290
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Language and emotional abilities in children with Williams syndrome and children with autism spectrum disorder: similarities and differences

, &
Pages 89-97 | Published online: 22 Jul 2016

Abstract

Williams syndrome (WS) is a genetic disease with a relatively homogeneous profile: relatively well-preserved language, impaired cognitive activities, and hypersociability. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) refers to a group of individuals with impairments in aspects of communication and a particular pattern of language acquisition. Although ASD and WS are polar opposites when it comes to communication abilities (language and emotion) and social behavior, comparisons between WS and ASD are still rare in the literature. ASD and WS are both associated with general language and developmental delays. Difficulties in social interaction and general pragmatic difficulties are reported in both ASD and WS, but are more pervasive in ASD. Regarding facial emotion recognition, the two syndromes differ markedly in sensitivity to human faces. Despite the heterogeneity of these two groups, only a few studies with children have paid sufficient attention to participant recruitment and study design. A number of aspects need to be taken into account (eg, small age range, homogeneity of the subgroups, matching with typically developing children) if scientific results are to inform the design of intervention programs for children with neurodevelopmental disorders such as ASD and WS.

Introduction

Language and emotion are both processes whose main function is communication. They allow individuals to communicate about the nature of their intentions and their motivations, and thus, to regulate interpersonal relations. Their successful development and acquisition allow individuals to adopt appropriate behaviors in the society, and are intertwined with the emergence of certain cognitive abilities. Nevertheless, emotion is a complex and multifarious domain. It involves the production and comprehension of perceptual aspects, as well as linguistic ones. The comparison between Williams syndrome (WS) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) regarding emotions is heuristic, in the sense that these two neurodevelopmental disorders can be viewed as polar opposites in terms of sociability. Both have nonuniform cognitive profiles. Their comparison is, therefore, of both theoretical and applied (assessment and care) interest. People with WS are socially engaged and interested in others, even unfamiliar persons, and are particularly attracted to human faces, whereas individuals with ASD exhibit deficits in social interaction and prefer inanimate objects to human faces. The aim of the present paper is to highlight the similarities and differences in language and emotional abilities between WS and ASD. After giving a general description of emotional abilities and language in these two disorders, we focus on the studies that have compared WS and ASD.

Williams syndrome

WS is a rare genetic disease (1 in 20,000 births) caused by a microdeletion on chromosome 7 (7q11.23).Citation1Citation3 Persons with WS are characterized by a heart condition and a facial dysmorphology described as elfin. From the intellectual standpoint, individuals with WS usually have an intelligence quotient of between 50 and 70. Their unique neuropsychological profile has often been described as dissociation between cognition and language. Language is relatively well preserved, whereas cognition (ie, visuospatial abilities, executive functions, and number skills) is impaired.Citation4Citation6 Moreover, individuals with WS are characterized by hypersociability because of their ease in interacting with unfamiliar people.Citation7,Citation8

Regarding morphosyntax, problem areas include “grammatical gender”, which could be explained by the interaction of multiple factors, including attention deficit.Citation9,Citation10 Moreover, for conjugations and plurals, there is a dissociation between regular and irregular forms, in favor of regular ones.Citation11Citation18 Some recent studies have shown that while the development of morphosyntactic aspects is similar to that observed in typical development, there is a 1-year lag.Citation19Citation21 Researchers claim that mental retardation cannot entirely explain the poor morphosyntactic abilities that individuals with WS display in narrative tasks.Citation22,Citation23 Concerning syntactic abilities, some authorsCitation14,Citation24,Citation25 showed that children with WS perform well, but their syntactic abilities vary according to the context.Citation19,Citation26 Meanwhile, Volterra et alCitation27 highlighted a delay in acquisition, suggesting that the ability to listen to and store the sounds of words is intact in WS. This ability would allow them to acquire some of the more difficult aspects of language. For their part, Losh et alCitation22 and Reilly et alCitation23 observed that children with WS use a less complex syntax than typical children. Grant et alCitation28 recorded the same results in a repetition task.

In the lexical domain, Bellugi et alCitation24,Citation25,Citation29 and Bromberg et alCitation30,Citation31 described their semantics as unusual (use of rare words). The lexicon of adolescents and adults with WS is similar in quantity and diversity to that of typical individuals.Citation24,Citation25,Citation32 In 1997, Stevens and Karmiloff-SmithCitation33 showed that children with WS can achieve good lexical levels, even if they do not master all the lexical constraints. Tyler et alCitation34 highlighted several lexical–semantic deficits in WS, characterized by difficulty defining frequently used words and difficulty with lexical access. Moreover, Johnson and CareyCitation35 and Jarrold et alCitation36 showed that individuals with WS are able to enrich their semantic knowledge. For their part, Lukács et alCitation15,Citation16 and Temple et alCitation37 found that the profile of access to semantic networks is characterized by representations that are looser or only partially activated for naming and pointing. In 2003, Levy and BalcharCitation38 showed that in semantic and phonological fluency tasks, children with WS and their typical peers produce similar numbers of words. Nevertheless, LevyCitation14 found that the former produce more errors. Nazzi and Karmiloff-SmithCitation39 showed that children with WS do not attend to the role played by nouns in the formation of categories, observing dissociation between lexical acquisition and the development of name-based categorization.

