271
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ORIGINAL RESEARCH

A Preliminary Validation of the Polish Version of the Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale (CIHS)

, ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 3627-3638 | Received 01 Jul 2022, Accepted 01 Oct 2022, Published online: 13 Dec 2022

Abstract

Background

In the last few years, empirical research on intellectual humility has grown notably, involving the elaboration of promising measures that provide a different outlook on the construct. Although all of them offer valid, theoretically sound, and meaningful contributions, we selected the 22-item Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale (CIHS) by Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse for validation. The rationale for choosing this questionnaire for Polish validation stands in its multidimensional nature, which enables the study of various nuances of this psychological concept.

Methods

The research was carried out with the participation of 260 adults (Study 1) and 210 adults (Study 2). The respondents completed a Polish translation of the original version of the CIHS, the Gratitude Questionnaire—Six Item Form (GQ-6), the General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES), and the Positive Orientation Scale (P-Scale).

Results

The findings obtained in both studies support the four-factor model of the CIHS with the higher order factor. The good fit indices of the CFA and MGCFA show the psychometric solidity of the 22-item structure of the Polish version of the CIHS. With respect to convergent validity, the validation study (Study 2) confirmed that gratitude, self-efficacy, and positive orientation are significant correlates of the CIHS.

Conclusion

Since intellectual humility is still a little-known psychological construct, both as a concept and as a possible antecedent or consequence, it would be worth examining it in the future with other variables of an intraindividual and interindividual nature.

Introduction

Intellectual humility belongs to a group of epistemic virtuesCitation1–5 that have been overlooked and understudied in the scholarly literature.Citation6–10 Recently, however, intellectual humility has attracted a lot of interest in the psychological sciences,Citation7,Citation9,Citation11,Citation12 resulting in the development of various conceptualizationsCitation13 and several measurement tools.Citation12,Citation14

Given that the research on intellectual humility is still in its beginnings,Citation15–17 there is no agreement on its precise descriptionCitation9,Citation18,Citation19 and a variety of definitions emphasize different features of this complex and multidimensional construct.Citation5,Citation20 Broadly speaking, intellectual humility has been considered as a subdomainCitation18,Citation21–23 or a more specific version of humility.Citation4,Citation11,Citation24 In fact, general and intellectual humility are often contrasted.Citation25 While general humility involves “an accurate view of one’s strengths and weaknesses”,Citation4 p. 215, intellectual humility refers mainly to being open to alternative points of viewCitation4 and recognizing the fallibility of one’s beliefs or knowledge.Citation12 Thus, general humility implies a realistic view of the self across events and relationships, and intellectual humility alludes to ideas in an intellectual domain or context.Citation2,Citation26,Citation27

Another division concerns implicit and explicit theories of intellectual humility. According to the former definitions that reflect “folk” comprehension,Citation28 intellectually humble people display a combination of cognitive (intelligence, curiosity, love of learning), self-oriented (modesty, not-a-showoff), and other-oriented (politeness, reliability, unselfishness) dimensions. Some individuals tend to confound humility with self-deception,Citation29,Citation30 self-abasement,Citation31,Citation32 weakness,Citation31,Citation33 low self-esteem,Citation6,Citation29 humiliation,Citation33 and ignorance.Citation29,Citation32 In turn, as attested by latter definitions represented by different philosophers and social scientists, intellectual humility tends to be understood somewhat distinctly.Citation15

A further review of the literature shows that most theories give intellectual humility the character of a traitCitation7,Citation11,Citation16,Citation34 and of a virtue.Citation11,Citation35 Some other researchers conceptualize intellectual humility as a state at a given timeCitation27 and in a particular situationCitation34 which consists in phenomenological experience. Nearly all definitions include two components of intellectual humility: intrapersonal and interpersonal.Citation22 Porter et alCitation36 even speak about a new framework of intellectual humility which advances in these two distinguished aspects.

The first one, called the self-focused feature, refers to one’s own knowledge and understanding. In this sense, intellectual humility is about an accurate or moderate perception of oneself as a knower.Citation2,Citation21,Citation23 According to Roberts and Wood,Citation37 intellectually humble individuals have a low consideration for self-importance and intellectual domination. Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse,Citation2 p. 210, define intellectual humility as “a nonthreatening awareness of one’s intellectual fallibility”. Likewise, Hill and Laney,Citation38 p. 243, consider intellectual humility as “a hypo-egoic phenomenon that involves a non-defensive willingness to see oneself accurately by acknowledging one’s personal limitations”. Viewed in this way, intellectual humility includes the consciousness and/or acceptance that one’s beliefs, knowledge or experience may be incomplete, erroneous, or mistaken.Citation30,Citation39,Citation40 Hook et alCitation26 consider this awareness as the intrapersonal dimension of intellectual humility that indicates a correct perception of one’s knowledge and beliefs. Some researchers underline that intellectual humility does not reflect only acknowledgment of one’s intellectual limitations. It also consists in being attentive to one’s intellectual strengths and abilities.Citation2,Citation21,Citation23 For example, Gregg and MahadevanCitation41 classify intellectual humility as a realistic evaluation of one’s epistemic competencies. Humble people can have positive thoughts and emotions about themselves,Citation42 and present good psychological adjustment.Citation31 Altogether, the intrapersonal aspect of intellectual humility is about personal awareness and impartial, non-defensive self-knowledge.Citation43

Other definitions stress the interpersonal character of intellectual humility, labeled as the other-focused feature, which implies others’ knowledge and understanding. McElroy et alCitation7 theorize that intellectual humility is relational in nature since it encompasses managing interplays with others. Porter et alCitation13,Citation44 speak about humility as a disposition to consider and appreciate others’ knowledge and intellectual potential. Thus, humble individuals seem to have an interpersonal stance that is other-oriented, rather than self-focused, characterized by respect for others.Citation2,Citation12,Citation20,Citation22

In the last few years, empirical research on intellectual humility has grown notably (), involving the elaboration of promising measures that provide a different outlook on the construct.Citation20,Citation45 Although all of them offer valid, theoretically sound, and meaningful contributions, we selected the 22-item Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale (CIHS) by Krumrei-Mancuso and RouseCitation2 for validation.

