520
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Emotional and Behavioral Changes in Preschool Firstborn Children During Transition to Siblinghood: A Mixed Methods Study

, , , , , & show all
Pages 2029-2044 | Received 09 Mar 2023, Accepted 31 May 2023, Published online: 03 Jun 2023

Abstract

Purpose

To help firstborn children in families expecting a second child navigate the role transition more smoothly, we investigated the emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children during the transition to siblinghood (TTS) and the factors that contribute to these changes.

Patients and Methods

From March to December 2019, a total of 97 firstborn children (Mage=3.00± 0.97, and female = 51) were included in the study through a questionnaire survey of their mothers, and two follow-up visits were conducted in Chongqing, China. Individual in-depth interviews were conducted with 14 mothers.

Results

Both quantitative and qualitative results suggest that emotional and behavioral problems of firstborn children tend to increase during TTS, particularly in issues such as anxiety/depression, somatic complaints, withdrawal, sleep problems, attention problems, and aggressive behavior, as well as internalization problems, externalization problems and total problems in the quantitative study (P<0.05). A poor father-child relationship may increase emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children (P=0.05). Further qualitative analysis found that younger age and outgoing personality of the firstborn child may improve the emotional and behavioral problems.

Conclusion

The firstborn children did have more emotional and behavioral problems during TTS. But these problems can be regulated by family factors and their own characteristics.

Introduction

Preschoolers are in a period of rapid brain development and are beginning to display profound emotional development. Physiologically, the brain has increased structures involved in interoception and emotion,Citation1,Citation2 and the ability of children to recognize emotion develops rapidly at preschool age.Citation3,Citation4 As preschool children have more opportunities to contact the outside world, their emotions, behaviors, and personalities are also easily affected.Citation5 Transition to siblinghood (TTS) refers to the process wherein a child becomes a sibling. About 90% of Western children have at least one sibling, and most of them experienced the birth of a younger sibling during their preschool years.Citation6,Citation7 TTS is a process of family structural adjustment that can have some positive or negative changes on older siblings, such as reduced parent-child interactions and emotional adjustment ability or improved language expression ability.Citation8 The vast majority of siblings had been at odds with each other in words or actions, want more attention, and engage in regressive behavior. If not handled properly, it may cause psychological problems for the child in the future.Citation9 Persistent negative influences in early childhood are associated with depression in adolescence and even mental health problems in adulthood.Citation4,Citation10 Hence, it is an important public issue to explore the emotional and behavioral changes in firstborn children during TTS to reduce influences on emotional and behavioral problems and help their transition to sibling relationships more smoothly.

Theory of Emotional and Behavioral Changes in Firstborn Children During TTS

How will the emotion and behavior of the firstborn child change upon the arrival of the second child, and will it have more positive or negative effects? Researchers have held different views on these issues, and there are some controversial theoretical ideas.Citation11

According to past empirical evidence, the birth of the second child may be a stressful event, and the firstborn child needs to adapt to the new role after the birth of the second child, which will cause certain psychological pressure and psychological threat to the firstborn child and will lead to a variety of negative emotions and behaviors.Citation12,Citation13 Firstborns would experience sadness, anxiety, depression, and other negative emotions.Citation14,Citation15 Under certain circumstances, firstborn children are indifferent to their mothers, and the mother-child attachment becomes unstable.Citation16–18 Firstborn children exhibit more dependent behavior, sleep problems, misbehavior, and regressive behavior than only child.Citation19 Cortisol is a biomarker of the stress response system in mammals. After the birth of a younger sibling, urinary cortisol levels in older wild bonobos increased fivefold and remained elevated for seven months.Citation20 Which provides physiological evidence that TTS is a stressful event for firstborns.