The few studies that have examined the pragmatic aspects of language in individuals with WS have focused on communicative, narrative, or conversational skills.Citation2 Based on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,Citation40 Mervis et alCitation41 ranked the abilities of children with WS as follows: socialization (strongest point), communication, daily living skills, and motor abilities (weakest point). These abilities were found to improve with age. In Laws and Bishop’s study,Citation42 parents, teachers, and other professionals were asked to fill out the Children’s Communication Checklist for WS. Results indicated that the WS group had a pragmatic deficit marked by inappropriate initiation of interactions and stereotyped conversations. Jones et alCitation43,Citation44 claimed that inappropriate initiation is due to a lack of inhibition with respect to unknown persons. Mervis et alCitation41 and Laws and BishopCitation42 further highlighted the disparity between the relatively good ability to socialize and the difficulty of interacting appropriately with others. Lacroix et al,Citation2 Losh et al,Citation22 and Reilly et alCitation45 looked at social evaluation, which involves assessing the speaker’s subjective point of view with respect to the listener and the story. Children with WS were found to engage more in social evaluation (dialogues between characters and attributing affective states) than their typical peers. These results were replicated by Reilly et al.Citation46 Lacroix et alCitation2 showed that children and adolescents with WS have a unique profile of pragmatic abilities, accounting for the nonhomogeneity of narrative skills in WS (ie, a deficit in narrative structuring and excessive engagement in social evaluation, compatible with the hypersociability described in WS). Stojanovik et alCitation47 and StojanovikCitation48 found that some of the utterances produced by children were inappropriate, featuring semantic and syntactic problems. Despite their reputation for being highly talkative, they actually supplied very little information. Regarding turn-taking violations, there was no difference between the groups. Nevertheless, the replies given by the children with WS to adults’ requests were not well adapted. Thus, it seems that children with WS have relatively poor conversational skills.

People with WS are described as sociable, friendly, and empathic, and they exhibit considerable sociability, such as engaging with unfamiliar people. This positive social bias is thought to affect their perception of facial expressions of emotions, leading to more positive interpretations of them.Citation49 Some studies on the identification of emotionsCitation50Citation52 have indicated that children with WS perform similarly to typical children. However, Jones et alCitation43,Citation44 and Plesa-Skewer et alCitation53 found that whereas a WS group performed just as well as typical controls on happiness, children of the WS group performed less well on negative emotions. The same results were observed for the revised Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task.Citation54 Facial emotion recognition is, therefore, not a spared ability in WS, unlike facial identity recognition. When Porter et alCitation55 studied the recognition of facial expressions of emotion in individuals with WS, they found that negative emotions were often identified as positive. The authors attributed this result to the positive social bias arising from a sociable personality–Doyle et alCitation7 had previously shown that children with WS are judged to be more sociable than typical children. In the Benton Facial Recognition Test,Citation56 children with WS have no difficulty matching two identical faces expressing distinct emotions.Citation24,Citation25,Citation50,Citation52 By contrast, several studies have highlighted a developmental lag in facial recognition in WS.Citation51,Citation57 Moreover, researchers have suggested that face processing is abnormal in WS, owing to the use of a local process rather than a global one. More specifically, Tager-Flusberg et alCitation58 showed that persons with WS have an advantage when it comes to processing complete faces when they are normally oriented, but not when they are inverted.

Regarding emotion production abilities, Jones et alCitation43,Citation44 found that children with WS less frequently produced negative facial expressions. Moreover, their expressions of vocal and facial distress and negativity were less intense than those of typical controls. When they were reunited with their parents, the frequency and intensity of their positive expressions were the same as those in the two control groups. Furthermore, when children with WS were shown a toy behind a barrier in a warm-up task, they tended to excessively engage the examiner through eye contact and smiling, compared with typically developing children. Regarding emotion regulation abilities, PhillipsCitation59 carried out a study including 37 children and adolescents with WS, who were aged 8–15 years. Results indicated that most of these participants had difficulty regulating their emotions. Moreover, the author showed that their emotion regulation difficulties were correlated with measures of adaptive behavior. The high level of emotion regulation difficulties was more likely to be interpreted by their parents as internalizing and externalizing behavior problems.