Table 1 Research Tools to Measure Intellectual Humility

The rationale for choosing this questionnaire for Polish validation stands above all in its multidimensional nature, which enables the study of various nuances of this psychological concept. Moreover, the CIHS is one of the first self-report and comprehensive scales which measures intellectual humility in both the intrapersonal and interpersonal domainsCitation2 and refers to social and epistemic dispositions.Citation46 From a psychometric perspective, both the four subdimensions and the overall score can be calculated for the CIHS.Citation2

The CIHS is a self-report questionnaire that consists of 22 items that reflect four facets: independence of intellect and ego, openness to revising one’s viewpoints, respect for others’ viewpoints, and lack of intellectual overconfidence.Citation2 Independence of intellect and ego enables an individual to be confident in their opinions.Citation2 In situations of disagreement or different views, such a person does not feel personally attacked, insignificant, or threatened. Openness to revising one’s viewpoint allows for the change of one’s important perspective when confronted with cogent and different evidence. Thus, intellectually humble people are disposed to change their opinion based on good, new, and convincing reasons or information. Respect for others’ viewpoints enables kind conversation even when discussing opposed issues. Such individuals, even if they disagree with others, still welcome different ways of thinking and esteem their interlocutors. A lack of intellectual overconfidence is based on an awareness of intellectual biases and having accurate intellectual self-regard. Humble individuals, listening to the perspectives of others, are eager to turn to them for expertise or to learn from them. In this sense, intellectual humility has both an intrapersonal and interpersonal character.Citation47

The CIHS, measuring intellectual humility and understood as a multi-faceted disposition, is one of the few scales that captures its various characteristics and thus provides a more graspable theoretical understanding.Citation18 The original study of the CIHSCitation2 shows that this measure is a reliable tool for investigating intellectual humility. Therefore, the main goal of Study 1 and Study 2 was to verify whether new datasets with Polish-speakers provide a similar goodness-of-fit index as the original model of the CIHS. Moreover, the aim of Study 2 was to demonstrate the convergent validity of the CIHS.

Methods and Materials

Study 1

Participants and Data Collection

The research was carried out with the participation of 260 adults (75.4% women). Their mean age was M = 20.52 with SD = 2.15 (range = 18–34 years). The data were gathered via the paper-and-pencil method through convenience sampling. This type of data collection was selected because of its simple, affordable, and prompt implementation. University students of psychology, pedagogy, national security, and economics were asked to take part in the research. They were informed of the goal of the study and were assured of the privacy protection policy. Those who agreed to participate in the study received extra credit in their classes. All of them provided fully informed, written consent. The study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Szczecin and carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Study Procedure

We applied a Polish translation of the original version of the Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale developed by Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse,Citation2 which contains 22 statements and estimates four dimensions of humility: intellect and ego, openness to revising one’s viewpoint, respect for others’ viewpoints, and lack of intellectual overconfidence. In the instructions, the participants were asked to read each statement and indicate their agreement or disagreement. All the items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale where 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree.

In terms of the forward-translation process, three independent, bilingual linguisticians translated the CIHS from English to Polish. Next, three psychologists assessed all aspects of the Polish versions of the CIHS and came to an agreement with respect to the items’ meanings or inadequate expressions. Afterward, twenty adults were invited to complete the Polish CIHS and state whether the statements were understandable. Finally, three different bilingual translators who previously did not know the questionnaire performed three back-translations that were found to be compliant with the English version of the CIHS. The final Polish edition, alongside the items of the CIHS original version, is available in the Appendix.

Statistical Analyses

Before carrying out the analyses to confirm the structure of the CIHS, we estimated a priori the appropriate sample size through G*Power 3.1.9.4,Citation48 using empirical evidence from the original research by Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse.Citation2 The studies prevalently demonstrated small and moderate correlations between the dimensions of the CIHS and other variables. Consequently, we computed the sample size given the power level of 0.90, critical significance of α = 0.05, and a small effect of 0.20. G*Power calculated that we would require at least 207 respondents in the study.