However, Volling’s team has a contrary view on this issue in their several years studies with a dozen papers, including a large SRCD monograph.Citation21 Her research does not support the idea that TTS is a crisis event and the impact of the arrival of a second child is acceptable for most firstborn children and families.Citation22,Citation23 Although he firstborn children may have severe adjustment difficulties during TTS, it is limited to a very small number of children. The challenge it can turn to an opportunity for further growth and development for younger children.Citation11 The arrival of the second child increases the willingness of the firstborn to share, collaborate, and help take care of the second child,Citation24,Citation25 improving self-care,Citation26 and enhancing verbal expression.Citation15

The Influencing Factors of Children’s Emotions and Behaviors

The factors that influence changes in firstborns come mainly from the individual level and the family level.Citation27 At the individual level, children’s emotions and behaviors are attached to their intrinsic characteristics.Citation11 Differences in gender, age, and temperament may cause firstborn children to exhibit different emotional and behavioral problems.Citation11 For younger and bad-tempered firstborns, emotional and behavioral problems are more pronounced. And there may be more problems with siblings of the same gender.Citation15 Meanwhile, children’s emotional and behavioral problems are closely linked to family factors. Family structure, parental relationship, parent-child attachment, parenting style, and family economic status are the main factors that produce differences in children’s emotions and behaviors.Citation28–30 Lack of parent-child interaction, maternal mental illness, and poor family environment are all associated with emotional and behavioral problems in children.Citation31

The Present Study

Currently, there is no systematic conclusion about the impact of having a second child on the first child. Especially after more than 30 years of the one-child policy in China, most families have only one child. Now, as birth control policies are adjusted, many families are having a second or even third child changing the family structure. The firstborn, who expected to be the only child, had to be transformed into the role of an older brother or sister. There are differences in the developmental trajectories of children across cultures. In China, research on changes and influencing factors in the emotions and behaviors of firstborn children have been carried out gradually. But still they have started relatively late and are limited. This study intends to follow up and observe the emotional and behavioral changes of preschool children during TTS through longitudinal cohort studies and to evaluate the emotional and behavioral characteristics of preschool children in different groups. We explored the factors that influence emotional and behavioral changes in children during TTS, with the goal of improving the mental health of firstborn children.

Materials and Methods

Participants

In the obstetric clinics of two hospitals in Chongqing, southwest China, we enrolled children who met the criteria from March to December 2019. The inclusion criteria were: (1) There was only one child in the family; (2) The age of the child was 1.5–5 years old; (3) The mother was pregnant with her second child and was in the second or third trimester. We excluded single-parent families, reorganized families, and firstborn children with mental health conditions such as autism and depression. Informed consent was issued to the mothers who met the standards, and the mothers filled in the questionnaire after informed consent.

A total of 97 children were investigated at baseline (mothers were in their third trimester). Two follow-up surveys were conducted at 2 and 12 months after the birth of the second child. Among them, seventy-two children (74.23%) were followed up 2 months after the birth of the second child, sixty children (61.86%) were followed up 12 months after the birth of the second child, and 35 children (36.08%) were followed up both times. Information in this study was provided by the firstborn’s mother. Mothers who met the criteria to participate in the questionnaire were contacted, and the semi-structured interview outline was used to conduct an in-depth individual interview with the mothers after their informed consent.

A total of 16 qualitative interviews were conducted between September 2019 to December 2020 among the mothers with informed consent in the cohort, involving 14 mothers of firstborn children from six districts in Chongqing. Two mothers (IDI-3 and IDI-7) were interviewed once when their second child was 2 months and 12 months old, respectively. One mother (IDI-8) had an interview when her second child was 2 months old, and the other 11 had an interview when their second child was 12 months old ().

Figure 1 Study framework.

Figure 1 Study framework.

Questionnaire Survey

Basic Information About Participants

According to the purpose of the survey, a questionnaire designed by our team was used to investigate the basic information of firstborn children. The survey contents mainly include (1) The information of firstborn children: the date of birth and gender; (2) The family information of participants: the date of birth of the mother, and the type of family (nuclear family or extended family), the annual household income, parents’ education level, parental relationship, mother-child relationship, father-child relationship, family atmosphere, parenting style, etc.

