176
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

The effect of perspective taking on the mediation process

, , &
Pages 411-416 | Published online: 27 Sep 2018

Abstract

Background

Previous research demonstrated several benefits of strategic perspective taking in the field of intergroup relations and, more specifically, in the negotiation processes aimed at conflict resolution. The present study, which analyzes the effect of perspective taking and mediation in a conflict setting, corroborates the psychological models that hypothesize the positive effects of the assumption of the competitor’s perspective on having intergroup conflict and lessening of negative consequences.

Materials and methods

After being involved in an epistolary debate on a topic for which their ingroup had very contrasting views compared to an outgroup, participants were asked to use a feeling thermometer to assess their level of intergroup hostility. Mediation was then used as a conflict-resolution strategy for half of the participants. Furthermore, the assumption of perspective was manipulated, resulting in a 2 (conflict: presence vs absence) × 2 (perspective taking: presence vs absence) × 2 (mediation: presence vs absence) between-subjects design. Finally, participants were asked to use the same feeling thermometer to evaluate whether feelings of hostility diminished.

Results

The results show that in conflict situations, the level of hostility decreases the most when the mediation process is accompanied by perspective taking.

Conclusion

The results extend recent results about the advantages of a significant social ability – perspective taking – for improving intergroup relations.

Introduction

Perspective taking is the cognitive ability to look at the world from another person’s perspective, which “allows an individual to anticipate the behavior and reactions of others.”Citation1 The capacity to assume the perspective of another person has been recognized as an important factor in social functioning that increases empathic attitudes and diminishes the amount of category-based responses toward outgroup members.Citation1,Citation2 Piaget asserted that the skill to modify perspectives is a key step in cognitive development.Citation3 Davis found that perspective taking is directly related to social competence and self-esteem.Citation1 In addition, perspective taking can promote helping behavior and reduce false-consensus effects.Citation4Citation8 Finally, researchers have related the lack of perspective taking to social dysfunction and aggressive behavior.Citation9,Citation10

The present study deepens the recent literature about the relative benefits of perspective taking in the domain of intergroup conflict.

Previous research demonstrated several benefits of strategic perspective taking in the field of intergroup relations and, more specifically, in the negotiation processes aimed at conflict resolution.Citation11,Citation12 In regard to the first aspect, researchers have determined the efficacy of perspective taking as a strategy for discrimination denial and the reduction of intergroup prejudice, both for positive explicit intergroup evaluations and even for implicit racial evaluations and attenuation of automatic expressions of racial bias.Citation13Citation17 For a review, see Todd and Galinsky.Citation18

As for what is concerned in the negotiation process, Bazerman and Neale affirmed that “taking the perspective of an opponent is not done for purely philanthropic reasons; rather, in achieving any set of objectives, there is valuable information to be gleaned from taking the perspective of the other negotiating party.”Citation11 The authors of several practical guidebooks have considered perspective taking to be a valuable strategy that helps negotiators to reach satisfying agreements.Citation19

In support of this hypothesis, Galinsky and Mussweiler, for example, showed that perspective taking is a potent instrument for getting over cognitive barriers, such as anchor effects, in negotiations.Citation20 Bazerman and Neale and Galinsky et al demonstrated that negotiators who took their counterparts’ perspective both increased their individual outcomes and succeeded in creating more value for the counterpart in an integrative negotiation task, that is, an integrative agreement which reconciles the interests of both parties and leads to higher joint benefit.Citation11,Citation21Citation23

Beyond highlighting the relative benefits of perspective taking in intergroup evaluations and behaviors, researchers have also deepened the study of the mechanisms underlying intergroup perspective taking. In their review, Todd and Galinsky suggested that perspective taking may have a positive effect on intergroup outcomes through both affective and cognitive mechanisms.Citation18 On the affective side, in fact, several researchers have documented increased empathic responding following perspective taking.Citation24Citation26 On the cognitive side, two different processes have been proposed: shifts in attributional style and self-outgroup merging. The former refers to a change in attributional style – specifically, stronger non-dispositional attributions and weaker dispositional attributions – following perspective taking. Vescio et al, for example, showed that perspective taking increases judgments relying on non-dispositional factors in the explanation of negative outgroup behaviors.Citation15,Citation26

