5,506
Views
31
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Revisiting the etiological aspects of dissociative identity disorder: a biopsychosocial perspective

, &
Pages 137-146 | Published online: 02 May 2017

Abstract

Dissociative identity disorder (DID) is a chronic post-traumatic disorder where developmentally stressful events in childhood, including abuse, emotional neglect, disturbed attachment, and boundary violations are central and typical etiological factors. Familial, societal, and cultural factors may give rise to the trauma and/or they may influence the expression of DID. Memory and the construction of self-identity are cognitive processes that appear markedly and centrally disrupted in DID and are related to its etiology. Enduring decoupling of psychological modes may create separate senses of self, and metamemory processes may be involved in interidentity amnesia. Neurobiological differences have been demonstrated between dissociative identities within patients with DID and between patients with DID and controls. Given the current evidence, DID as a diagnostic entity cannot be explained as a phenomenon created by iatrogenic influences, suggestibility, malingering, or social role-taking. On the contrary, DID is an empirically robust chronic psychiatric disorder based on neurobiological, cognitive, and interpersonal non-integration as a response to unbearable stress. While current evidence is sufficient to firmly establish this etiological stance, given the wide opportunities for innovative research, the disorder is still understudied. Comparison of well-selected samples of DID patients with non-dissociative subjects who have other psychiatric disorders would further delineate the neurobiological and cognitive features of the disorder, whereas genetic research on DID would further illuminate the interaction of the individual with environmental stress. As such, DID may be seen as an exemplary disease model of the biopsychosocial paradigm in psychiatry.

Introduction

Dissociative identity disorder (DID) is multifactorial in its etiology. Whereas psychosocial etiologies of DID include developmental traumatization and sociocognitive sequelae, biological factors include trauma-generated neurobiological responses. Biologically derived traits and epigenetic mechanisms are also likely to be at play. At this point, no direct examination of genetics has occurred in DID. However, it is likely to exist, given the genetic link to dissociation in general and in relation to childhood adversity in particular.Citation1,Citation2

Studies conducted in various countries led to a consensus about prevalences of DID:Citation3 5% among psychiatric inpatients, 2–3% among outpatients, and 1% in the general population. Prevalences appear heightened among adolescent psychiatric outpatients and in the psychiatric emergency unit.Citation4,Citation5 The latter points to the acute transient crisis situations that may be superimposed on an underlying DID. Such crises usually serve as a diagnostic window for the clinician in conditions when core symptoms of DID remain dormant until a stressful event triggers a more prominent manifestation.

Self-report instruments such as the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES),Citation6 the Dissociation Questionnaire (DIS-Q),Citation7 and the Multidimensional Inventory of Dissociation (MID)Citation8 serve as potential screening measures for patients with DID. Clinician-administered instruments such as the Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule (DDIS)Citation9 and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D)Citation10 guide the clinician in making a firm diagnosis of DID.

DID is a stable diagnostic category when the primary components of the disorder are considered: the existence of dissociative identities, confusion about and alterations between them, amnesia, and experiences of depersonalization and derealization.Citation11 The foundation of dissociative identities is thought to be divisions or dissociations within the individual at the level of identity or the personality. Dissociation or a breakdown of integration between psychobiological aspects and systems that make up the totality of the person’s functioning can occur at the level of sensation (e.g., temporary/functional blindness), cognition (e.g., amnesia), affect (e.g., numbing), behavior (e.g., temporary/functional paralysis), and consciousness (e.g., ego-observing depersonalization), among others.Citation12

Yet, when all these systems come together to underpin and maintain a person’s identity, and dissociation occurs at this (identity/personality) level, it creates dissociative identities. Here, separate organized systems of functioning, with their own unique perspective on the world and who they are, appear to co-exist within the individual.Citation13 Each of these identities has their own first-person perspective or experience of self-consciousness.Citation14Citation16 Consequently, each of these identities reports their own subjective experiences and memories, their own sense of agency and will, and their own perspective on who they are.Citation14 They often report being unaware of other identities or report amnesia for experiences that presumably occurred when other identities were engaging in executive actions.

This paper examines the etiological factors of DID, setting them around the current most widely accepted primary driver of the psychopathology: developmental traumatization. Secondarily, family and sociocultural factors and further cognitive and neurobiological disturbances emerge in tandem with this primary factor. These etiological factors have also been the subject of an accumulating body of research in the field of dissociation in general and dissociative disorders other than DID. However, in this paper, only studies directly addressing DID have been considered for examination.

Developmental traumatization

DID is currently understood as a chronic complex post-traumatic developmental disorder where adverse experiences usually begin in early childhood and in which the dissociative identities result from the child’s inability to develop and maintain a unified sense of self across various discrete behavioral states.Citation17Citation23 (See also the literature referred to in the next paragraph.) Identity alterations observed in DID may also be considered as an elaborated version of trauma-related mental intrusions and avoidance that corresponds to the basic mechanism of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).Citation24Citation26 While the dynamics are similar, DID has discrete identities with their own first-person perspective (multiple “I” selves) and breaks in consciousness between these identities, which do not occur in PTSD.Citation4

More severe and earlier-onset child abuse appears to differentiate DID from other disorders.Citation27,Citation28 Large-scale clinical and epidemiological studies in the United States, Australia, Turkey, Puerto Rico, the Netherlands, Germany, and Canada have consistently found that DID is linked to antecedent chronic abusive experiences in childhood, typically at the hands of an attachment figure.Citation29Citation31 A large representative sample of women from the general population in Turkey (N=994) was evaluated in three stages: after completing a self-report measure of dissociation, two groups of participants with high and low scores were administered the DDIS by a researcher blind to scores, followed by blinded clinical examination. The researchers were able to identify four cases of DID, all of whom reported childhood abuse and/or neglect.Citation32