To test the underlying measurement model of the measure, we applied a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in Study 1 and Multigroup Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MGCFA) in Study 2. The measurement model comprised of four orthogonal latent factors, which were the indicators of a higher order latent variable. Although the scale is comprised of five categories, we treated the data as categorical given the unequal distribution of answers. As a result, we used polychoric correlation matrices and applied the Weighted Least Squares with Means and Variances adjusted (WLSMV) estimation method. No correlations between the residuals were added. To evaluate the CFA model fit, we relied on standard recommendations. That is, we deemed the analyzed model to be well fitted to the data if the values of the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) were ≥ 0.90 and the values of the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) were ≤ 0.08.Citation49,Citation50

In the MGCFA, we compared whether the measurement model found in Study 1 is invariant with the data from Study 2. For this purpose, we evaluated two subsequent models with an increasing level of constraints: configural and scalar.Citation50 The configural model is an unconditional model, where no constraints are imposed. In the scalar model, not only the values of factor loadings but also the item intercepts are constrained to be equal across the compared groups. Although it is possible to find an in-between model in the literature (ie, metric model), it is recommended to directly compare the configural model with the scalar in the assessment of categorical data. To evaluate if the results across studies are invariant, we relied on standard recommendations.Citation51,Citation52 Thus, we deemed a model as invariant if the values of the ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA did not exceed 0.010. The analyses were carried out in the lavaan package.Citation53

Finally, we tested the convergent validity of the CIHS to verify whether intellectual humility correlates positively with gratitude, self-efficacy, and positive orientation. The theoretical foundations for a potential positive association between intellectual humility and gratitude rely on the fact that both of these constructs involve an other-oriented stance,Citation54 expressing the capacity to consider others’ points of view.Citation55 In fact, Krumrei-MancusoCitation56 suggests that intellectual humility may act as a precursor to experiencing gratitude. In turn, Kruse et alCitation57 support that expressing gratitude is an antecedent or elicitor of humility. The authors demonstrate that there may be a strengthening relationship between humility and gratitude. Another facet that connects both constructs concerns appreciation. Intellectual humility, in its interpersonal component, means appreciating others’ intellectual capacities.Citation13 Gratitude is considered to be one of the aspects of appreciation which per se denotes acknowledging the value of something or someone.Citation58 Given the common characteristics, a positive correlation can be expected between intellectual humility and gratitude.

Next, potential correlations between intellectual humility and self-efficacy can be based on self-expansion theory. According to this model, people are inherently motivated to extend their potential efficacy through seeking new things, acquiring new knowledge, or gaining a new competence.Citation59 Given that intellectual humility implicates openness to new paradigmsCitation60 and openness to revising one’s viewpoint,Citation2,Citation39 involves gainingCitation11 and sharing new knowledge,Citation61 and facilitates the development of new skills,Citation60 we assumed that both constructs would be positively associated. Moreover, the rationale behind choosing self-efficacy stems from the fact that we know little empirically about the possible relationships between these variables. For example, Porter et alCitation44 note that it is necessary to know to what extent the two variables are similar or, on the contrary, differ from each other.

Finally, a positive correlation between intellectual humility and positive orientation may be based on research that confirms that humble people tend to have positive thoughts about themselvesCitation42 and present self-knowledge non-defensively.Citation43 Hook et alCitation26 also observe that they are likely to perceive the needs of others and regulate social bonds. Thus, a positive attitude toward oneself and others may distinguish intellectually humble people. Moreover, previous studies have shown that intellectual humility is positively associated with variables that form the essence of a positive outlook, such as life satisfaction,Citation62,Citation63 self-esteemCitation18 and optimism,Citation28 but there is no research investigating the relationship between intellectual humility and positive orientation.

Results

Assessment of the Measurement Model

The fit indices of the analyzed four-factor model with the higher order factor suggested an optimal fit to the data (χ2(205) = 510.55; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.923; RMSEA = 0.076 [90% CI = 0.068, 0.084]). The descriptive statistics, item-total correlations and the standardized factor loadings are presented in . The strength of the factor loadings on all hypothesized factors was adequateCitation64 (ie, ≥ 0.30), with only one item (ie, item 2) loading weaker than 0.40 and only two other items (ie, items 4 and 11) loading weaker than 0.50. The estimates of internal consistency were good for all the analyzed factors except overconfidence, which was acceptable. The respect factor appeared to be the best indicator of the higher order latent variable, while overconfidence was the weakest. The provided results support the hypothesized measurement model.

Table 2 Descriptive Statistics, Item-Total Correlations and Standardized Factor Loadings

Study 2

Participants and Data Collection

The research was conducted on a group of 210 adults (89.5% women). The mean age of the respondents was M = 21.45 with SD = 1.95 (range = 18–27 years). The data were gathered via the paper-and-pencil method through convenience sampling, similarly to Study 1. University students of psychology and pedagogy were asked to participate in the research. They were informed of the aim of the study and were assured of the privacy protection policy. All of them provided fully informed, written consent. The procedure applied in Study 2 was consistent with the approach used in Study 1.

Measurement

The Polish CIHS from Study 1 was administrated in Study 2.

The Gratitude Questionnaire—Six Item Form (GQ-6), created by McCullough et al,Citation65 and adapted into Polish by Kossakowska and Kwiatek,Citation66 estimates individual differences in the disposition to experience gratitude in daily life. It is a concise, six-item tool where the participants indicate their answers on a 7-point scale where 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree. A higher score implies a higher degree of gratitude. Consistent with the original research on gratitude, the reliability of the questionnaire in our study was α = 0.84.

The General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES), developed by Schwarzer and Jerusalem and adapted into Polish by Juczyński,Citation67 is a brief, 10-item scale that measures the respondent’s beliefs concerning their expectations of their confidence to confront the various daily activities that could produce stress. The respondents assess each of the ten statements using a 4-point Likert scale (from 1 = no to 4 = yes). The possible range of scores is between 4 and 40. The higher the final result, the stronger the general self-efficacy. Various studies provide good coefficient alphas from 0.76 to 0.90. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.91.