Autism spectrum disorder

ASD refers to a group of neurodevelopmental disorders marked by impairments in social interaction and communication, and repetitive and stereotypical behavior, which are generally evident before 3 years of age, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition.Citation60 Although impairments in aspects of communication are considered to be one of the core deficits in ASD, less is known about the structural aspects of language acquisition in ASD.Citation61Citation66 Because of this disorder’s variability of expression, there is also considerable variation in language skills among individuals with ASD. Impaired language use is one of the earliest symptoms that alerts parents to their children’s development.Citation65,Citation67 Moreover, ASD is classically characterized by impairments in the processing of social and emotional information, responses to the emotional displays of others, and face recognition.Citation49 In ASD, several language impairments have been described, such as developmental delay, echolalia, and pragmatic difficulties.

Individuals with ASD display deficits in communication, pragmatics, and social aspects of language.Citation62 However, the same is not true for grammar. It has been argued that lower functioning individuals are more at risk of deficient grammar than higher functioning individuals so much so that “ language impairment and intellectual disability almost always occur together when associated with autism”.Citation61 In children with Asperger syndrome, grammar has been reported to be mostly spared.Citation61 Following Tager-Flusberg et al,Citation65 it is now common in the literature to identify two major subgroups within the ASD population: children with ASD whose performances on standardized language assessments are in the impaired range and children with ASD whose language seems normal on standardized measures.

Studies on morphology in children with ASD are rare. Nevertheless, some research has demonstrated that grammar is relatively intact. In a longitudinal study of six children with ASD aged 3-7 years, Tager-Flusberg et alCitation66 observed that they followed the same developmental pattern as typical children, in terms of increases in the mean length of utterance. For their part, Naigles et alCitation68 demonstrated that preschoolers with ASD have levels comparable to those of language-matched typically developing children, when it comes to understanding some syntactic–semantic linkages. Other studies, however, have reported atypical morphosyntax in children with ASD. Bartolucci et alCitation69 found that children with ASD were more likely than children in other groups to omit certain morphemes (articles, auxiliary forms, past tense). They also found that these morphemes emerged later in the speech of children with ASD. This finding was replicated by Howlin.Citation70 More recently, Eigsti et alCitation62 investigated spontaneous speech during free play in 3- to 6-year-old children with ASD. These authors found that the children with ASD produced few syntactically complex utterances – a finding confirmed by Park et al.Citation64 In ASD, morphology is acknowledged to be deficient, especially finiteness marking.Citation71,Citation72

The few experimental studies that have investigated complex syntactic structures have highlighted difficulty with passives,Citation73,Citation74 relative clauses,Citation75 raising,Citation73 and subject control structures.

Some studies, however, have reported intact lexical/semantic skills in ASD. Vocabularies increase steadily with age, but are composed primarily of nouns.Citation66,Citation76,Citation77 Even though young children with ASD may not rely on the same lexical learning mechanisms as typical children, they can still acquire a sizable vocabulary.Citation78Citation80 Mental state terms have also been shown to be underrepresented in conversations, suggesting that vocabulary use by children with ASD may be deficient, compared with that of typical children.Citation81

Pragmatic skills are generally considered to be impaired in ASD. Rutter et alCitation82 found that children with ASD tend to use formal words, suggesting a lack of experience with peer interactions and a preference for interacting with adults. VoldenCitation83 showed that they are able to respond to failures of communication, but produce inappropriate responses. This last finding is consistent with earlier ones.Citation84 In narrative situations, children with ASD have difficulty understanding why a character feels a particular internal state.Citation85

Interpreting the results of studies in ASD is a complex business, as it depends on the methodology and the homogeneity of the sample. As mentioned by Lacroix et al,Citation49 ASD is often characterized by deficits in the processing of social and emotional information,Citation86Citation88 responses to the emotional displays of others,Citation89 and face recognition.Citation90Citation92 More specifically, children with ASD perform better for simple emotions related to external situations (happiness and sadness) than for emotions related to internal cognition, such as surprise,Citation93Citation95 and they are less sensitive to negative emotions.Citation89 People with ASD have difficulty recognizing, identifying, and understanding the significance of emotions.Citation96Citation98 Recently, CastelliCitation99 claimed that children with ASD are just as capable of identifying emotions from facial expressions as typically developing children. By contrast, Lindner and RosenCitation100 reported that children with Asperger syndrome find it harder than typically developing children to identify emotions in static facial expressions, dynamic facial expressions, and tones of voice. Losh et alCitation63 showed that high-functioning people with autism, aged between 16 and 27 years, had difficulties to identify fear, but not with happiness and sadness. More recently, Lacroix et alCitation101 showed that children with ASD, aged 4-8 years, had greater difficulty labeling emotions, though not with matching or identifying them, than typically developing children matched on either chronological or verbal mental age. Happiness was the easiest one to recognize, and surprise the hardest. The children with ASD did not exhibit any delayed onset in the development of facial emotion recognition. Emotion recognition difficulties in children with ASD, therefore, are primarily concerned with the recognition of negative emotions and the identification of surprise, just as they do in typically developing groups.