The latter, self-outgroup merging, refers to an overlap in mental representations of the self and the other. Todd and Burgmer suggest an associative self-anchoring account of automatic intergroup evaluation change following perspective taking.Citation17 This account entails a causal sequence whereby adopting an outgroup member’s perspective strengthens associations between a targeted outgroup and the self, which, in turn, enables more positive automatic intergroup evaluations. In fact, because most people’s self-associations are positive, this associative link, in general, generates greater positivity toward the outgroup. As a result of this increased self-outgroup overlapping, implicit intergroup evaluation seems to be more positive.Citation2,Citation15

Perspective taking can thus play a significant role in negotiating intergroup conflicts. More specifically, when mediation is adopted as the resolution strategy, perspective taking can boost the efficacy of this process, which is a very effective tool adopted to face common (tractable) conflicts and even to contain the harmful consequences of intractable intergroup conflicts.Citation27

The study

The aim of this study was to investigate the role of perspective taking in the mediation process during an attempted intergroup conflict resolution. It is hypothesized that assuming the competitor’s point of view might be a psychological process that fosters the reestablishment of a cooperative setting. In addition, the presence of a mediator should facilitate the resolution, something that should be more evident when participants assume the competitor’s perspective.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 160 graduate students in psychology (33 men and 127 women; age: M=28.8; SD=3.2) took part in the experiment on a voluntary basis and were randomly assigned to one of the conditions of a 2 (conflict: presence vs absence) × 2 (perspective taking: presence vs absence) × 2 (mediation: presence vs absence) between-subjects experimental design.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The ethics committee of the Department of Psychological, Pedagogical and Educational Sciences, University of Palermo, Italy, reviewed the methods of the study and approved it. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Participants were debriefed about the study’s purpose and were informed that they could withdraw from participation at any time and without any consequences.

Procedure and measures

All participants were run individually. They were invited to take part in the experiment without knowing the real aim of the study. At the end of the experiment, all participants were thoroughly debriefed and then thanked for their participation.

The experimental procedure consisted of an epistolary exchange between two people, a medical doctor and a psychologist (the participant him/herself). Participants read an excerpt of what they believed was a true interview with a medical doctor, who was named “Dr Mario Rossi” for privacy reasons. In the experimental conditions in which the conflict was present, Mario Rossi used harsh tones, and he strongly discredited psychologists’ work while praising the role of medical doctors in treating mental illness. In the experimental conditions in which the conflict was absent, Mario Rossi used mostly positive arguments when he referred to psychologists and appreciated their contributions in treating mental illness. The participants, who were all graduate students in psychology, were asked to pay attention to the interview and then write a reply to Dr Mario Rossi describing the psychologists’ points of view. A feeling thermometer, with a scale ranging from 0 to 100, was used to assess the intensity of the hostility created.

The second variable of the experiment was mediation. An associate of the experimenter was trained to act as a conflict mediator and took part in all of the experimental situations in which an attempt at conflict mediation was present. Thus, mediation accompanied half of the conditions with conflict and half of the conditions without conflict. In the conditions in which mediation was absent, participants wrote the answer to Mario Rossi’s interview alone.

To manipulate perspective taking, we adopted the procedure originally developed by Galinsky and Moskowitz.Citation2 In the condition with a presence of perspective taking, the participants were asked to put themselves in their counterparts’ shoes. In the condition with an absence of perspective taking, this request was omitted.

After experimental manipulations, the feeling thermometer was administered once again.

Results

Manipulation check

Analyses of variance (ANOVA) revealed that the participants in the conflict condition had experienced more increased feelings of hostility against the doctor (M=4.67, SD =1.36) than the participants in the conflict-absent condition (M=3.67, SD =1.86), F(1, 109) =6.38, P=0.013. The experimental procedure thus succeeded in producing an intergroup conflict situation.

Measure of hostility

In all experimental conditions, hostility was assessed twice with the same instrument – a thermometer. For each participant, the dependent variable’s value was calculated as the difference in hostility between the first measurement (before treatments) and the second one (after treatments). In this way, positive values indicated a decrease of hostility, while negative values indicated an increase.