The Positive Orientation Scale (P-Scale), originated by Caprara et alCitation68 and adapted into Polish by Łaguna et al,Citation69 is a short, single-factor measure that assesses the positive evaluation of oneself, one’s life, and the future. The scale is composed of 8 items (with one reverse-coded item: “At times, the future seems unclear to me”). The participants rate their tendency to express positive judgments using a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Different studies show that the P-Scale has very good psychometric properties. In the current study, the overall Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.86.

Results

Assessment of the Measurement Model

In Study 2, the expected measurement model was also well-fitted to the data (χ2(205) = 440.78; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.937; RMSEA = 0.074 [90% CI = 0.065, 0.084]). The estimates of the internal consistency were consistent with those reported in Study 1 and were as follows: αIndependence = 0.86 (0.83, 0.89); αOpenness = 0.77 (0.72, 0.82); αRespect = 0.77 (0.72, 0.82); αOverconfidence = 0.61 (0.53, 0.69). To evaluate the extent to which the results from Study 2 are comparable to those reported in Study 1, we conducted the MGCFA. The fit of the configural model was adequate (χ2(410) = 943.40; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.931; RMSEA = 0.075 [90% CI = 0.068, 0.081]) as was the scalar model (χ2(491) = 1080.97; p < 0.001; CFI = 0.923; RMSEA = 0.072 [90% CI = 0.066, 0.077)]. The overall difference between the analyzed models was within the assumed range, that is, ΔCFI = 0.008 and ΔRMSEA = 0.003. Thus, the measurement model of the scale across both studies can be seen as fully invariant.

Convergent Validity

As shown in , convergent validity was assessed by measuring the correlation (Pearson correlation coefficient) between the four subscales of the Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale, gratitude, self-efficacy, and positive orientation.

Table 3 Correlations Between Dimensions/Overall Score of CIHS, GQ-6, GSES, and P-Scale

There was a positive correlation of gratitude with all the dimensions and the overall score of intellectual humility, indicating that the more grateful an individual is, the more they tend to declare intellectual humility in its four dimensions (independence of intellect and ego, openness to revising one’s viewpoints, respect for others’ viewpoints, lack of intellectual overconfidence). Self-efficacy correlated positively with IIE, ORV, ROV and IHO, and was negatively associated with LIO. In addition, positive orientation was positively associated with IIE, ROV and IHO.

Discussion

Given the importance of intellectual humility in different spheres of personal and social life, we conducted two studies to validate the CIHS into Polish. To our knowledge, the present project is the first attempt to examine the latent structure of the original CIHS and verify whether new datasets provide similar goodness-of-fit indexes as the original CIHS. We also confirmed the scale’s convergent validity.

The findings obtained in both studies corroborate the results of Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse,Citation2 thus supporting the four-factor model of the CIHS with the higher order factor. The good fit indices of the CFA and MGCFA show the psychometric solidity of the 22-item structure of the Polish version of the CIHS. The reliability values, like the original ones for the four subscales, support the internal consistency of the measure. The outcomes indicate that the Polish version of the scale is a reliable tool and manifests similar psychometric characteristics to Krumrei-Mancuso’s version. Therefore, the CIHS, in its four dimensions of independence of intellect and ego, openness to revising one’s viewpoint, respect for others’ viewpoints, and lack of intellectual overconfidence, can be used to assess intellectual humility.

With respect to convergent validity, all the dimensions/overall score of intellectual humility represented in the CIHS correlated with grateful disposition and self-efficacy. Such results are understandable considering that intellectual humility shares the interpersonal dimension with gratitude and the intrapersonal dimension with self-efficacy. According to Ballantyne,Citation27 intellectual humility, like gratitude, concerns the prosocial aspects of life and relationships with others. Wong and WongCitation70 suggest that both constructs entail other-oriented components. Humility consists in a lack of self-focus and reflects an appreciation for others.Citation57 Likewise, gratitude has been demonstrated to correlate negatively with self-focused attentionCitation71 and is considered an appreciation of other people.Citation72 Gratitude belongs to the group of other-focused emotions.Citation57,Citation73 Moreover, humble people tend to be less self-oriented and more open to the needs of others.Citation10,Citation57

A similar pattern of correlations has been observed between the three dimensions of intellectual humility (independence of intellect and ego, openness to revising one’s viewpoints, respect for others’ viewpoints), its overall score, and self-efficacy, with the exception of a negative relationship between lack of intellectual overconfidence and self-efficacy. The results of our research are in conformity with some previous empirical studies. For example, humility has been found to be not only a positive correlate of self-efficacyCitation74,Citation75 but its positive predictor,Citation76 as well. Although humility is considered as a “de-centering of the self”,Citation77 p. 5, it does not involve an absence of self-confidence. People who are humble seem to have a realistic, accurate self-concept,Citation74,Citation78 and secure self-identity.Citation78 Also noteworthy is the inverse association between lack of intellectual overconfidence and self-efficacy. Although there are no similar studies on this topic, it can be assumed that lack of intellectual overconfidence correlates with lower intellectual confidence and therefore lower self-esteem.