Comparisons between WS and ASD

Few studies have compared the two developmental disorders, and we only found one study for language abilities. Philofsky et alCitation102 investigated the pragmatic language profiles of children with ASD and children with WS. They administered the Children’s Communication Checklist-2Citation103 to 22 parents of children with ASD aged between 7 years and 1 month, and 12 years and 10 months, 21 parents of children with WS aged between 6 years and 2 months, and 12 years and 5 months, and 19 parents of typically developing children aged between 5 years and 10 months, and 10 years. Results indicated similar levels of pragmatic impairment in the ASD and WS groups, especially for the Inappropriate Initiation, Use of Context, and Interests scales. However, the children with WS performed significantly better than those with ASD on the Coherence, Stereotyped Language, Nonverbal Communication, and Social Relations scales. The children with WS had several areas of relative strength, compared with the children with ASD, who exhibited impaired social pragmatic functioning. The children with WS were given significantly higher ratings by their parents than the children with ASD on several items referring to the use and understanding of affective expressions, empathy, and social relationships. Smaller, but still significant, differences were noted in favor of the children with WS (over ASD), regarding their ability to appropriately sequence and reference events for a listener. Both clinical groups displayed impairment in the quality of their initiations with others and in the overall quality and variety of their interests.

This kind of study has implications for speech-language pathologists, educators, and other professionals working with children who have these disorders. The investigation of pragmatic functioning in both groups allows for a richer understanding of what constitutes atypical pragmatic language functioning in children.

Studies of emotions comparing WS and ASD are more frequent, but still few and far between. Rose et alCitation104 investigated the facial discrimination abilities of 19 participants with WS and 16 with ASD in neutral versus affective expression and upright versus inverted face conditions. Participants with WS performed at the same level as both the ASD and typical groups in the recognition of upright faces with neutral expressions. However, although the participants with WS and those with ASD both used a featural strategy to process the faces, their performances differed in the inverted neutral condition. Individuals with WS and ASD may not, therefore, process faces in an identical manner. Moreover, data revealed a strong inversion effect in the WS group relative to both the ASD and typical control groups, suggesting that individuals with WS process faces holistically, even though studies have frequently reported a local bias in visual processing. The WS group performed better than the ASD group when the emotional expression varied between stimulus and target.

Riby and HancockCitation105 used eyetracking techniques to investigate how individuals with ASD and WS view scenes containing people. Participants were 18 young individuals with WS, aged between 8 years 9 months, and 28 years, and 20 young individuals with ASD, aged between 6 years and 4 months, and 18 years 4 months. They were shown 20 photographs, and the authors explored their gaze behavior. Results indicated that the two groups differed both from each other and from the typically developing controls. Participants with ASD and WS were found to have a number of atypical features, but a different set for each disorder. Those with WS fixated the face, especially the eyes, for a long time, but this was not the case for those with ASD. This difference indicates that people with WS are better able to interpret gaze cues and expressions than those with ASD. In WS, the cause of hypersociability is still subject to debate. Results of Porter et alCitation55 support the explanation whereby behavior is poorly controlled owing to frontal lobe anomalies, while Riby and HancockCitation105 aver that individuals with WS display prolonged eye contact because of an inability to direct their gaze appropriately or difficulty understanding social rules. Both ASD and WS are associated with atypicalities of the social brain.Citation106 Moreover, Riby et alCitation105 reported that individuals with ASD present deficits when they need to use the eye region, whereas people with WS perform better when they use the eye rather than the mouth region.

In 2009, Lacroix et alCitation49 compared the two disorders on the ability to recognize emotional and nonemotional facial expressions. Participants with WS were aged between 6 years 1 month, and 15 years, and participants with ASD were aged between 4 years 9 months, and 8 years. Results failed to indicate any difference among WS, ASD, and typically developing groups, matched on verbal mental age, in performances on matching and labeling tasks. Poorer performances were, however, observed in the WS group on the identification task. Moreover, language partially facilitated the recognition of emotion, depending on the nature of the emotion. In 2009, Annaz et alCitation107 conducted a cross-syndrome study investigating the development of holistic face recognition between ages 5 and 12 years in typically developing children (n=25) and in three disorders: ASD (n=33), Down syndrome (n=15), and WS (n=15). A target face was displayed on a screen, above two stimuli: either whole faces (whole-face discrimination) or isolated facial features (part-face discrimination). Participants had to say which stimulus was the same as the target face. People with WS and ASD performed better on the part-face discrimination task than on the whole-face one. More specifically, the participants with WS seemed to process faces at the level of facial features such as the nose, mouth, and eyes, whereas participants with ASD, and more specifically high-functioning autism, relied on more fine-grained visual details.

