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.2797
  • Kapoor N, Ciuffreda KJ. Assessment of neuro-optometric rehabilitation using the Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test in adults with acquired brain injury. J Optom. 2018;11(2):103–112. doi:10.1016/j.optom.2017.01.001
  • Radomski MV, Finkelstein M, Llanos I, Scheiman M, Wagener SG. Composition of a vision screen for servicemembers with traumatic brain injury: consensus using a modified nominal group technique. Am J Occup Ther. 2014;68(4):422–429.
  • Bellocchi S, Ducrot S, Tallet J, Jucla M, Jover M. Effect of comorbid developmental dyslexia on oculomotor behavior in children with developmental coordination disorder: a study with the Developmental Eye Movement test. Hum Mov Sci. 2021;76. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2021.102764
  • Toffalini E, Giofrè D, Cornoldi C. Strengths and weaknesses in the intellectual profile of different subtypes of specific learning disorder: a study on 1049 diagnosed children. Clin Psychol Sci. 2017;5(2):402–409. doi:10.1177/2167702616672038
  • Maples WC, Ficklin T. Comparison of eye movement skills between above average and below average readers. J Behav Optom. 1990;1(4):87–91.
  • Bilbao C, Piñero DP. Clinical characterization of oculomotricity in children with and without specific learning disorders. Brain Sci. 2020;10(11):836. doi:10.3390/brainsci10110836
  • Facchin A, Maffioletti S, Carnevali T. Validity reassessment of developmental eye movement (DEM) test in the Italian population. Optom Vis Dev. 2011;42:155–167. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01279
  • Tassinari JT, DeLand P. Developmental Eye Movement Test: reliability and symptomatology. Optometry. 2005;76:387–399. doi:10.1016/j.optm.2005.05.006
  • Facchin A, Maffioletti S, Carnevali T. The developmental eye movement (DEM) test: normative data for Italian population. Optom Vis Dev. 2012;43.
  • Orlansky G, Hopkins KB, Mitchell GL, et al. Reliability of the developmental eye movement test. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:1507–1519. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e318230f03a
  • Griffin JR, Lung J, Nguyen M. The developmental eye movement test and detection of dyslexia. Optom Vis Sci. 1998;75(12):17.
  • Blachman BA. Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. J Educ Psychol. 1984;76(4):610. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.76.4.610
  • Di Filippo G, Brizzolara D, Chilosi A, et al. Rapid naming, not cancellation speed or articulation rate, predicts reading in an orthographically regular language (Italian). Child Neuropsychol. 2005;11(4):349–361. doi:10.1080/09297040490916947
  • Cornoldi C, Colpo G. Prove Di Lettura MT per La Scuola Elementare-2 Seconda Elementare. Giunti: OS Organizzazioni Speciali; 1998.
  • Sartori G, Job R, Tressoldi PE. Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva. Firenze Organ Spec. 1995.
  • Vagge A, Cavanna M, Traverso CE, Iester M. Evaluation of ocular movements in patients with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 2015;65(1):24–32. doi:10.1007/s11881-015-0098-7
  • Moiroud L, Gerard CL, Peyre H, Bucci MP. Developmental Eye Movement test and dyslexic children: a pilot study with eye movement recordings. PLoS One. 2018;13(9):e0200907. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0200907
  • Eden GF, Stein JF, Wood HM, Wood FB. Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal children. Vision Res. 1994;34(10):1345–1358. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90209-7
  • Lagae L. Learning disabilities: definitions, epidemiology, diagnosis, and intervention strategies. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2008;55(6):1259–1268. doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2008.08.001
  • Peterson RL, McGrath LM, Willcutt EG, Keenan JM, Olson RK, Pennington BF. How specific are learning disabilities? J Learn Disabil. In press 2021. doi:0022219420982981
  • Elliott DB. Evidence-based optometry and in-practice research. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2012;32(2):81–82. doi:10.1111/j.1475-1313.2012.00899.x
  • Elliott DB. What is the appropriate gold standard test for refractive error? Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2017;37(2):115–117. doi:10.1111/opo.12360
  • Kline P. The Handbook of Psychological Testing. 2nd ed. London: Routledge; 1999.
  • Urbina S. Essentials in Psychological Testing. Hoboken NJ: John Wiley and Sons; 2004.
  • Anastasi A, Urbina S. Psychological Testing. 7th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1997.
  • Norris M, Lecavalier L. Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in developmental disability psychological research. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(1):8–20. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0816-2
  • Ayton LN, Abel LA, Fricke TR, McBrien NA. Developmental eye movement test: what is it really measuring? Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86:722–730. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e3182195dae
  • Heim S, Tschierse J, Amunts K, et al. Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2008;68(1):73.