Finally, independence of intellect and ego, respect for others’ viewpoints, and intellectual humility overall correlated positively with positive orientation. It can be assumed that the broaden-and-build model developed by FredricksonCitation79 is a theoretical perspective that may help explain the different correlations between these constructs. According to this approach, positive emotions seem to enlarge people’s range of thought-action choices and strengthen their personal resources. If intellectual humility consists in being confident of one’s own opinion, welcoming different ways of thinking and not feeling personally attacked by them, it can be implicit that people who are intellectually humble tend to distinguish themselves by a positive orientation toward the self and life in general. From an empirical point of view, since there is no research on the relationship between intellectual humility and positive orientation, we considered those studies that combine intellectual humility with life satisfaction, self-esteem, and optimism. It is these variables that form the core of positive orientation. For example, KrauseCitation80 and Rowatt et alCitation33 speak about the positive association between humility and life satisfaction. This relationship is further explained by levels of wisdom because it is stronger among individuals who show higher levels of wisdom than among people who tend to display its lower levels. There is also some evidence that humility positively correlates with self-esteem.Citation81 Alfano et alCitation18 suggest that different dimensions of intellectual humility (open-mindedness, modesty, corrigibility, engagement) correlate positively with self-esteem. Bak and KutnikCitation82 point out that self-esteem predicts the four dimensions of intellectual humility described by Krumrei-Mancuso and RouseCitation2 and the three dimensions (love of learning, appropriate discomfort with limitations, owning intellectual limitations) proposed by Haggard et al.Citation14 In another study,Citation33 humility is positively correlated with self-esteem and with optimism. Self-esteem correlates negatively with humility considered as embarrassment or humiliation, and positively with humility understood as good adjustment.Citation31

Moreover, positive orientation did not correlate significantly with openness to revising one’s viewpoints (r = 0.09; p = 0.11) or lack of intellectual overconfidence (r = −0.11; p = 0.08). As can be noted, both results exceed the threshold of p = 0.05Citation83 but are within the conventional range of tendency (0.05 < p < 0.1) so we interpret these findings with caution, assuming that with a larger research group, these results may reduce to significant. Based on the outcomes, it can be tentatively assumed that people with a positive perception of themselves and the world may tend to be open to revising their perspective (eg, when confronted with solid and credible alternative evidence) and may express a lower awareness of intellectual biases and accurate intellectual self-regard. There is some empirical evidence that positive orientation correlates with openness to experience.Citation84,Citation85 Such a result is not surprising if we think that openness to experience and openness to revising one’s viewpoints likely share a common variance. However, there are also some studies that, like the current study, show an association between positive orientation and openness to experience at the level of tendency.Citation86–88 Regarding the inverse correlation between positive orientation and the lack of intellectual overconfidence, it can be assumed that a positive view of oneself is not always related to having accurate intellectual self-regard.

Limitations

Some limitations need to be mentioned. Both samples were predominantly female. While this trend is often seen in research projects, it would be valuable to ensure an equal ratio of women to men in the future. The research was mainly conducted among young adults studying at university. While such a choice is good as a starting point in the context of intellectual humility, it is worth extending the research groups to people representing different stages of development and professional groups. Moreover, although important correlates were included in the convergent validity, in future research conducted in the Polish context, the selection of variables could be extended to those that are more closely connected to the definition of intellectual humility (eg, overclaiming, need for cognition, dogmatism).

From a psychometric point of view, the relatively lower reliability (compared to the values obtained in the original American version) obtained for the lack of overconfidence undoubtedly deserves attention in future analyses. Although too soon to conclude, there may be some cultural differences in understanding and experiencing the concept of overconfidence between Americans and Poles. At least two things seem to support this line of thought. First, in the present study, the values of lack of overconfidence gained in the Polish samples were lower than these obtained by American respondents (for the remaining CIHS subscales, the results were more similar). Second, there are studies confirming the existence of differences between the regions of the world in terms of overconfidence.Citation89 Therefore, further research is needed to verify how this subscale operates in the Polish context. Another factor that may have made lack of overconfidence less reliable is the lack of clarity of the construct of overconfidence itself. According to Moore and Healy,Citation90 this term has been researched in distinct ways (most commonly overestimation vs overprecision vs overplacement) often leading to inconsistent results. In future analyses, it would be good to verify empirically which of these meanings most closely corresponds to lack of overconfidence.

Conclusion

This validation study showed that the CIHS is not only a suitable tool for measuring intellectual humility but also confirmed that gratitude, self-efficacy, and positive orientation are its significant correlates. Since intellectual humility is still a little-known psychological construct, both as a concept and as a possible antecedent or consequence, it is worth examining it in the future with other variables of an intraindividual and interindividual nature. Moreover, both studies present the cross-cultural validation of the scale and the expansion of intellectual humility research to another country and culture outside of the United States.

Data Sharing Statement

The datasets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to data analysis, drafting or revising the article, have agreed on the journal to which the article will be submitted, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

Acknowledgment

The authors thank the study participants.