Conclusion

ASD and WS are polar opposites when it comes to communication abilities (language and emotion) and social behavior. This is why it is interesting to compare these two neurodevelopmental disorders, although such comparisons are still rare in the literature. Concerning language abilities, language delay and poor intellectual abilities do not seem to play a major role in the grammatical development of either of the populations we investigated here, even though both ASD and WS are associated with general language and developmental lags. Difficulties with social interaction and general pragmatic difficulties are reported in both ASD and WS, but are more pervasive in ASD. These difficulties can be explained by a lack of inhibition in WS, but not in ASD. The deficits in social abilities observed in ASD and WS do not have the same origins or the same consequences. Using facial emotion recognition tasks, several studiesCitation104,Citation108 have highlighted the contrasting social features of the two syndromes: individuals with WS are reported to have better social skills than individuals with ASD. However, Lacroix et alCitation49 showed that children with WS have greater difficulty identifying emotions such as fear and sadness than children with ASD or typically developing children matched for verbal mental age. This could have something to do with their indiscriminate approach to strangers, relative absence of social anxiety, and poor ability to recognize facial expressions of emotion.Citation109 The deficits observed in ASD for facial emotion recognition must be linked to their poor visual strategies and their lexical and pragmatic language difficulties. We did not find any reports of severe lexical or semantic deficits among participants with ASD, except regarding emotional vocabulary. We did, however, find pragmatic impairments. The deficits observed in ASD in the recognition of facial emotions, especially when language is involved in the task, could be due to the fact that people with ASD do not understand the meaning of emotions. Moreover, they have a specific way of exploring the scene and the faces to extract the meaning of the situation. The social difficulties observed in WS stem from other deficits, namely visuospatial deficits, because these children focus their attention primarily on the eyes. Furthermore, their lack of inhibition leads them to exhibit a positive social bias, preferring positive emotions to negative ones.