Data distribution of the average hostility scores was analyzed by looking at the skewness and kurtosis indexes. The results showed a normal univariate distribution with an asymmetry value of –0.91 and kurtosis of –0.16.Citation28

A 2 (conflict) × 2 (perspective taking) × 2 (mediation) between-groups ANOVA was conducted to compare the average hostility levels across the conditions. The mean and standard deviation values are reported in .

Table 1 Mean and standard deviation of hostility scores

The results showed significant main effects of conflict, F(1,159) =727.02, P<0.001; mediation F(1,159) =115.16, P<0.001; and perspective taking F(1,159) =387.07, P<0.001. In addition, significant interaction effects were shown between conflict and mediation, F(1,159) =105.94, P<0.001; conflict and perspective taking, F(1,159) =334.54, P<0.001; and conflict, mediation, and perspective taking, F(1,159) =12.80, P<0.001, and the interaction effect between mediation and perspective taking was not significant, F(1,159) =3.7, P=0.074.

Because we were mainly interested in the effects of mediation and perspective taking in overt intergroup conflict, the conditions in which conflict was present or absent were also analyzed separately. The results showed that, in the absence of intergroup conflict, neither mediation, F(1,79) =0.16, P=ns, nor perspective taking, F(1,79) =1.62, P=ns, produced main effects or interactions, F(1,79) =2.31, P=ns. On the other hand, in the presence of conflicts between psychologists and medical doctors, the results showed a significant main effect of both mediation, F(1,79) =157.01, P<0.001, and perspective taking, F(1,79) =511.97, P<0.001, and a significant interaction effect between these two variables, F(1,79) =10.75, P<0.01. Looking more closely at the mean scores, it may be observed that perspective taking may effectively reduce intergroup conflict and that its effect is amplified by mediation ().

Figure 1 Mean of the hostility scores in conditions of conflict presence (A) and absence (B).

Figure 1 Mean of the hostility scores in conditions of conflict presence (A) and absence (B).

In summary, the results show that in conflict situations, a mediation process may decrease the level of hostility among the parts in conflict and that this result can be magnified when one can assume the perspective of the counterpart.

Conclusion

In the last two decades, several authors have looked at the role of perspective taking in decreasing the amount of bias associated with stereotyping and intergroup relations.Citation2,Citation12,Citation29,Citation30

These researchers have shown that perspective taking is a suitable approach to obtaining a better social judgment of the outgroup and proved that perspective taking reduces stereotypical expressions and diminishes the accessibility of the stereotyped category.

To this end, this study’s rationale was to assess whether perspective taking facilitates a mediation process set to reduce hostility between two groups. The analysis of the roles of perspective taking and mediation showed that both variables were able to reduce hostility. Moreover, the level of hostility was further reduced when perspective taking accompanied mediation. Thus, perspective taking might function as a critical factor in the treatment of intergroup conflict, particularly when mediation is adopted as the treatment strategy.

Therefore, perspective taking not only seems to be a necessary condition to reduce the stereotypes and prejudices that could interfere with a successful mediation process but it may also empower good relations between the involved parties and reorganize dysfunctional rules that permeate an hostile social environment.