References

  • Spiegel JS. Open-mindedness and intellectual humility. Theory Res Educ. 2012;10(1):27–38. doi:10.1177/1477878512437472
  • Krumrei-Mancuso EJ, Rouse SV. The development and validation of the comprehensive intellectual humility scale. J Pers Assess. 2016;98(2):209–221. doi:10.1080/00223891.2015.1068174
  • Kidd IJ. Intellectual humility, confidence, and argumentation. Topoi. 2016;35(2):395–402. doi:10.1007/s11245-015-9324-5
  • Davis DE, Rice K, McElroy S, et al. Distinguishing intellectual humility and general humility. J Posit Psychol. 2016;11(3):215–224. doi:10.1080/17439760.2015.1048818
  • Jarvinen MJ, Paulus TB. Attachment and cognitive openness: emotional underpinnings of intellectual humility. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(1):74–86. doi:10.1080/17439760.2016.1167944
  • Tangney JP. Humility: theoretical perspectives, empirical findings and directions for future research. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2000;19(1):70–82. doi:10.1521/jscp.2000.19.1.70
  • McElroy SE, Rice KG, Davis DE, et al. Intellectual humility: scale development and theoretical elaborations in the context of religious leadership. J Psychol Theol. 2014;42(1):19–30. doi:10.1177/009164711404200103
  • Weidman AC, Cheng JT, Tracy JL. The psychological structure of humility. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2018;114(1):153–178. doi:10.1037/pspp0000112
  • Hook JN, Farrell JE, Johnson KA, Van Tongeren DR, Davis DE, Aten JD. Intellectual humility and religious tolerance. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(1):29–35. doi:10.1080/17439760.2016.1167937
  • Chancellor J, Lyubomirsky S. Humble beginnings: current trends, state perspectives, and hallmarks of humility. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2013;7(11):819–833. doi:10.1111/spc3.12069
  • Krumrei-Mancuso EJ, Haggard MC, LaBouff JP, Rowatt WC. Links between intellectual humility and acquiring knowledge. J Posit Psychol. 2020;15(2):155–170. doi:10.1080/17439760.2019.1579359
  • Zmigrod L, Zmigrod S, Rentfrow PJ, Robbins TW. The psychological roots of intellectual humility: the role of intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Pers Individ Differ. 2019;141(15):200–208. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.016
  • Porter T, Schumann K. Intellectual humility and openness to the opposing view. Self Identity. 2018;17(2):139–162. doi:10.1080/15298868.2017.1361861
  • Haggard M, Rowatt WC, Leman JC, et al. Finding middle ground between intellectual arrogance and intellectual servility: development and assessment of the limitations-owning intellectual humility scale. Pers Individ Differ. 2018;124:184–193. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.014
  • Samuelson PL, Jarvinen MJ, Paulus TB, Church IM, Hardy SA, Barrett JL. Implicit theories of intellectual virtues and vices: a focus on intellectual humility. J Posit Psychol. 2014;10(5):1–18. doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.967802
  • Reis HT, Lee KY, O’Keefe SD, Clark MS. Perceived partner responsiveness promotes intellectual humility. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;79:21–33. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2018.05.006
  • Danovitch JH, Fisher M, Schroder H, Hambrick DZ, Moser J. Intelligence and neurophysiological markers of error monitoring relate to children’s intellectual humility. Child Dev. 2019;90(3):924–939. doi:10.1111/cdev.12960
  • Alfano M, Iurino K, Stey P, et al. Development and validation of a multidimensional measure of intellectual humility. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182950. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0182950
  • Barrett JL. Intellectual humility. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(1):1–2. doi:10.1080/17439760.2016.1167945
  • Hoyle RH, Davisson EK, Diebels KJ, Leary MR. Holding specific views with humility: conceptualization and measurement of specific intellectual humility. Pers Individ Differ. 2016;97:165–172. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.043
  • Davis DE, Hook JN. Humility, religion, and spirituality: an endpiece. J Psychol Theol. 2014;42(1):111–117. doi:10.1177/009164711404200112
  • Davis DE, Hook JN, Worthington EL, et al. Relational humility: conceptualizing and measuring humility as a personal judgment. J Pers Assess. 2011;93(3):225–234. doi:10.1080/00223891.2011.558871
  • Meagher BR, Leman JC, Bias JP, Latendresse SJ, Rowatt WC. Contrasting self-report and consensus ratings of intellectual humility and arrogance. J Res Pers. 2015;58:35–45. doi:10.1016/j.rpg.2015.07.002
  • McElroy-Heltzel S, Davis DE, DeBlaere C, Worthington EL, Hook JN. Embarrassment of riches in the measurement of humility: a critical review of 22 measures. J Posit Psychol. 2019;14(3):393–404. doi:10.1080/17439760.2018.1460686
  • Wang J, Yang X. Intellectual humility and owning one’s limitations. FJHSS. 2019;12:353–369. doi:10.1007/s40647-019-00260-8
  • Hook JN, Davis DE, Van Tongeren DR, et al. Intellectual humility and forgiveness of religious leaders. J Posit Psychol. 2015;10(6):499–506. doi:10.1080/17439760.2015.1004554
  • Ballantyne N. Recent work on intellectual humility: a philosopher’s perspective. J Posit Psychol. 2021;1–21. doi:10.1080/17439760.2021.1940252
  • Church IM, Samuelson PL. Intellectual Humility: An Introduction to the Philosophy and Science. London: Bedford Square; 2017.
  • Richards N. Is humility a virtue? Am Philos Q. 1988;25(3):253–259.