Dalenberg et alCitation20 calculated Ross and Ness’Citation33 comparison of physical and sexual abuse in DID patients and controls, finding effect sizes of 0.74–0.78. By using corroborating documentation from hospital, police, and child protection agencies or witnesses, several studies have confirmed histories of severe abuse in DID.Citation29,Citation34,Citation35 In most clinical series, childhood abuse and/or neglect is reported by 90–100% of the patients directly during the study examination.Citation36,Citation37 Dissociative amnesia for childhood events may prevent such reports by some of the patients.Citation38

Milder presentations of DID are sometimes associated with traumatization that is covert, such as enduring severely dysfunctional communication and relationship styles in family members, including subtle forms of emotional neglect. In a study conducted on a large group of college students, Şar et alCitation39 demonstrated that emotional neglect predicted a dissociative disorder (including DID) diagnosis. Krüger and FletcherCitation37 demonstrated that self-reported emotional neglect by biological parents or siblings in childhood was the strongest individual predictor of an adult diagnosis of a dissociative disorder (including DID) in psychiatric patients (out of all other combinations of abuse type and abuser-abused relational ties).

Some data and theory suggest that disorganized attachment style may also underpin the development of DID.Citation40Citation45 BowlbyCitation46 proposed that inadequate care-seeking interactions with primary caregivers could lead the infant to develop multiple internal representations of self and attachment figures (which he called internal working models [IWM]). Main and HesseCitation47 identified disorganized attachment developing from a relational context where the child who is seeking safety and comfort is frightened by the caregiver whom the child is pursuing for soothing or the child frightens the caregiver, thereby impeding connection. Contradictory IWM develop to represent the caregiver as dangerous and safe at the same time. These models activate in contexts of abuse to motivate protection from the same caregiver to whom safety seeking is also sought. Early-onset abuse and/or neglect by a relational figure is associated with disorganized attachment (and disorganized attachment may heighten the likelihood of abuse experiences). Hence, disorganized attachment may provide a foundation for DID.Citation40,Citation48

DID patients often feel very isolated/lonely, in the sense that they believe they are the only one in the universe who is “different” from others and that they do not understand themselves. Hence, they experience depersonalization and derealization that may go back to their childhood.Citation49 DePrince et alCitation50 found that alienation was the only cognitive appraisal variable to differentiate DID from PTSD. While the groups had similar appraisals of shame, betrayal, self-blame, anger, and fear, the DID participants had higher appraisal of themselves as experiencing alienation. This construct is associated with feeling alone, disconnected, and different.

Abuse and neglect may activate feelings of alienation, isolation, and loneliness, and such experiences may compound the impact of trauma and the development and maintenance of DID. If the necessary relational support to enable constructive processing of specific abuses is absent, the child is impeded in their ability to make sense of these experiences through narrative and the containment of the affective states activated, thus inhibiting the integration of the abuse with other autobiographical experiences. Consequently, the representations of abuse/neglect experiences remain isolated from integration, and with further incidences and isolation, the child’s ability to develop an ordinary sense of self-in-relation-to-others, based on a coherent narrative that includes the abuse experiences, is impeded and dissociative identities may begin to form. Interpersonal and internal phobias (e.g., phobias of other dissociative identities) then interfere with cooperation toward change, integration, and growth.Citation51,Citation52

High hypnotizability – itself a non-pathological, genetically derived capacity – has also been proposed to be a necessary diathesis for DID.Citation53 Although patients with DID have higher hypnotizability than those with other mental disorders, higher hypnotizability is also found in patients with chronic refractory post-traumatic states in general.Citation54Citation56 Hence, data are required to determine the degree it represents a diathesis for DID that may reflect part of the biological contribution to the development of the disorder in the presence of other necessary factors.

Family, society, and culture

DID can be found in all cultural settings.Citation57 Cultural processes influence the development and phenomenology of DID.Citation58,Citation59 The role of culture may be divided into two components: as the origin of trauma and as modifier of expression of the disorder. Although childhood abuse and neglect require the presence and actions of “perpetrators”, they can occur only in a suitable environment. This environment is characterized by denial, boundary violations, reality distortions, paranoia, narcissism, and dramatic posturing, which usually serves the purpose of maintaining the family structure. These features and dynamics may derive from psychological, relational, and economic needs of one or both of the parents, as well as oppressive traditions that do not allow a dissolution of marriage and other contextual issues in the family.Citation60

Dysfunctions in the family may partly originate from parents’ own traumatic antecedents that lead to inter-generational transmission of developmental stress as reported in the context of the “apparently normal (dissociative) family” by Öztürk and Şar.Citation61 In their empirical study, family members of patients with DID and related dissociative disorders reported frequent mood swings, intense anger and inability to control anger, transient dissociative experiences or paranoid ideas, and identity confusion more frequently than controls. Some of these features were correlated with certain types of childhood trauma in this group. For example, frequent mood swings were associated with all types of childhood trauma except sexual abuse, and identity confusion was correlated with emotional abuse.Citation61

In such a family with subclinical dissociative characteristics, individuals can interchange their social roles over time, alternating between being a victim, abuser, and rescuer.Citation62 Depending on their own traumatic past, or on their current interaction between each other and with their children, the parents may maintain trust and present themselves in a positive role (“angelic”, affectionate/compassionate parent), but they can turn to an abusive parenting style (angry, aggressive, insistent) at any time. The changing attitudes of their parents and the marital discord will often cause contradictory feelings within the children. Family members often feel trapped, first being unable to leave in the midst of a crisis as it is not safe. Then, they do not leave the family when the crisis is over and the need to escape has vanished, as the atmosphere becomes less threatening and more settled. Third, in an environment of neglect, chaos may be an opportunity for making contact with others in the unit.Citation61 Upon direct traumatization early in life, the ever-changing roles in an enduring family system continue to push children and adolescents to utilize a dissociative adaptation style in a period sensitive to the establishment of a stable identity.