Emotional and Behavioral Problems

Firstborn children’s emotional and behavioral problems were collected using the 1.5–5-year-old Children’s Behavioral Scale Parent Version (CBCL),Citation32 which contained 99 items (67 items were summarized into 7 emotional and behavioral factors and 3 comprehensive questions, and 32 items were summarized into other questions). Each of the 99 items was scored on a scale of 0, 1, and 2, with 0 representing “not accurate”, 1 representing “near or sometimes accurate”, and 2 representing “very accurate or often accurate”. The higher the score, the more severe the emotional and behavioral problems. In this study, Cronbach’s alpha of each factor of the CBCL scale ranged from 0.602 to 0.879, and the value of Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) was 0.928. After Bartlett’s Test, P<0.001.

The seven emotional and behavioral factors are as follows: emotionally reactivity, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, withdrawn, sleep problems, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. Three comprehensive emotional and behavioral problems are internalizing problems (emotionally reactivity, anxious/depressed, somatic complaints, withdrawn), externalizing problems (attention problems, aggressive behavior), and total problems.

Qualitative Interviews

Interview Methods

Individual in-depth interviews were conducted by telephone/WeChat and recorded after obtaining the consent of the interviewees. The interviews were semi-structured, using a prepared interview outline, and each interview lasted about 20–40 minutes.

Content of Interviews

The interview outline was prepared by referring to the quantitative questionnaire results. The specific interview contents were as follows: (1) The changes in the firstborn’s attitude and attachment to parents and their emotional and behavioral changes before the birth of the second child, at 2 and 12 months of age of the second child; (2) The reasons for the emotional and behavioral changes of the firstborn children; (3) Intervention measures and their effects on the change of children’s negative emotions and behaviors, as well as mother’s suggestions.

Quality Control

Provide training manuals and conduct training for investigators before fieldwork and formal interviews to ensure they were familiar with the program’s background, methodology, and content. A 4-day pre-survey was conducted in the maternity clinic of the hospital before the formal investigation. The questionnaires were checked immediately during and after the survey. Before each interview, the interviewer explained the interview criteria to the interviewee. After each interview, investigators transcribed the interview recordings verbatim into a one-word document, which was then double-checked. Multiple investigators were familiar with the transcribed material, and at least two did the coding, tabulating, and interpreting independently.

This study conducted questionnaire surveys, combined with individual in-depth interviews with the mothers of subjects, compared, analyzed, and combined the description of the mothers on the changes in firstborn children’s emotion and behavior and the reasons for the changes with the results of quantitative questionnaire analysis, so as to make the research results more objective and scientific.Citation33,Citation34

Data Analyses

Epidata 3.1 was used to double input the collected data and check and correct errors, and IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) was used to analyze the data. Chi-square tests and t-tests were used to compare the differences in basic information, family socioeconomic status, family relationships, and parenting styles between children in the third trimester, two months after birth, and 12 months after the birth of the second child, and to analyze the influence factors of emotional and behavioral changes. A One-Way Repeated Measures ANOVA was used to analyze changes in children’s emotional and behavioral problems during TTS. The data was judged anomalous by boxplots and corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser method when the spherical assumption was violated. The emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children were grouped by the Group-Based Trajectory Modeling (GBTM) in STATA16 (StataCorp. 2019. Stata Statistical Software: Release 16. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC), and the distribution of influencing factors between different groups was compared. The significance level was set at 0.05.

Thematic framework analysis was used to formulate a thematic framework based on the qualitative interview outline and emerging issues from the pre-experimental interview transcript data. MAXQDA 2020 (VERBI GmbH, Berlin, Germany) was used to manage the data (The thematic framework can be seen in Appendix 1). The data was collated and analyzed in strict accordance with the five steps of familiarizing with the data, formulating a theme frame, encoding, tabulating, and interpreting.