  • Wright JC, Nadelhoffer T, Perini T, Langville A, Echols M, Venezia K. The psychological significance of humility. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(1):3–12. doi:10.1080/17439760.2016.1167940
  • Exline JJ, Geyer AL. Perceptions of humility: a preliminary study. Self Identity. 2004;3(2):95–114. doi:10.1080/13576500342000077
  • Spezio M, Peterson G, Roberts RC. Humility as openness to others: interactive humility in the context of l’Arche. J Moral Educ. 2018;48(2):1–20. doi:10.1080/03057240.2018.1444982
  • Rowatt WC, Powers C, Targhetta V, Comer J, Kennedy S, Labouff J. Development and initial validation of an implicit measure of humility relative to arrogance. J Posit Psychol. 2006;1(4):198–211. doi:10.1080/17439760600885671
  • Zachry CE, Phan LV, Blackie LER, Jayawickreme E. Situation-based contingencies underlying wisdom-content manifestations: examining intellectual humility in daily life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2018;73(8):1404–1415. doi:10.1093/geronb/gby016
  • Hill PC, Dunnington K, Lewis Hall ME. Glad intellectual dependence on God: a theistic account of intellectual humility. J Psychol Christ. 2018;37(3):195–204.
  • Porter T, Baldwin CR, Warren MT, et al. Clarifying the content of intellectual humility: a systematic review and integrative framework. J Pers Assess. 2021;27:1–13. doi:10.1080/00223891.2021.1975725
  • Roberts CR, Wood WJ. Humility and epistemic goods. In: DePaul M, Zagzebski L, editors. Intellectual Virtue: Perspectives from Ethics and Epistemology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003:257–279.
  • Hill PC, Laney EK. Beyond self-interest: humility and the quieted self. In: Brown KW, Leary MR, editors. Oxford Handbook of Hypo-Egoic Phenomena. New York: Oxford University Press; 2016:243–256.
  • Deffler SA, Leary MR, Hoyle RH. Knowing what you know: intellectual humility and judgments of recognition memory. Pers Individ Differ. 2016;96:255–259. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.016
  • Johnson CR. Intellectual humility and empathy by analogy. Topoi. 2019;38(6):221–228. doi:10.1007/s11245-017-9453-0
  • Gregg AP, Mahadevan N. Intellectual arrogance and intellectual humility: an evolutionary-epistemological account. J Psychol Theol. 2014;42(1):7–18. doi:10.1177/009164711404200102
  • Exline JJ, Campbell WK, Baumeister RF, Joiner T, Krueger J. The Values in Action (VIA) classification of strengths. In: Peterson C, Seligman M, editors. A Life Worth Living: Contributions to Positive Psychology. Cincinnati, OH: Values in Action Institute; 2004:461–475.
  • Argandoña A. Humility in management. J Bus Ethics. 2014;132(1):63–71. doi:10.1007/s10551-014-2311-8
  • Porter T, Schumann K, Selmeczy D, Trzesniewski K. Intellectual humility predicts mastery behaviors when learning. Learn Individ Differ. 2020;80. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2020.101888
  • Senger AR, Huynh HP. Intellectual humility’s association with vaccine attitudes and intentions. Psychol Health Med. 2020;16:1–10. doi:10.1080/13548506.2020.1778753
  • Westbrook C. The validation of the general Intellectual Humility Scale as a measure of intellectual humility [dissertation]. Georgia State University; 2022. doi:10.57709/26675627.
  • Hill PC, Lewis Hall ME, Wang D, Decker LA. Theistic intellectual humility and well-being: does ideological context matter? J Posit Psychol. 2019;16(2):155–167. doi:10.1080/17439760.2019.1689424
  • Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–191. doi:10.3758/bf03193146
  • Byrne BM. Structural Equation Modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1994.
  • Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Müller H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol Res. 2003;8(2):23–74.
  • Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58:525–543. doi:10.1007/BF02294825
  • Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14(3):464–504. doi:10.1080/10705510701301834
  • Rosseel Y. Iavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i02
  • Dwiwardani C, Hill PC, Bollinger RA, et al. Virtues develop from a secure base: attachment and resilience as predictors of humility, gratitude, and forgiveness. J Psychol Theol. 2014;42(1):83–90. doi:10.1177/009164711404200109
  • Merçon-Vargas EA, Poelker KE, Tudge JRH. The development of the virtue of gratitude: theoretical foundations and cross-cultural issues. Cross Cult Res. 2018;52(1):3–18. doi:10.1177/1069397117736517
  • Krumrei-Mancuso EJ. Intellectual humility and prosocial values: direct and mediated effects. J Posit Psychol. 2017;12(1):13–28. doi:10.1080/17439760.2016.1167938
  • Kruse E, Chancellor J, Ruberton PM, Lyubomirsky S. An upward spiral between gratitude and humility. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2014;5(7):805–814. doi:10.1177/1948550614534700
  • Fagley NS. The construct of appreciation: it is so much more than gratitude. In: Perspectives on Gratitude: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2016:70–84.
  • Aron A, Norman CC, Aron EN. The self-expansion model and motivation. Represent Res Soc Psychol. 1998;22:1–13.
  • Trinch MP. Overcoming the shadow of expertise: how humility and learning goal orientation help knowledge leaders become more flexible. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2505. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02505
  • Anand A, Walsh I, Moffett S. Does humility facilitate knowledge sharing? Investigating the role of humble knowledge inquiry and response. J Knowl Manag. 2019;23(6):1218–1244. doi:10.1108/JKM-06-2018-0353