Betrayal traumaCitation63 (i.e., trauma perpetrated by someone the victim relies on, e.g., by a primary caregiver)Citation64 is common in such family systems that are characterized by secrets and denial. Betrayal trauma theory suggests that dissociative amnesia is an adaptive response to childhood abuse that allows for survival by enabling the child to maintain attachment to an abusive figure who is also vital to his or her development. A recent study by Kaehler and FreydCitation65 found that higher betrayal traumas are associated with greater “borderline” characteristics that are common in DID as secondary features that do not necessarily point to an underlying personality disorder.Citation24,Citation38,Citation39,Citation49

As a possible example of the influence of culture as a modifier of the expression of DID, in a comparison of Turkish and Dutch patients with DID, large differences existed between the two groups in meeting borderline personality disorder criteria.Citation27,Citation36 Namely, Turkish patients reported intense anger and lack of control of this emotion, chronic feelings of emptiness and boredom, efforts to avoid abandonment, and intense but unstable relationships more frequently than Dutch patients. In turn, Dutch patients reported frequent mood swings, physically self-damaging acts, identity confusion, and impulsive and unpredictable behavior more frequently than Turkish patients. While such differences seem to originate from cultural factors, some types of affect dysregulation, possibly independent of cultural environment, were common to both groups. In another study – possibly as an indicator of the relational nature of the local culture – Turkish adolescent outpatients with dissociative disorders (including DID) differed from non-dissociative psychiatric outpatients on heightened prevalence of concurrent separation anxiety disorder.Citation4

Families constitute a subsystem in the broader context of society and culture. As presented in a South African example of culture as modifier of the expression of DID,Citation66 dissociation may help individuals or communities to survive in a world of conflicting messages, where conflict is often interpersonal/cultural/societal in nature, rather than primarily intrapsychic. The resultant fluctuating self-states – that might have been diagnosed as the dissociative identities of DID in a different cultural context – may be tolerated as normal expressions of societal conflict in a culture where conflicting ideologies pervade everyday life and where a normal sense of self is deeply dependent on a certain level of connectedness with others. Covering a subtler version of such systemic influence as well, Şar and ÖztürkCitation67 proposed a model of sociocognitive adaptation to developmental traumatization at the cost of internal detachment of the individual (“functional dissociation of the self ”).

The so-called sociocognitive model of DID (e.g., Lynn et alCitation68) went beyond recognizing the influence of sociocognitive factors on the development and phenomenology of DID. This model suggested that media reports, a high level of social knowledge about DID, influential and suggestive therapists, as well as patients’ own suggestibility, cognitive distortions and fantasy proneness all led patients to believe (wrongly) that they had dissociative identities. This view of DID markedly contrasts with the post-traumatic model of DID (outlined earlier), which proposes that dissociative identities are the primary results of early trauma and the relational, cognitive, emotional, and neurobiological consequences of it (along with other related factors as outlined in this paper) rather than primarily the result of social and cognitive forces. Moreover, the presence of sociocognitive forces does not provide any proof for iatrogenesis. Yet, sociocognitive and trauma models are not entirely contradictory,Citation69 as the trauma model, e.g., argues that social and cultural factors influence the presentation, but not typically the creation, of dissociative identities.Citation20,Citation69Citation71 In fact, societal conditions themselves may also be the source of traumatic antecedents as observed in oppressive communities and traditions.Citation72

Cognitive etiological models and factors

Memory and the construction of self-identity are cognitive processes that appear markedly and centrally disrupted in DID, such that the discriminating symptoms of the disorder are the experience of amnesia for autobiographical events and the presence (or experience) of non-integrated, dissociative identities. Consequently, cognitive processes are implicated in the etiology of DID. The way cognition is organized and the appraisals and beliefs associated with what autobiographical information is available for retrieval may give rise to dissociative identities and amnesia between them. The development of a sense of self is predicated on the collection of life experiences that are encoded as occurring to the self.Citation73 Thus, the construction of self is underpinned by episodic and semantic autobiographical memories laid down with autonoetic consciousness (i.e., this experience happened to me, and when I remember, it has the felt sense that I experienced it).Citation74

As such, an apparent etiological feature of dissociative identities is associated with the breakdown between encoding experienced events (i.e., the functioning of the human memory system) and perceiving ownership of those events as part of autobiographical experience (i.e., self-referencing the events). That is, there is a breakdown between memory and sense of self. Dissociative identities have their origin in memories of experienced events being owned or feeling like they relate to self in some identities but not others. Thus, different identities draw on, and feel ownership of, 1) different memories, 2) different aspects of the same experience, or 3) overlapping memories/experiences with different appraisals and narratives. Dissociative identities and the different bundles of owned memorized experience that characterize them may differ markedly from each other on lower order characteristics, such as physiological arousal (e.g., heart and respiration rate, blood pressure), affective tone, and neurobio-logical correlates (e.g., dominant brain activation), and higher order characteristics, such as the ability to have ownership of lived experiences, as well as appraise and narrate them.Citation14,Citation18,Citation75

Dissociative identities

Kennedy et al have elucidated a cognitive perspective on the etiology of DID, drawing on the work of Beck.Citation76Citation78 BeckCitation76 proposed that the personality is made up of “modes”, which contain cognitive, affective, behavioral, and physiological representations or schema for encoding experience and responding to internal and environmental demands. For example, a woman may have a “mother” mode that contains how she thinks, feels, behaves, and gets physiologically activated when caring for her child. These modes and the schema that make them up are mobilized by cues detected by what Beck called “orienting schema”, which appraise the internal and external landscape.