  • Palomo-álvarez C, Puell MC. Relationship between oculomotor scanning determined by the DEM test and a contextual reading test in schoolchildren with reading difficulties. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2009;247:1243–1249. doi:10.1007/s00417-009-1076-8
  • Hopkins S, Black AA, White SLJ, Wood JM. Visual information processing skills are associated with academic performance in Grade 2 school children. Acta Ophthalmol. 2019;97(8):e1141–e1148. doi:10.1111/aos.14172
  • Wood JM, Black AA, Hopkins S, White SLJ. Vision and academic performance in primary school children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2018;38(5):516–524. doi:10.1111/opo.12582
  • Northway N. Predicting the continued use of overlays in school children–a comparison of the Developmental Eye Movement test and the rate of reading test. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2003;23(5):457–464. doi:10.1046/j.1475-1313.2003.00144.x
  • Webber A, Wood J, Gole G, Brown B. DEM test, visagraph eye movement recordings, and reading ability in children. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:295–302. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e31820846c0
  • Tanke N, Barsingerhorn AD, Boonstra FN, Goossens J. Visual fixations rather than saccades dominate the developmental eye movement test. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1162. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-80870-5
  • Okumura T, Wakamiya E. Visual Skills in Children with Learning Difficulties. Tokyo, Japan: Meijitosho Shuppan Corporation; 2010.
  • Jimenez R, Gonzalez MD, Perez MA, Garcia JA. Evolution of accommodative function and development of ocular movements in children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2003;23:97–107. doi:10.1046/j.1475-1313.2003.00093.x
  • Baptista AM, de Sousa RA, de Morais Guerra Casal CC, Marques RJ, da Silva CM. Norms for the developmental eye movement test for portuguese children. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88:864–871. doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e3182195dae
  • Xie Y, Shi C, Tong M, et al. Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test norms for mandarin chinese-speaking chinese children. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0148481. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0148481
  • Moiroud L, Royo A, Bucci MP. The Developmental Eye Movement test in french children. Optom Vis Sci. 2020;97(11):978–983. doi:10.1097/OPX.0000000000001598
  • Facchin A, Maffioletti S. Comparison, within-session repeatability and normative data of three phoria tests. J Optom. 2020. doi:10.1016/j.optom.2020.05.007
  • Santiago HC, Perez MA Test-retest reliability of the developmental eye movement test. AAO Annual Meeting; 1992. Available from: https://www.aaopt.org/detail/knowledge-base-article/test-retest-reliability-developmental-eye-movement-test. Accessed February15, 2021.
  • Rouse MW, Nestor EM, Parot CJ, Deland PN. A reevaluation of the Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test’s repeatability. Optom Vis Sci. 2004;81:934–938.
  • Nguyen MQ, King D, Pearce AJ. A reliability and comparative analysis of the new randomized King-Devick test. J Neuro-Ophthalmol. 2020;40(2):207–212. doi:10.1097/WNO.0000000000000829
  • Subotic A, Ting WK-C, Cusimano MD. Characteristics of the King-Devick test in the assessment of concussed patients in the subacute and later stages after injury. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0183092. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0183092
  • Lawrence JB, Haider MN, Leddy JJ, Hinds A, Miecznikowski JC, Willer BS. The King-Devick test in an outpatient concussion clinic: assessing the diagnostic and prognostic value of a vision test in conjunction with exercise testing among acutely concussed adolescents. J Neurol Sci. 2019;398:91–97. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2018.12.020
  • Galetta KM, Liu M, Leong DF, Ventura RE, Galetta SL, Balcer LJ. The King-Devick test of rapid number naming for concussion detection: meta-analysis and systematic review of the literature. Concussion. 2016;1(2). doi:10.2217/cnc.15.8
  • Konynenbelt B, Harris P, Robles FP. Article 4 A comparison of performance on the NYSOA King-Devick Test between Mexican and American school-aged children. Optom Vis Performance. 2016;4(6):221–224.
  • Smaakjær P, Tødten ST, Rasmussen RS. Therapist-assisted vision therapy improves outcome for stroke patients with homonymous hemianopia alone or combined with oculomotor dysfunction. Neurol Res. 2018;40(9):752–757. doi:10.1080/01616412.2018.1475321
  • Piñero DP. Oculomotor dysfunctions: evidence-based practice. J Optom. 2020;13(3):137–138. doi:10.1016/j.optom.2020.06.001
  • Reddy AVC, Mani R, Selvakumar A, Hussaindeen JR. Reading eye movements in traumatic brain injury. J Optom. 2020;13(3):155–162. doi:10.1016/j.optom.2019.10.001
  • Scheiman M, Rouse MW. Optometric Management of Learning-Related Vision Problems. Elsevier Health Sciences; 2006.
  • Scheiman M, Wick B. Clinical Management of Binocular Vision: Heterophoric, Accommodative, and Eye Movement Disorders. 5th ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2019.
  • Facchin A, Giordano L, Brebbia G, Maffioletti S. Application, limits, scoring and improvements of Groffman Visual Tracing Test. Scand J Optom Vis Sci. 2020;13(1):2–9. doi:10.5384/sjovs.vol13i1p2-9