  • Worthington EL, Goldstein L, Hammock B, et al. Humility: a qualitative review. In: The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology. Oxford Academic; 2021:643–656. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199396511.013.39
  • Porter T, Elnakouri A, Meyers EA, Shibayama T, Jayawickreme E, Grossmann I. Predictors and consequences of intellectual humility. Nat Rev Psychol. 2022;1(9):524–536. doi:10.1038/s44159-022-00081-9
  • Schmitt TA, Sass DA. Rotation criteria and hypothesis testing for exploratory factor analysis: implications for factor pattern loadings and interfactor correlations. Educ Psychol Meas. 2011;71(1):95–113. doi:10.1177/0013164410387348
  • McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang JA. The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82(1):112–127. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112
  • Kossakowska M, Kwiatek P. Polska adaptacja kwestionariusza do badania wdzięczności GQ-6. Przeglad Psychol. 2014;57(4):503–514.
  • Juczyński Z. Skala Uogólnionej Własnej Skuteczności [Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP; 2009.
  • Caprara GV, Alessandri G, Eisenberg N, Kupfer A. The positivity scale. Psychol Assess. 2012;24(3):701–712. doi:10.1037/a0026681
  • Łaguna M, Oleś P, Filipiuk D. Orientacja pozytywna i jej pomiar: polska adaptacja Skali Orientacji Pozytywnej [Positive orientation and its measure: Polish adaptation of the Positivity Scale]. Stud Psychol. 2011;49(4):47–54. doi:10.2478/v10167-010-0035-7
  • Wong IHM, Wong TTY. Exploring the relationship between intellectual humility and academic performance among post-secondary students: the mediating roles of learning motivation and receptivity to feedback. Learn Individ Differ. 2021;88(4):102012. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102012
  • Mathews MA, Green JD. Looking at me, appreciating you: self-focused attention distinguishes between gratitude and indebtedness. Cogn Emot. 2010;24(4):710–718. doi:10.1080/02699930802650796
  • Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. Gratitude and well-being: a review and theoretical integration. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(7):890–905. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005
  • Algoe SB, Stanton AL. Gratitude when it is needed most: social functions of gratitude in women with metastatic breast cancer. Emotion. 2012;12(1):163–168. doi:10.1037/a0024024
  • Chen L. Linking leader personality traits to motivation to lead: a self-concept approach. Soc Behav Pers. 2016;44(11):1913–1926. doi:10.2224/sbp.2016.44.11.1913
  • Crabtree SA, Bell CA, Rupert DA, Sandage SJ, Devor NG, Stavros G. Humility, differentiation of self, and clinical training in spiritual and religious competence. J Spiritual Ment Health. 2021;23(4):342–362. doi:10.1080/19349637.2020.1737627
  • Sezgin F, Edroğan O. Humility and forgiveness as predictors of teacher self-efficacy. Educ Res Rev. 2018;13(4):120–128. doi:10.5897/ERR2017.3449
  • Ross LT, Wright JC. Humility, personality, and psychological functioning. Psychol Rep. 2021;332941211062819. doi:10.1177/00332941211062819
  • Ruberton PM, Kruse E, Lyubomirsky S. Boosting state humility via gratitude, self-affirmation, and awe: theoretical and empirical perspectives. In: Worthington EL, Davis D, Hook JN, editors. Handbook of Humility: Theory, Research, and Applications. New York: Routledge; 2017:260–273.
  • Fredrickson BL, Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1367–1378. doi:10.1098/rstb.2004.1512
  • Krause N. Assessing the relationship among wisdom, humility, and life satisfaction. J Adult Dev. 2016;23(3):140–149. doi:10.1007/s10804-016-9230-0
  • Landrum RE. Measuring dispositional humility: a first approximation. Psychol Rep. 2011;108(1):217–228. doi:10.2466/02.07.09.PRO.108.1.217-228
  • Bak W, Kutnik J. Domains of intellectual humility: self-esteem and narcissism as independent predictors. Pers Individ Differ. 2021;177:110815. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2021.110815
  • Amrhein V, Korner-Nievergelt FK, Roth T. The earth is flat (p > 0.05): significance threshold and the crisis of unreplicable research. PeerJ. 2017;5:e3544. doi:10.7717/peerj.3544
  • Szcześniak M, Potemkowski A, Brola W, et al. The Big Five personality traits and positive orientation in Polish adults with multiple sclerosis: the role of meaning in life. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(9):5426. doi:10.3390/ijerph19095426
  • Przepiorka A, Siu NY-F, Szcześniak M, Timoszyk-Tomczak C, Jiaying J, Pino Muñoz M. The relation between personality, time perspective and positive orientation in Chile, Hong Kong, and Poland. J Happiness Stud. 2020;21(3):1081–1101. doi:10.1007/s10902-019-00113-x
  • Miciuk ŁR, Jankowski T, Laskowska A, Oleś P. Positive orientation and the five-factor model. Pol Psychol Bull. 2016;47(1):141–148. doi:10.1515/ppb-2016-0016
  • Miciuk ŁR, Jankowski T, Oleś P. Incremental validity of positive orientation: predictive efficiency beyond the five-factor model. Health Psychol Rep. 2016;4(4):294–302. doi:10.5114/hpr.2016.59895
  • Laguna M, De Longis E, Mazur-Socha Z, Alessandri G. Explaining prosocial behavior from the inter- and within-individual perspectives: a role of positive orientation and positive affect. J Happiness Stud. 2022;23:1599–1615. doi:10.1007/s10902-021-00464-4
  • Stankov L, Lee J. Overconfidence across world regions. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2014;45(5):821–837. doi:10.1177/0022022114527345
  • Moore DA, Healy PJ. The trouble with overconfidence. Psychol Rev. 2008;115(2):502–517. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.2.502