Given the heterogeneity of these two groups of children, and the need to find out more about their language and emotional abilities, we still lack studies that pay sufficient attention to participant recruitment (ie, small age range, homogeneity of the subgroups, matching with typically developing children, etc) and study design. Language and emotion are both complex abilities, as we have seen, and studies are often not comparable because of the different methodologies they use (stimuli, procedures, etc). Language can refer to morphology, syntax, pragmatics, and so on, while facial expression of emotions can refer to identifying, labeling, or matching, and may or may not involve language. Other aspects of emotion can refer to regulation and, more broadly, to theory of mind. Further studies will need to take all these aspects into account if we want scientific results to inform the design and assessment of intervention programs for children with ASD, WS, and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Leyfer et alCitation110 demonstrated that 7% of children with WS meet the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) criteria for ASD. Nevertheless, the particularities behind criteria such as social/communicative impairment are clearly different. It is important to specify the profile of children with WS and ASD as precisely as possible. A better understanding of the characteristics of emotional and language abilities would be useful for developing the right intervention tools.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • BellugiULichtenbergerLJonesWLaiZGeorgeMStThe neurocognitive profile of Williams’ syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. In: Bellugi U, St George M, editors. Linking cognitive neuroscience and molecular genetics: New perspectives from Williams syndrome [Special Issue]J Cognitive Neurosci2000121129
  • LacroixABernicotJReillyJNarration and collaborative conversation in French-speaking children with Williams syndromeJ Neurolinguistics2007206445461
  • SiegmüllerJBartkeSWilliams syndrome from a clinical perspectiveBartkeSSiegmüllerJWilliams Syndrome Across LanguagesAmsterdamJohn Benjamins2004937
  • Karmiloff-SmithAThomasMAnnazDExploring the Williams syndrome face processing debate: the importance of building developmental trajectoriesJ Child Psychol Psychiatry20044571258127415335346
  • KleinBPMervisCBContrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndromeDev Neuropsychol1999162177196
  • PezziniGVicariSVolterraVMilaniLOssellaMTChildren with Williams’s syndrome: Is there a single neuropsychological profile?Dev Neuropsychol199915141155
  • DoyleTFBellugiUKorenbergJRGrahamJ“Everybody in the world is my friend” hypersociability in young children with Williams’ syndromeAm J Med Genet A2003124253263
  • FrigerioEBurtDMGagliardiCIs everybody always my friend? Perception of approachability in Williams syndromeNeuropsychologia200644225425916005478
  • Karmiloff-SmithAGrantJBerthoudIDaviesMHowlinPUdwinOLanguage and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”?Child Dev1997682462629180000
  • MonnerySSeigneuricAZagarDRobichonFA linguistic dissociation in Williams syndrome: good at gender agreement but poor at lexical retrievalRead Writ200215589612
  • ClahsenHRingMTempleCLexical and morphological skills in English-speaking children with Williams’ syndromeBartkeSSiegmüllerJWilliams’ Syndrome Across LanguagesAmsterdamJohn Benjamins2004221244
  • JoffeVVarlokostaSLanguage abilities in Williams syndrome: exploring comprehension, production and repetition skillsAdv Speech Lang Pathol200793213225
  • KrauseMPenkeMInflectional morphology in German Williams syndromeBrain Cogn20024841041212030478
  • LevyYA longitudinal study of language development in children with Williams syndromeJ Child Lang2004312298310
  • LukácsAPléhCRacsmányMLanguage in Hungarian children with Williams syndromeBartkeSSiegmüllerJWilliams Syndrome Across LanguagesAmsterdamJohn Benjamins2004187221
  • LukácsARacsmányMPléhCVocabulary and morphological patterns in Hungarian children with Williams syndrome: a preliminary reportActa Linguistica200148243269
  • PenkeMKrauseMRegular and irregular inflectional morphology in German Williams syndromeBartkeSSiegmüllerJWilliams Syndrome Across LanguagesAmsterdamJohn Benjamins2004244271
  • ThomasMSCGrantJBarhamZPast tense formation in Williams syndromeLanguage Cognitive Proc2001162–3143176
  • CapirciOSabbadiniLVolterraVLanguage development in Williams’ syndrome: a case studyCognitive Neuropsychol199613710171039
  • LukácsAPléhCRacsmányMSpatial language in Williams syndrome: evidence for a special interaction?J Child Lang200734231134317542160
  • VolterraVCapirciOPezziniGSabbadiniLVicariSLinguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndromeCortex1996326636778954245
  • LoshMBellugiUReillyJAndersonDThe integrity and independence of evaluation in narratives: evidence from children with Williams syndromeNarrat Inq2001102126
  • ReillyJLoshMBellugiUWulfeckBFrog, where are you?: Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams syndromeBrain Lang20048822924714965544
  • BellugiUMarksSBihrleASaboHDissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndromeBishopDMogfordKLanguage Development in Exceptional CircumstancesLondonChurchill Livingstone1988171189
  • BellugiUSaboHVaidJSpatial deficit in children with Williams syndromeStiles-DavisJKritchevskyMBellugiUSpatial Cognition: Brain Bases and DevelopmentHillsdale, NJLawrence Erlbaum1988273298
  • VicariSCaselliMCGagliardiCTonucciFVolterraVLanguage acquisition in special populations: a comparison between Down and Williams syndromesNeuropsychologia2002402461247012417473
  • VolterraVCaselliMCCapirciOTonucciFVicariSEarly linguistic abilities of Italian children with Williams syndromeDev Neuropsychol2003131–23359
  • GrantJValianVKarmiloff-SmithAA study of relative clauses in Williams syndromeJ Child Lang200229430416
  • BellugiUBihrleAJerniganTTraunerDDohertySNeuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams’ syndromeAm J Med Genet Suppl199061151252144426
  • BrombergHSUllmanMMarcusGKelleyKBLevineKA dissociation of lexical memory and grammar in Williams’ syndrome: evidence from inflectional morphologyPoster presented at: The Sixth International Professional Conference of the Williams Syndrome AssociationUniversity of CaliforniaSan Diego1994