Limitations

This work undoubtedly has limitations. The experimental procedure gave rise to an artificial and controllable conflict in which participant’s counterpart was always physically absent. The procedure, in fact, required the assumption of perspective to be suggested by the mediator only to the participant, as Dr Rossi (the counterpart) was absent, and his point of view was accessible through the report of an interview. In this way, it can be said that the assumption of the perspective was one-sided. In a real mediation context, the mediator generally does not act in a unilateral way; instead, he/she should drive each conflicting part to assume the point of view of the other one. Thus, it may be premature to generalize these results to any overt intergroup conflict, as timing and context matter in ways that are not captured by the procedure of this study. At the same time, these findings have theoretical implications, especially in regard to the joint effect of mediation and perspective taking. They seem to be something more than two different tools that can be extracted from the toolbox when needed. Their joint effect cannot be reduced to the sum of the effect of the two strategies. Instead, being invited to assume the counterpart’s point of view magnifies the effect of mediation. Thus, it is very important that the mediator possesses the appropriate skills to make the groups in conflict understand each other’s points of view and reestablish an effective communication flow based on a mutual understanding free of prejudice.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • DavisMHMeasuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approachJ Pers Soc Psychol1983441113126
  • GalinskyADMoskowitzGBPerspective-taking: decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritismJ Pers Soc Psychol200078470872410794375
  • PiagetJThe Moral Judgment of the ChildLondonKegan, Paul, Trench & Trubner1932
  • BatsonCDThe Altruism Question: Toward a Social-Psychological AnswerHillsdale, NJErlbaum1991
  • BatsonCDAltruism and prosocial behaviorGilbertDTFiskeSTLindseyGThe Handbook of Social PsychologyBostonMcGraw Hill1998282316
  • Scaffidi AbbateCRuggieriSThe fairness principle, reward, and altruistic behaviorJ Appl Soc Psychol2011511101120
  • GendollaGHEWicklundRASelf-focused attention, perspective-taking, and false consensusSocial Psychology2009406672
  • Scaffidi AbbateCBocaSGendollaGHESelf-awareness, perspective-taking, and egocentrismSelf Identity2016154371380
  • Baron-CohenSMindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of MindCambridge, MABradford Books MIT Press1995
  • RichardsonDRHammockGSSmithSMGardnerWSignoMEmpathy as a cognitive inhibitor of interpersonal aggressionAggress Behav1994204275289
  • BazermanMHNealeMAHeuristics in negotiation: limitations to effective dispute resolutionArkesHRHammondKRJudgment and Decision Making: An Interdisciplinary ReaderNew YorkCambridge University Press1986311321
  • GalinskyADKuGThe effects of perspective-taking on prejudice: the moderating role of self-evaluationPers Soc Psychol Bull200430559460415107159
  • BatsonCDEarlySSalvaraniGPerspective taking: imagining how another feels versus imaging how you would feelPers Soc Psychol Bull1997237751758
  • DovidioJFTen VergertMStewartTLPerspective and prejudice: antecedents and mediating mechanismsPers Soc Psychol Bull200430121537154915536238
  • ToddARBodenhausenGVGalinskyADPerspective taking combats the denial of intergroup discriminationJ Exp Soc Psychol2012483738745
  • ToddARBodenhausenGVRichesonJAGalinskyADPerspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial biasJ Pers Soc Psychol201110061027104221381852
  • ToddARBurgmerPPerspective taking and automatic intergroup evaluation change: testing an associative self-anchoring accountJ Pers Soc Psychol2013104578680223527849
  • ToddARGalinskyADPerspective-taking as a strategy for improving intergroup relations: evidence, mechanisms, and qualificationsSoc Personal Psychol Compass201487374387
  • FisherRUryWGetting to YesBostonHoughton Miffl1981
  • GalinskyADMussweilerTFirst offers as anchors: the role of perspective-taking and negotiator focusJ Pers Soc Psychol200181465766911642352
  • GalinskyADMadduxWWGilinDWhiteJBWhy it pays to get inside the head of your opponent: the differential effects of perspective taking and empathy in negotiationsPsychol Sci200819437838418399891
  • PruittDGStrategic choice in negotiationAm Behav Sci1983272167194
  • WaltonREMcKersieRBA Behavioral Theory of Labor NegotiationsNew YorkMcGraw-Hill1965
  • BatsonCDAltruism in HumansNew YorkOxford University Press2011
  • EssesVMDovidioJFThe role of emotions in determining willingness to engage in intergroup contactPers Soc Psychol Bull200228912021214
  • VescioTKSechristGBPaolucciMPPerspective taking and prejudice reduction: the mediational role of empathy arousal and situational attributionsEur J Soc Psychol2003334455472
  • Bar-TalDIntractable Conflicts: Socio-Psychological Foundations and DynamicsCambridgeCambridge University Press2013
  • MuthénBKaplanDA comparison of some methodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal Likert variablesBr J Math Stat Psychol1985382171189
  • GalinskyADCreating and reducing intergroup conflict: the role of perspective-taking in affecting outgroup evaluationsMannixNMAMannixEASondakHToward Phenomenology of Groups and Group MembershipGreenwich, CTJAI200285113
  • WangCSKennethTKuGGalinskyADPerspective-taking increases willingness to engage in intergroup contactPLoS One201491e8568124465648