Results

Quantitative Results

The mean age of the subjects was 3.00 ± 0.97 years old at baseline, with 45 boys and 51 girls (One gender information is missing, and one participant did not report annual household income). There were no differences in basic information (P>0.05) (). There were also no differences in basic information and baseline CBCL scores between retained and lost subjects at the 2-month follow-up and 12-month follow-up (P>0.05) (Tables S1 and S2, shown in Appendix 2).

Table 1 Comparison of Basic Characteristics of Firstborn Children at Baseline, 1st Follow-Up, and 2nd Follow-Up After the Birth of a Second Child

For firstborn children, except for emotionally reactive () scores, the scores of the other six emotional and behavioral factors, as well as internalizing problems, externalizing problems, and total problems changed from baseline to follow-up survey. The scores of anxious/depressed (P=0.017, P=0.012) (), aggressive behavior (P=0.004, P=0.002) (), internalizing problems (P=0.035, P=0.004) (), externalizing problems (P=0.002, P=0.001) () and total problems (P=0.002, P=0.001) () were significantly higher at 1st follow-up and 2nd follow-up than those at the baseline. The score of sleep problems (P=0.018) () increased only when the second child was followed up at 1st follow-up and the scores of somatic complaints (P=0.013) () and attention problems (P=0.007) () increased only at 2nd follow-up. The withdrawn (P=0.002, P=0.036) () scores of firstborn children at 2nd follow-up were higher than those at baseline and 1st follow-up.

Figure 2 Changes in emotional and behavioral problem scores in firstborn children at baseline, 1st follow-up, and 2nd follow-up.

Notes: *The significant level is less than 0.05. (A) Emotionally reactivity; (B) Anxious/depressed; (C) Somatic complaints; (D) Withdrawn; (E) Sleep problems; (F) Attention problems; (G) Aggressive behavior; (H) Internalizing problems; (I) Externalizing problems; (J) Total problems.
Figure 2 Changes in emotional and behavioral problem scores in firstborn children at baseline, 1st follow-up, and 2nd follow-up.

During TTS, the total problems score of firstborn children continued to increase. The change in the total problems score of the firstborn children was modeled based on the trajectory of the group. According to the model fit (), the firstborn children were divided into three groups according to the increased levels of total problems score (). The total problems score of group 1 showed a low level and a slow growth trend, the total problems score of group 2 showed a medium level and a rapid growth trend, and the total problems score of group 3 showed a high level and a rapid growth after 2 months of the second child.

Table 2 Model Fit Indicators and Average Posterior Probability with Two Through Four Classes

Figure 3 Changes in total problems scores across three groups.

Figure 3 Changes in total problems scores across three groups.

A good father-child relationship may be associated with lower levels of total problem scores, but there was no statistical significance (P=0.05) ().

Table 3 Comparison of Basic Information Between Groups with Different Changes in Scores of Emotional and Behavioral Problems

Qualitative Results

The average age of mothers who participated in the qualitative interview was 31.07±2.56 years old. Among the 14 children (7 girls and 7 boys), the average age of the children at baseline was 4.21±0.97 years old (Table S3, shown in Appendix 2). The qualitative data were summarized as four main themes through the thematic framework analysis.

Theme 1: Emotional and Behavioral Changes of Firstborn Children During TTS

According to the mothers’ description, although some children had positive changes in their emotions and behaviors during TTS, more of them showed negative changes ( and ). Some citations are shown in Box 1 and Box 2, shown in Appendix 3.

Figure 4 Emotional changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Notes:
: Positive change;
: Negative change.
Figure 4 Emotional changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Figure 5 Behavioral changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Notes:
: Positive change;
: Negative change.
Figure 5 Behavioral changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Theme 2: Reasons for Emotional or Behavioral Changes in Firstborn Children

Based on the results of the interviews, it was found that the child’s own personality and traits, as well as certain family factors, affect the emotions and behavior of the firstborn. Some mothers reported that children with positive and cheerful personalities and younger children showed fewer emotional and behavioral problems during TTS. A good relationship between parents, a harmonious home atmosphere, and good home education can reduce emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children. Some mothers saw the arrival of a second child as a challenge for their firstborn. In addition, the emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children may also be influenced by conditions in other families with a second child (). Some citations are shown in Box 3, shown in Appendix 3.