  • BrombergHUllmanMMarcusGKellyKCoppolaMA dissociation of memory and grammar: evidence from Williams syndromePaper presented at: The 18th Annual Boston University Conference on Language DevelopmentBoston1994
  • StevensTLexical constraints on language acquisition in Williams syndrome [PhD Thesis]MRC Cognitive Development Unit and University CollegeLondon1996
  • StevensTKarmiloff-SmithAWord learning in a special population: do individuals with WS obey lexical constraints?J Child Lang1997247377659519593
  • TylerLKarmiloff-SmithAVoiceJKDo individuals with Williams syndrome have bizarre semantics? Evidence for lexical organization using an on-line taskCortex1997335155279339332
  • JohnsonSCareySKnowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folk biology: evidence from Williams syndromeCogn Psychol1998371561849878105
  • JarroldCHartleySJPhillipsCBaddeleyADWord fluency in Williams syndrome: evidence for unusual semantic organization?Cogn Neuropsychiatry200054293319
  • TempleCMAlmazanMSherwoodSLexical skills in Williams syndrome: a cognitive neuropsychological analysisJ Neurolinguist200215463495
  • LevyYBalcharTCognitive, lexical and morphosyntactic profiles of Israeli children with Williams syndromeCortex20033925527112784888
  • NazziTKarmiloff-SmithAEarly categorization abilities in young children with Williams syndromeNeuroreport200213101259126212151782
  • SparrowSSBallaDCicchettiDVineland Adaptive Behavior ScalesCircle Pines, MNAmerican Guidance Service1984
  • MervisCBKlein-TasmanBPMastinMEAdaptive behaviour of 4-through 8-year children with Williams syndromeAm J Ment Retard20011064829311246716
  • LawsGBishopVMPragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairmentInt J Lang Commun Disord20043914564
  • JonesWBellugiULaiZHypersociability in Williams syndromeJ Cognitive Sci2000123047
  • JonesWBellugiULaiZHypersociability in Williams syndrome. In: Bellugi U, St George M, editors. Linking cognitive neuroscience and molecular genetics: new perspectives from Williams syndrome [Special Issue]J Cognitive Neurosci20001213046
  • ReillyJLacroixAPoirierJBernicotJBellugiUKlimaENarratives in French and American children with Williams syndromeLe Langage et l’Homme2005402111126
  • ReillyJBernicotJVicariSLacroixABellugiUNarratives in children with Williams syndrome: a cross-linguistic perspectiveRavidDBatzeev ShyldkrotHPerspectives on Language and Language Development Essays in Honor of Ruth A BermanDordrecht, NetherlandsKluwer2004303312
  • StojanovikVPerkinsMHowardSLanguage and conversational abilities in Williams syndrome: how good is good?Int J Lang Commun Disord20013623423911340788
  • StojanovikVSocial interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndromeJ Neurolinguist2006192157173
  • LacroixAGuidettiMRogéBReillyJRecognition of emotional and nonemotional facial expressions: a comparison between Williams syndrome and autismRes Dev Disabil20093097698519286347
  • BellugiUWangPJerniganTWilliams syndrome: an unusual neuropsychological profileBromanSGrafmanJAtypical Cognitive Deficits in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Implications for Brain FunctionHillsdale, NJLawrence Erlbaum19942356
  • GagliardiCFrigerioEBurtDMCazzanigaIPerrettDIBorgattiRFacial expression recognition in Williams syndromeNeuropsychologia20034173373812591030
  • WangPPDohertySRourkeSBBellugiUUnique profile of visuoperceptual skills in a genetic syndromeBrain Cogn199529154658845123
  • Plesa-SkewerDFajaSVerbalisASchofieldCTager-FlusbergHPerceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in Williams syndromeAm J Ment Retard20061111152616332153
  • SkwererDPVerbalisASchofieldCFajaSTager-FlusbergHSocial-perceptual abilities in adolescents and adults with Williams syndromeCogn Neuropsychol200623233834921049334
  • PorterMAColtheartMLangdonRThe neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndromeNeuropsychologia2007452839284917597166
  • BentonALSivanABHamsherKDVarneyNRSpreenOContributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical ManualNew YorkOxford University Press1983
  • DeruelleCManciniJLivetMOCassé-PerrotCde SchonenSConfigural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndromeBrain Cogn19994127629810585239
  • Tager-FlusbergHPlesa-SkwererDFajaSJosephRMPeople with Williams syndrome process faces holisticallyCognition2003891112312893122
  • PhillipsKDEmotion Regulation and Dysregulation in Children and Adolescents with Williams Syndrome (doctoral dissertation) Retrieved from ProQuest (UMI: 3332181)2008
  • American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders5th edArlington, VAAmerican Psychiatric Publishing2013
  • BoucherJThe Autistic Spectrum: Characteristics, Causes and Practical IssuesLondonSage2009
  • EigstiIMBennettoLDadlaniMBeyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autismJ Autism Dev Disord2007371007102317089196
  • LoshMAdolphsRPoeMDNeuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotypeArch Gen Psychiatry200966551852619414711
  • ParkSChoSCChoIHSex differences in children with autism spectrum disorders compared with their unaffected siblings and typically developing childrenRes Autism Spect Disord20126861870
  • Tager-FlusbergHPaulRLordCELanguage and communication in autismVolkmarFPaulRKlinACohenDJHandbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Vol 1. Diagnosis, Development, Neurobiology, and BehaviorNew YorkWiley2005335364
  • Tager-FlusbergHCalkinsSNolinTBaumbergerTAndersonMChadwick-DiasAA longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome childrenJ Autism Dev Disord1990201212139024
  • SzatmariPBrysonSDukuESimilar developmental trajectories in autism and Asperger syndrome: from early childhood to adolescenceJ Child Psychol Psychiatry2009501459146719686332
  • NaiglesLKeltyEJafferyRFeinDAbstractness and continuity in the syntactic development of young children with autismAutism Res2011442243722012625
  • BartolucciGPierceSJStreinerDCross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded childrenJ Autism Dev Disord19801039506927677