Cues identified by the orienting schema to indicate the requirements of child care will initiate, for instance, the mother mode. Under normal circumstances, this mode will be connected with other modes, such as the mode processing and responding to demands about the woman’s job or modes associated with defensive operations such as verbal aggression. All these modes are umbrellaed under the “conscious control system” that provides a means of unifying the multiple modes into a more integrated sense of self (i.e., a sense of “I” who has multiple ways of being and negotiating the world, via different modes or different “Me’s”). DID arises when modes become decoupled and exist in smaller, more isolated pockets (e.g., modes associated with relationship functioning, being a mother, and being a partner become decoupled from modes associated with occupational functioning).Citation77Citation79

This decoupling creates multiple conscious control systems that capture the different and discontinuous arrays of modes, each having their own way of representing that aspect of self, based on the modes that make them up. The etiology of DID then, from a cognitive perspective, is related to the more enduring decoupling or dissociation of the association between modes and the development of different or unique first-person perspectives, or separate senses of self, based on the make-up of these non-coupled clusters of modes.

Amnesia

Attempts to account for amnesia reported across different dissociative identities in clinical practiceCitation63,Citation76 initially drew on state-dependent memory phenomena as more controlled research began investigating memory anomalies across different identities. State-dependent memory refers to the superior recall of information encoded and retrieved in the same emotional state.Citation80 Alternatively, it is the process by which an individual fails to remember mundane information (e.g., where they left their phone) because they encoded the experience (putting their phone on the mantelpiece) in a different state of mind to that in which they are trying to retrieve the information. They may have encoded the information in a highly aroused state following a conflict with their partner and now in a calm state are attempting to find their phone.

Perhaps, the most elaborated framework of this theory for dissociative disorders is encompassed in Putnam’sCitation75 discrete behavioral states model. He argues that initially through biological decrees (need to feed, sleep) and then through experience (e.g., exposure to traumatic stress), different behavioral states come online and/or develop to support survival and promote adaptation to the environment.Citation18 These behavioral states differ not only in their manifest behavior (e.g., sleeping, feeding, socializing, escaping danger via fleeing) but also in all other psychophysiological dimensions, including arousal level, heart rate, motivation, affective tone, thought patterns and content, appraisals, and brain area activation.

PutnamCitation75 argues that trauma exposure leads to the development of discrete behavioral states that differ vastly in their psychophysiological make-up from other states of consciousness, giving rise to pathological dissociative symptoms and, in the most extreme case, dissociative identities. In the case of amnesia, it is proposed to be evident in situations where differences in state-dependent characteristics between encoding and retrieval are most distinct, so, e.g., retrieval failure when a state characterized by relatively minimal arousal is asked about an experience encoded in a high state of arousal (e.g., an episode of incestuous abuse).

In DID, amnesia across identities is accounted for by encoding in one identity that has a very different psychophysiological make-up than the identity attempting to retrieve the information. As a result, amnesia would be predicted to be more likely across identities that are vastly different in their biopsychosocial characteristics. This theory draws on mechanisms evident in normal cognitive functioning (i.e., state-dependent memory) to explain amnesia across dissociative identities. It is proposed to account for findings in research that suggested that information encoded in one dissociative identity may not be available in a dissociative identity reporting amnesia for that information.Citation81

Recent research examining amnesia across dissociative identities has required an elaboration of earlier theories in an attempt to address the etiology of amnesia in dissociative identities. This work has found, at least in patients presenting for treatment, that memories residing in another dissociative identity for which amnesia is perceived are actually available for retrieval or at least on objective assessment appear accessible.Citation82 This has prompted the question of whether amnesia across identities in DID is a cognitive process (i.e., associated with encoding characteristics and/or retrieval anomalies) or a metacognitive process (e.g., no encoding or retrieval deficits but a belief that content is not available). This latter explanation attempts to account for findings that representations of experience in memory appear to be accessible for retrieval in dissociative identities that believe they have no memory of such material.

Metacognitive processes, or more specifically metamemory processes, are involved in the monitoring, control, and appraisal of memories. They monitor for memories that fit goal-directed aims and assess them for subjective accuracy (e.g., the confidence in which the details retrieved feel accurate), they control which information is best to volunteer, and they subjectively appraise the ability to remember.Citation83

Huntjens et al draw on metacognitive processes to account for recent empirical findings, suggesting that amnesia in DID is not the result of being unable to access information but the result of believing that such information is inaccessible. She and her colleagues argued that, “[m]etamemory problems may lead patients not to acknowledge correctly retrieved material, which is believed to ‘belong’ to other identities, or indeed choose an incorrect answer alternative following the belief that the correct answer alternative cannot be known if the material that one is tested for was learned [or experienced] by another identity”.Citation84 The etiology of interidentity amnesia from this perspective is a metamemory belief that the material is unavailable rather than it being unavailable.Citation85 It resides in metamemory processes rather than in deficit memory functions. Consequently, the etiology of dissociative amnesia across identities may lie in information being encoded in a state (i.e., identity) that the retrieving identity, 1) believes they have no access to, and therefore gives up the search for such information, despite its availability, 2) appraises retrieval success as minimal, or 3) monitors with no confidence that retrieved information belongs to the self and therefore discards it.