Figure 6 Factors influencing emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Figure 6 Factors influencing emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children during TTS by mother’s description.

Theme 3: Measures Taken by Parents and Their Effects

Fourteen mothers shared their experiences dealing with the negative emotions and behaviors of their firstborns during TTS. Four mothers adopted the method of meeting the needs of the child; two criticized the child; two directly ignored the child’s problems; ten adopted the method of communication and reasoning; one adopted the method of accompanying the firstborn and creating a harmonious family atmosphere. The effect of these measures is shown in . Some citations are shown in Box 4, shown in Appendix 3.

Figure 7 Treatment measures and effects of firstborn children’s bad emotional and behavioral problems during TTS by mother’s description.

Figure 7 Treatment measures and effects of firstborn children’s bad emotional and behavioral problems during TTS by mother’s description.

Theme 4: Parenting Experiences and Lessons for Firstborn Children

The mothers we interviewed also shared their suggestions on how to better deal with the emotional and behavioral problems of firstborn children during TTS, based on their experience, self-learning, and observation of second-child families around them, which were summarized as follows: (1) Be fair to both children, regardless of age; (2) Perspective-taking: thinking from the perspective of firstborn children; (3) Give more education, communication, and guidance to lead children to take the responsibility as the older brother (older sister); (4) Pay more attention to and accompany firstborn children, and comfort them as soon as possible if they have emotional and behavioral problems; (5) Show the firstborn children the vulnerability of mothers and let the them be the master on some decisions. Some citations are shown in Box 4, shown in Appendix 3.

Discussion

Changes in Firstborn Children’s Emotional and Behavioral Problems

In this study, during TTS, emotional and behavioral problems among firstborn children increased significantly with the birth of a second child. Before and after the birth of a second child, the average score of total problems of firstborn children ranged from 20.98 to 28.02, with detection rates ranging from 11.34% to 20.83%. Through quantitative surveys and qualitative interviews, we found that some firstborns did show an increase in negative emotions and behaviors, such as anxiety, jealousy, and withdrawal at 2 and 12 months after the delivery of a second child, indicating that the birth of the second child may lead to emotional and behavioral problems of firstborn children. This finding can be explained by the stress event model theory and ecosystem theory. The theory of the stress event model suggests that the birth of a second child is a stressful event for the firstborn and the family, posing an inevitable psychological threat to each family member.Citation13,Citation16 Ecosystem theory emphasizes the interaction between people and their surrounding social and cultural environment and argues that children’s emotions and behaviors are closely related to their environment. According to this theory, individual behavior is the result of interaction between the individual and the environment.Citation35 During the normal ecological transition period, with some predictable stressful events (such as the birth of a second child), they may still lead to sudden changes in the family environment and the status of children. These pressures alter the psychological structure of the firstborn child and force them to readjust their cognitive structure to adapt to the new environment.Citation11 As a result, we found that they also tend to show some increased capacity for self-care, expression, and care for others from qualitative interviews.

Temporal Patterns of Change in Emotional and Behavioral Problems During TTS

Anxiety/depression, aggressive behavior, and sleep problems in firstborns begin to increase significantly 2 months after the birth of a second child. Similarly, we found an increase in anxiety, depression, fear, and aggressive behavior in the firstborn children 2 months after the birth of a second child from maternal descriptions. Stewart found a higher rate of aggressive confrontations between firstborn children and their mothers in the two months following the birth of a second child.Citation15 Because the newborn attracted most of the mother’s attention, the mother and the firstborn were briefly separated after the birth and reminded relatively distant for the next two months.Citation20 And family members paid less attention to firstborn children, which may damage the sense of security of firstborn children and lead to anxiety and depression.Citation36 To some extent, milestones in the development of the second child can explain the temporal patterns of change in the behavior of the firstborn.Citation37 Two months after the birth of the second child, behaviors such as looking up and turning over begin to occur. Family members pay more attention to the second child and ignore the firstborn child. Therefore, firstborn children have high levels of anxiety and antagonistic behavior.