  • HowlinPThe acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: a critique and replication of the findings of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980J Autism Dev Disord1984141271366746504
  • KjelgaardMTager-FlusbergHAn investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroupsLang Cogn Process20011628730816703115
  • RobertsJARiceMLTager-FlusbergHTense marking in children with autismAppl Psycholinguist2004253429448
  • PerovicAModyanovaNWexlerKKnowledge of c-command and A-movement in children and adolescents with autism and with Asperger syndromePaper presented at: GALA; 2007Generative Approaches to Language AcquisitionBarcelona, Spain2007
  • Tager-FlusbergHOn the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autismJ Autism Dev Disord19811145566927698
  • RichesNGLoucasTBairdGCharmanTSimonoffESentence repetition in adolescents with specific language impairments and autism: an investigation of complex syntaxInt J Lang Commun Disord2010451476019343567
  • FeinDDunnMAAllenDMNeuropsychological and language findingsRapinIPreschool Children with Inadequate Communication: Developmental Language Disorder, Autism, Low IQLondonMacKeith Press1996123154
  • SwensenLDKelleyEFeinDNaiglesLRProcesses of language acquisition in children with autism: evidence from preferential lookingChild Dev20077854255717381789
  • GastgebHZStraussMSMinshewNJDo individuals with autism process categories differently? The effect of typicality and developmentChild Dev2006771717172917107456
  • KelleyEPaulJFeinDNaiglesLResidual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autismJ Autism Dev Disord20063680782816897404
  • TekSJafferyGFeinDNaiglesLDo children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning?Autism Res2008120822219360671
  • Baron-CohenSRingHMoriartyJShmitzPCostaDEllPRecognition of mental state terms: a clinical study of autism, and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adultsBr J Psychiatry19941656406497866679
  • RutterMMawhoodLHowlinPLanguage delay and social developmentFletcherPHallDSpecific Speech and Language Disorders in Children: Correlates, Characteristics and OutcomesLondonWhurr19926378
  • VoldenJConversational repair in speakers with autism spectrum disordersInt J Lang Commun Disord20043917118915204450
  • CappsLKehresJSigmanMConversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delaysAutism19982325344
  • CappsLLoshMThurberC“The frog ate a bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autismJ Abnorm Child Psychol20002819320410834770
  • Baron-CohenSTager-FlusbergHCohenDJUnderstanding Other Minds: Perspectives from AutismOxfordOxford University Press1993
  • DawsonGMeltzoffANOsterlingJRinaldiJBrownEChildren with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuliJ Autism Dev Disord1998284794859932234
  • TeunisseJPDe GelderBDo autistic children have a generalized face processing deficit?Int J Neurosci1994771107989155
  • SigmanMDKasariCKwonJHYirmiyaNResponses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal childrenChild Dev1992637968071505241
  • DawsonGCarverLMeltzoffAPanagiotidesHMcPartlandJWebbSNeural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical developmentChild Dev20027370071712038546
  • KlinASparrowSSde BildtACicchettiDVCohenDJVolkmarFRA normed study of face recognition in autism and related disordersJ Autism Dev Disord19992949950810638462
  • NarzisiAMuratoriFCalderoniSFabbroFUrgesiCNeuropsychological profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disordersJ Autism Dev Disord2013431895190923224514
  • Baron-CohenSSpitzACrossPDo children with autism recognize surprise? A research noteCogn Emot19937507516
  • CappsLYirmiyaNSigmanMUnderstanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autismJ Child Psychol Psychiatry199233116911821400699
  • YirmiyaNSigmanMDKasariCMundyPEmpathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autismChild Dev1992631501601551323
  • CelaniGBattacchiMWArcidiaconoLThe understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autismJ Autism Dev Disord199929576610097995
  • HobsonRPOustonJLeeAWhat’s in a face? The case of autismBr J Psychol1988794414533208000
  • LovelandKATunali-KotoskiBChenRBrelsfordKAOrtegonJPearsonDAIntermodal perception of affect in persons with autism or Down syndromeDev Psychopathol19957409418
  • CastelliFUnderstanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal developmentAutism2005942844916155058
  • LindnerJLRosenLADecoding of emotion through facial expression, prosody, and verbal content in children and adolescents with Asperger’s syndromeJ Autism Dev Disord20063676977716639533
  • LacroixAGuidettiMRogéBReillyJFacial emotion recognition in 4- to 8-year-olds with autism spectrum disorder: a developmental trajectory approachRes Autism Spect Disord20148911461154
  • PhilofskyAFidlerDJHepburnSLPragmatic language profiles of school-aged children with autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndromeAm J Speech Lang Pathol20071636838017971496
  • BishopDVMThe Children’s Communication Checklist, Version 2 (CCC-2)LondonPsychological Corporation2003
  • RoseFELincolnAJLaiZEneMSearcyYMBellugiUOrientation and affective expression effects on face recognition in Williams syndrome and autismJ Autism Dev Disord20073751352216906460
  • RibyDMHancockPJBViewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autismNeuropsychologia2008462855286018561959
  • JohnsonMHThe ontogeny of the social brainMayrUAwhEKeeleSWDeveloping individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael PosnerWashington DCAPA press2005125140
  • AnnazDKarmiloff-SmithAJohnsonMHThomasMSCA cross-syndrome study of the development of holistic face recognition in children with autism, Down syndrome and Williams syndromeJ Exp Child Psychol200910245648619193384
  • RibyDMDoherty-SneddonGBruceVThe eyes or the mouth? Feature salience and unfamiliar face processing in Williams syndrome and autismQ J Exp Psychol2009621189203
  • RibyDMDoherty-SneddonGBruceVExploring face perception in disorders of development: evidence from Williams syndrome and autismJ Neuropsychol20082476419334304
  • LeyferOTWoodruff-BordenJKlein-TasmanBFrickeJMervisCBPrevalence of psychiatric disorders in 4–16-year olds with Williams syndromeAm J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet2006114B615622