While it remains to be examined empirically, the reported experience of amnesia across identities in DID may have its origin in retrieval states (i.e., dissociative identities) being psychobiologically distinct from encoding states (i.e., different dissociative identities). Such state-dependent processes may disrupt the search for and appraisal of encoded information (i.e., metamemory processes).

Neurobiology of DID

Studies on the neurobiology of DID are in their infancy. They have focused mainly on two issues: differences between dissociative identities within patients with DIDCitation86 and differences between patients with DID and controls (the controls being either healthy subjects or other psychiatric patients). There are both structural and functional neuroimaging findings that demonstrate differences between dissociative patients and non-clinical populations. However, the specificity of these findings for DID would hinge on future comparisons with patients with other psychiatric disorders and whether the structural anomalies, e.g., are a cause or consequence of DID.

In positron emission tomography (PET) studies, when compared to a dissociative identity focused on the tasks of daily living, a dissociative identity focused on trauma memories and defensive operations showed increased cerebral blood flow in the amygdala, insular cortex, somatosensory areas in the parietal cortex, and the basal ganglia, as well as in the occipital and frontal regions and anterior cingulate.Citation87,Citation88 In a subsequent PET study, healthy controls simulating distinct dissociative identities were unable to reproduce the same network patterns as the DID patients.Citation86

In the quantitative electro-encephalogram (QEEG) study by Lapointe et al,Citation89 variability between identity states involved mostly beta activity in the frontal and temporal lobes. Hopper et alCitation90 demonstrated that the average alpha coherence on QEEG was lower for “alter” identities than for “host” identities (i.e., the identity predominantly engaging with the external world) in five DID patients in some temporal, frontal, parietal, and central regions.

In an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, women with DID and matched healthy female actors were compared in their responses (assessed both in “host” and ”emotionally laden” dissociative identities) when consecutively exposed to masked neutral and angry faces.Citation91 Differences between “host” and “emotionally laden” identities in DID patients and between DID and simulating controls were generally larger for neutral than angry faces. In DID, compared to “host”, “emotionally laden” dissociative identities were associated with more activation of the parahippocampal gyrus. Following neutral faces and compared to the “emotionally laden” identity in controls, “emotionally laden” identity in DID had more activation in brainstem, face-sensitive regions, and motor-related areas. There were neither significant within-group differences nor significant between-group differences in state anxiety. Controls were not able to simulate genuine “host” and “emotionally laden” dissociative identities.

A structural MRI study established that DID patients have smaller hippocampi and amygdalae than normal controls.Citation92 Ehling et alCitation93 also found reduced volumes in the parahippocampal gyrus of individuals with DID and strong correlations between reduction of parahippocampal volume and both cognitive-emotional and sensorimotor dissociation, in comparison with normal controls. In two single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) studies, DID patients exhibited orbitofrontal hypoperfusion in comparison with normal controls conducted in “host” identities.Citation94,Citation95 Bilaterally increased perfusion in prefrontal regions and occipital areas accompanied this in one of these studies.Citation95 In the other one, the “host” identity showed increased perfusion in the left (dominant hemisphere) lateral temporal region compared to healthy controls.Citation94 This lateralization was not replicated in a follow-up study.Citation95

Notwithstanding the possible effect of psychiatric comorbidity as a confounding factor (hence, it cannot be considered as specific to DID), the findings concerning orbitofrontal hypoperfusion do not seem to be at odds with the theoretical understanding of developmental neurobiology. Longitudinal neuroimaging studies suggest that the orbitofrontal cortex is one of the last regions in the brain to fully develop in humans.Citation95 A tensor-based morphometry investigation indicates that orbitofrontal cortex volumes are smaller in children who have suffered early aberrant parental care in the form of physical abuse and that these volumetric alterations are associated with difficulties children experience in various aspects of their social lives.Citation96 The orbitofrontal cortex is a key component of a circuit that aids in adaptation to changing environmental contingencies and plays an important role in the control of emotion and motivational states. In this regard, SchoreCitation97 reported that there is a relationship between the development of the orbitofrontal cortex, emotion regulation, and attachment. In accordance with these observations and based on a neurodevelopmental approach, ForrestCitation98 proposed an “orbitofrontal model” for DID, which integrates and elaborates on theory and research from four domains: the neurobiology of the orbitofrontal cortex and its protective inhibitory role in the temporal organization of behavior, the development of emotion regulation, the development of the self, and experience-dependent maturation of the orbitofrontal cortex. This model hypothesizes that the orbitofrontal cortex plays a critical role in the development of distinct mental states (i.e., dissociative identities) due to its inhibitory functions.