Somatic complaints, withdrawal, and attention problems of firstborn children increased significantly 12 months after the birth of a second child, and withdrawal has continued to increase after the birth of a second child. Some firstborn children become apathetic and passive, exhibit imitation behavior, and even have sleep problems. The increase in somatic complaints in the firstborn may be related to language and motor development in the second child. Helland found that internalizing problems in one sibling were associated with language problems in another sibling and suggested that there may be shared family factors.Citation38 The firstborn child begins to get along with the second child in more activities, which often leads to conflicts. The partiality of the family to the second child may cause more complaints and problem behaviors of the firstborn child.Citation39,Citation40 After 12 months of birth, the second child can move independently and walk in a small range. For various reasons, the child may be distracted by the activities of the second child, which interferes with the attention of the firstborn, and the older sibling’s craving for attention. The increase in withdrawal behavior may be due to the fact that parents need to learn to take care of two children at an early stage after the birth of the second child causing less time and quality to accompany the firstborn child.Citation39 The mother is busy caring for the second child, which may make the firstborn child more likely to feel threatened by the birth of the second child, resulting in an unsafe parent-child relationship.Citation17,Citation41 Since the withdrawal behavior of the child may be influenced by the unsafe parent-child relationship,Citation42 the withdrawal behavior of firstborn children in the 2 months after second childbirth is higher than that of firstborn children in the third trimester. Twelve months after the birth of the second child, the mother may return to work and spend less time with the firstborn child, which causes the child to develop separation anxiety and then exhibit more nervousness, rumination, and withdrawal behavior 12 months after the birth of the second child than 2 months after the birth of the second child.Citation43

In addition, through interviews, it was found that firstborn children had less dependence on their parents 12 months after the birth of a second child, and their self-reliance and self-care ability improved. After the birth of a second child, most firstborn children in China are mainly cared for by their grandparents, who tend to have more parenting experience than their parents. And studies have shown that more contact with grandparents can improve children’s social skills and cognitive development.Citation44,Citation45

Related Risk Factors

The adverse effects caused by the birth of a second child may be acceptable But the role of the family ecological environment is particularly important.Citation46 Due to physical changes during pregnancy and the care of the second child after the birth of the second child, the mother pays less attention to and accompanies the firstborn child, and the father may play a more important role during TTS.Citation47 Active father involvement in early parenting may yield additional benefits.Citation48 A better relationship between children and their fathers and a democratic paternal parenting style appeared to be associated with lower levels of emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children. Some studies have shown that the quality of a father’s companionship has a greater coefficient of correlation with psychological and behavioral adaptation in children.Citation49 Different parenting styles can directly affect a child’s emotions and behavior, and a father’s parenting style can predict a child’s future social behavior.Citation50,Citation51

Through interviews, it was found that younger firstborns had fewer emotional and behavioral problems. Based on Teti’s research and Piaget’s theory of cognitive development,Citation52,Citation53 we conjecture that children under the age of 2 have low social cognitive ability, are in the sensorimotor stage and are unable to distinguish between their own emotions and thoughts and those of others. The period between the ages of 2 and 7 is a critical time for children’s language development, during which their speech and thinking develop rapidly. However, there may be competition for resources between siblings born at short birth intervals, which is directly linked to a higher risk of death in older siblings.Citation54,Citation55 The time of the birth of younger siblings may be related to the risk of the vulnerability of older siblings in early childhood development, and parents should consider optimizing the birth spacing when welcoming a second child.Citation56