Conclusion and directions for future research

Understanding the etiology of DID requires integration of trauma exposure, coping, cognitive, neurobiological, systemic, and developmental factors. These include traumatic experiences, family dynamics, child development, and attachment.Citation17,Citation63,Citation99 DID develops when a child is exposed to chaos, coercion, and overt severe physical and/or sexual abuse or, alternatively, to “apparently normal” dissociative families often with subtle neglect, disorganized attachment to caregivers, and misattuned communication styles.Citation59 While the role of the child’s biological capacity to dissociate to an extreme level is unclear yet, there is evidence demonstrating the neurobiological impact of developmental stress. The latter converges around an impairment of connectivity in the central nervous system in affected individuals. From a psychological point of view, multiple self-states that do not become integrated over time allow the child to compart-mentalize trauma-related cognitions and overwhelming and conflicting feelings of betrayal, terror, love, and shame.Citation13,Citation17 Overwhelmed by intense conflicting needs and emotions, the child is unable to integrate discrete behavioral and emotional states into a coherent or relatively integrated self according to the appropriate sociocultural construction of self.Citation17,Citation100 Further research on the etiology of DID should be able to link clinical and empirical dimensions while considering the four domains addressed in this paper: i.e., developmental traumatization, family and sociocultural factors, cognitive functioning, and neurobiological anomalies.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • WolfEJRasmussonAMMitchellKSLogueMWBaldwinCTMillerMWA genome-wide association study of clinical symptoms of dissociation in a trauma-exposed sampleDepress Anxiety201431435236024677629
  • LochnerCSeedatSHemmingsSMJDissociative experiences in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania: clinical and genetic findingsCompr Psychiatry200445538439115332202
  • ŞarVEpidemiology of dissociative disorders: an overviewEpidemiol Res Int201120118 Article ID 404538
  • ŞarVÖnderCKılınçaslanAZoroğluSSAlyanakBDissociative identity disorder among adolescents: prevalence in a university psychiatric outpatient unitJ Trauma Dissociation201415440241924283750
  • ŞarVKoyuncuAÖztürkEDissociative disorders in psychiatric emergency wardGen Hosp Psychiatry2007291455017189745
  • CarlsonEPutnamFWAn update on the Dissociative Experiences ScaleDissociation1993611627
  • VanderlindenJVan DyckRVandereyckenWVertommenHJan VerkesRThe dissociation questionnaire (DIS-Q): development and characteristics of a new self-report questionnaireClin Psychol Psychother1993112127
  • DellPFThe Multidimensional Inventory of Dissociation (MID): a comprehensive measure of pathological dissociationJ Trauma Dissociation2006727710616769667
  • RossCAHeberSNortonGRAndersonGAndersonDBarchetPThe dissociative disorders interview schedule: a structured interviewDissociation198923169189
  • SteinbergMStructured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders – Revised (SCID-D-R)Washington, DCAmerican Psychiatric Press1994
  • SteinbergMHandbook for the Assessment of Dissociation A Clinical GuideWashington, DCAmerican Psychiatric Publications1995
  • BraunBGThe BASK (behavior, affect, sensation, knowledge) model of dissociationDissociation199811423
  • Van der HartONijenhuisERSSteeleKThe Haunted Self: Structural Dissociation and the Treatment of Chronic TraumatizationNew York, NYNorton2006
  • NijenhuisERSThe Trinity of Trauma: Ignorance, Fragility and Control: The Evolving Concept of Trauma/The Concept and Facts of Dissociation in TraumaGottingenVandenhoeck & Ruprecht2015
  • O’NeilJADissociative multiplicity and psychoanalysisDellPFO’NeilJADissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and BeyondNew York, NYRoutledge2009287325
  • NijenhuisERSVan der HartODissociation in trauma: a new definition and comparison with previous formulationsJ Trauma Dissociation201112441644521667387
  • PutnamFWDissociative disordersCicchettiDCohenDJDevelopmental Psychopathology2New YorkWiley2006657695
  • PutnamFWThe Way We Are. How States of Mind Influence Our Identities, Personality and Potential for ChangeLos Gatos, CAInternational Psychoanalytic Books2016
  • HowellEFBlizardRAChronic relational trauma disorder: a new diagnostic scheme for borderline personality and the spectrum of dissociative disordersDellPFO’NeilJADissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and BeyondNew York, NYRoutledge2009495510
  • DalenbergCJBrandBLGleavesDHEvaluation of the evidence for the trauma and fantasy models of dissociationPsychol Bull2012138355058822409505
  • AlexanderPCSchaefferCMA typology of incestuous families based on cluster analysisJ Family Psychol199484458470
  • AllenJGFultzJHuntoonJBrethourJRJrPathological dissociative taxon membership, absorption, and reported childhood trauma in women with trauma-related disordersJ Trauma Dissociation20023189110
  • CarlsonEBDalenbergCArmstrongJDanielsJWLoewensteinRRothDMultivariate prediction of posttraumatic symptoms in psychiatric inpatientsJ Trauma Stress200114354956711534885
  • ŞarVAlioğluFAkyüzGTayakısıEÖğülmüşEFSönmezDAwareness of identity alteration and diagnostic preference between borderline personality disorder and dissociative disordersJ Trauma Dissociation Epub2016125117
  • ŞarVIdentity revised: a clinician’s perspective on how an identity-based model of mind would look likeSinnottJIdentity Flexibility During Adulthood: Perspectives in Adult DevelopmentNew York, NYSpringerIn press2017
  • ŞarVParallel-distinct structures of internal world and external reality: disavowing and re-claiming the self-identity in the aftermath of trauma-generated dissociationFront Psychol2017821628261144
  • BoonSDraijerNMultiple personality disorder in The Netherlands: a clinical investigation of 71 patientsAm J Psychiatry199315034894948434668
  • DorahyMJMiddletonWSeagerLWilliamsMChambersRChild abuse and neglect in complex dissociative disorder, abuse-related chronic PTSD and mixed psychiatric samplesJ Trauma Dissociation201617222323626275087
  • Martinez-TaboasAMultiple personality in Puerto Rico: analysis of fifteen casesDissociation199144189192
  • MiddletonWButlerJDissociative identity disorder: an Australian seriesAust N Z J Psychiatry1998326786804
  • RossCANortonGRWozneyKMultiple personality disorder: an analysis of 236 casesCan J Psychiatry19893454134182766193
  • AkyüzGDoğanOŞarVYargıçLİTutkunHFrequency of dissociative identity disorder in the general population in TurkeyCompr Psychiatry199940215115910080263
  • RossCANessLSymptom patterns in dissociative identity disorder patients and the general populationJ Trauma Dissociation201011445846820938869
  • CoonsPMConfirmation of childhood abuse in child and adolescent cases of multiple personality disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specifiedJ Nerv Ment Dis199418284614648040657
  • LewisDOYeagerCASwicaYPincusJHLewisMObjective 4 documentation of child abuse and dissociation in 12 murderers with dissociative identity disorderAm J Psychiatry199715412170317109396949
  • ŞarVYargicLITutkunHStructured interview data on 35 cases of dissociative identity disorder in TurkeyAm J Psychiatry199615310132913338831443
  • KrügerCFletcherLPredicting a dissociative disorder from type of childhood maltreatment and abuser-abused relational tieJ Trauma Dissociation Epub2017223
  • ŞarVAlioğluFAkyüzGKarabulutSDissociative amnesia in dissociative disorders and borderline personality disorder: self-rating assessment in a college populationJ Trauma Dissociation201415447749324678926
  • ŞarVAkyüzGKuğuNOzturkEErtem-VehidHAxis-I dissociative disorder comorbidity of borderline personality disorder and childhood trauma reportsJ Clin Psychiatry200667101583159017107251
  • BarachPMMultiple personality disorder as an attachment disorderDissociation199143117123
  • BlizardRADisorganized attachment, development of dissociated self states, and a relational approach to treatmentJ Trauma Dissociation2003432750
  • ByunSBrumariuLELyons-RuthKDisorganized attachment in young adulthood as a partial mediator of relations between severity of childhood abuse and dissociationJ Trauma Dissociation201617446047926836233
  • LiottiGTrauma, dissociation, and disorganized attachment: three strands of a single braidPsychotherapy2004414472486
  • LiottiGA model of dissociation based on attachment theory and researchJ Trauma Dissociation200674557317182493
  • Lyons-RuthKDutraLSchuderMRBianchiIFrom infant attachment disorganization to adult dissociation: relational adaptations or traumatic experiences?Psychiatr Clin North Am2006291638616530587
  • BowlbyJAttachment and LossNew York, NYBasic Books1969/1982
  • MainMHesseEParents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: is frightened or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism?GreenbergMCicchettiCCummingsEMAttachment in the Preschool YearsChicago, ILUniversity of Chicago Press1990161182
  • SachsAThrough the lens of attachment relationship: stable DID, active DID and other trauma-based mental disordersJ Trauma Dissociation Epub2017223121
  • ŞarVAlioğluFAkyüzGDepersonalization and derealization in self-report and clinical interview: the spectrum of borderline personality disorder, dissociative disorders, and healthy controlsJ Trauma Dissociation Epub2016928117
  • DePrinceAPHuntjensRJCDorahyMJAlienation appraisals distinguish adults diagnosed with DID from PTSDPsychol Trauma20157657858226168348
  • FrankelASO’HearnTCSimilarities in responses to extreme and unremitting stress: cultures of communities under siegePsychotherapy1996333485502
  • SteeleKvan der HartONijenhuisERSDependency in the treatment of complex posttraumatic stress disorder and dissociative disordersJ Trauma Dissociation20012479116
  • DellPFIs high hypnotizability a necessary diathesis for pathological dissociation?J Trauma Dissociation2017181588727216366
  • FrischholzELipmanLBraunBSachsRPsychopathology, hypnotizability, and dissociationAm J Psychiatry199214911152115251415819
  • SpiegelDHuntTDondershineHEDissociation and hypnotizability in posttraumatic stress disorderAm J Psychiatry198814533013053344845
  • StutmanRKBlissELPost-traumatic stress disorder, hypnotizability and imageryAm J Psychiatry198514267417424003596
  • ŞarVThe scope of dissociative disorders: an international perspectivePsychiatr Clin North Am200629122724416530595
  • DorahyMJCulture, cognition and dissociative identity disorderSchumakerJFWardTCulture, Cognition and PsychopathologyWestport, CTPraeger2001157169
  • KrügerCVariations in identity alteration – a qualitative study of experiences of psychiatric patients with dissociative identity disorderVan der MerweAPSinasonVShattered but Unbroken: Voices of Triumph and TestimonyLondonKarnac Books2016133161
  • GoldSNNot Trauma Alone: Therapy for Child Abuse Survivors in Family and Social ContextPhiladelphia, PABrunner & Routledge2000
  • ÖztürkEŞarV“Apparently normal” family: a contemporary agent of transgenerational trauma and dissociationJ Trauma Pract200543–4287303
  • RossCADissociative Identity Disorder: Diagnosis, Clinical Features, and Treatment of Multiple Personality2nd edNew York, NYWiley1997
  • FreydJJBetrayal trauma: traumatic amnesia as an adaptive response to childhood abuseEthics Behav199444307329
  • GoldsmithREFreydJJDePrinceAPBetrayal trauma: associations with psychological and physical symptoms in young adultsJ Interpers Violence201227354756721987504
  • KaehlerLAFreydJJBorderline personality disorder: a betrayal trauma approachPsychol Trauma200914261268
  • KrügerCSokudelaBFMotlanaLMMatabogeCKDikobeAMDissociation: a preliminary contextual modelSouth African J Psychiatry20071311321
  • ŞarVÖztürkEFunctional dissociation of the self: a sociocognitive approach to dissociationJ Trauma Dissociation2007846989
  • LynnSJLilienfeldSOMerckelbachHGiesbrechtTVan der KloetDDissociation and dissociative disorders: challenging conventional wisdomCurr Direc Psychol Sci2002214853
  • ŞarVKrügerCMartinez-TaboasAMiddletonWDorahyMJSociocognitive and posttraumatic models are not opposedJ Nerv Ment Dis2013201543944023639894
  • DalenbergCJBrandBLLoewensteinRJReality vs. fantasy: reply to Lynn et al. (2014)Psychol Bull2014140391192024773506
  • DorahyMJBrandBLŞarVDissociative identity disorder: an empirical overviewAust N Z J Psychiatry201448540241724788904
  • ŞarVÖztürkEStimulus deprivation and overstimulation as dissociogenic agents in postmodern oppressive societiesJ Trauma Dissociation201314219821223406224
  • ConwayMAMemory and the selfJ Mem Lang200553594628
  • PrebbleSCAddisDRTippettLJAutobiographical memory and sense of selfPsychol Bull2013139481584023025923
  • PutnamFWDissociation in Children and Adolescents: A Developmental PerspectiveNew York, NYGuilford Press1997
  • BeckATBeyond belief: A theory of modes, personality, and psychopathologySalkovskisPMFrontiers of Cognitive TherapyNew York, NYGuilford Press1996125
  • KennedyFDissociation, personality and psychopathology: a cognitive approachKennedyFKennerleyHPearsonDCognitive Behavioural Approaches to the Understanding and Treatment of DissociationHove, East SussexRoutledge20131739
  • KennedyFClarkeSStopaLTowards a cognitive model and measure of dissociationJ Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry2004351254815157816
  • KennedyFKennerleyHThe development of our understanding of dissociationKennedyFKennerleyHPearsonDCognitive Behavioural Approaches to the Understanding and Treatment of DissociationHove, East SussexRoutledge2013116
  • LangAJCraskeMGBrownMGhaneianAFear-related state dependent memoryCognition Emotion2001155695703
  • DorahyMJDissociative identity disorder and memory dysfunction: the current state of experimental research, and its future directionsClin Psychol Rev200121577179511434230
  • HuntjensRJCVerschuereRMcNallyRJInter-Identity autobiographical amnesia in patients with dissociative identity disorderPLoS One201277e4058022815769
  • KoriatAGoldsmithMMonitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracyPsychol Rev199610334905168759045
  • HuntjensRJCPetersMLWoertmanLBovenschenLMMartinRCPostmaAInter-identity amnesia in dissociative identity disorder: a simulated memory impairment?Psychol Med200636685786316438741
  • HuntjensRJCPetersMLWoertmanLVan der HartOPostmaAMemory transfer for emotionally valenced words between identities in dissociative identity disorderBehav Res Ther200745477578916928360
  • ReindersAATSWillemsenATMVosHPJDen BoerJANijenhuisERSFact or factitious? A psychobiological study of authentic and simulated dissociative identity statesPLoS One201276e3927922768068
  • ReindersAATSNijenhuisERSPaansAMJKorfJWillemsenATMDen BoerJAOne brain, two selvesNeuroimage20032042119212514683715
  • ReindersAATSNijenhuisERSQuakJPsychobiological characteristics of dissociative identity disorder: a symptom provocation studyBiol Psychiatry200660773074017008145
  • LapointeARCraytonJWDeVitoRFichtnerCGKonopkaLMSimilar or disparate brain patterns? The intra-personal EEG variability of three women with multiple personality disorderClin EEG Neurosci200637323524216929711
  • HopperACiorciariJJohnsonGSpensleyJSergejewAStoughCEEG coherence and dissociative identity disorderJ Trauma Dissociation2002317588
  • SchlumpfYRNijenhuisERSChalaviSDissociative part dependent biopsychosocial reactions to backward masked angry and neutral faces: an fMRI study of dissociative identity disorderNeuroimage Clin20133546424179849
  • VermettenESchmahlCLindnerSLoewensteinRBremnerJHippocampal and amygdalar volumes in dissociative identity disorderAm J Psychiatry2006163463063616585437
  • EhlingTNijenhuisERSKrikkeAPVolume of discrete brain structures in complex dissociative disorders: preliminary findingsProg Brain Res2007167307310
  • ŞarVÜnalSNKızıltanEKundakçıTÖztürkEHMPAO SPECT study of cerebral perfusion in dissociative identity disorderJ Trauma Dissociation200122525
  • ŞarVÜnalSNOzturkEFrontal and occipital perfusion changes in dissociative identity disorderPsychiatry Res2007156321722317961993
  • HansonJLChungMKAvantsBBEarly stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: a tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral riskJ Neurosci201030227466747220519521
  • SchoreANThe experience-dependent maturation of a regulatory system in the orbital prefrontal cortex and the origin of developmental psychopathologyDev Psychopathol199685487
  • ForrestKAToward an etiology of dissociative identity disorder: a neurodevelopmental approachConscious Cogn200110325929311697866
  • KluftRPMultiple personality disorderSpiegelDDissociative Disorders: A Clinical ReviewLutherville, MDSidran Press19931744
  • SchimmentiACarettiVLinking the overwhelming with the unbearable: developmental trauma, dissociation, and the disconnected selfPsychoanal Psychol2016331106128