Implications and Limitations

Psychological problems in early childhood often persist into adolescence and even adulthood.Citation57,Citation58 Many children will face the transition from being an only child to being an older brother/sister under the trend of encouraging child-bearing. During this time, if a child’s emotional and behavioral problems fail to attract attention and are not well addressed, this can affect long-term health and functioning. Policies and programs that improve children’s mental health during childhood can also improve long-term health and may prevent children from developing diagnosable diseases.Citation59–61 Therefore, promoting good mental health in firstborn children and addressing mental disorders are important public health issues. This study focused on the emotional and behavioral problems of firstborn children during TTS to make up for the lack of research in this area in recent years. We found that firstborn children during TTS are at high risk for emotional and behavioral problems. Questions will be explored more fully and in depth through quantitative research combined with qualitative interviews. Several influencing factors affect the emotional and behavioral changes of firstborn children, which is expected to provide direction for interventions to address emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children during TTS.

The small sample size of the cohort in this study, less than 100 participants, and some loss of follow-up in the two follow-up surveys make it difficult to explore in depth the influencing factors of emotional and behavioral changes in firstborn children through quantitative studies. In addition, increased emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children during TTS may also occur with age. Because there was no control group of only children in the study, it was difficult to discern whether the change was caused solely by age. Some researchers have also suggested that the transition period includes a longer period after the birth of a second child than the transition period in this study. Further prospective follow-up studies with larger sample sizes, longer follow-up periods, and a control group are needed to further identify the factors and mechanisms that influence the development of emotional and behavioral problems in firstborn children during TTS.

Conclusions and Recommendations

The arrival of a second child can lead to more emotional and behavioral problems for the firstborn, but it may also increase their independence and self-care. The age and temperament of the firstborn and the father-child relationship are linked to the changes in the firstborn’s emotional and behavioral problems and should be considered fully when welcoming a second child. Based on the findings of this study, we propose the following measures to reduce children’s emotional and behavioral problems during TTS and thus help them successfully survive the sibling transition: (1) Pay attention to the emotional behavior of children as early as possible, especially within 2 months after the birth of the second child. During this period, the emotional and behavioral changes in children are more obvious, especially anxiety/depression, sleep problems, and aggressive behaviors. (2) Children aged 3–5 years have the higher social cognitive ability and sensitivity than children aged 1–2 years. Children with rebellious personalities are more likely to have emotional and behavioral problems. Therefore, more attention should be paid to children aged 3–5 years and children with bad temperaments. (3) Parents need to adjust their bad emotions and behaviors to avoid showing too much anxiety and tension to their children. At the same time, both parents need to learn scientific parenting methods, especially fathers who need to become more aware of being involved in the upbringing of their children and try to improve the quality of companionship on the premise of ensuring sufficient time with firstborn children. (4) Families jointly create a good parent-child relationship and harmonious atmosphere, which plays a crucial role in avoiding and improving the emotional and behavioral problems of firstborn children.

Abbreviation

TTS, Transition to Siblinghood.

Data Sharing Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Q. Liu, [email protected].

Ethical Approval

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Chongqing Medical University.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study. And all participants agreed to publish anonymously.

Disclosure

The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest to disclose for this work.

Acknowledgments

We thank all participants in this study.

Additional information

Funding

This research was supported by the Science and Technology Research Program of Chongqing Municipal Education Commission (Grant No. KJQN201800425).

References

  • Schaan L, Schulz A, Nuraydin S, et al. Interoceptive accuracy, emotion recognition, and emotion regulation in preschool children. Int J Psychophysiol. 2019;138:47–56. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.02.001
  • Gil Madrona P, Romero Martínez SJ, Sáez-Gallego NM, Ordóñez Camacho XG. Psychomotor limitations of overweight and obese five-year-old children: influence of body mass indices on motor, perceptual, and social-emotional skills. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(3):427. doi:10.3390/ijerph16030427
  • Rao Z, Wu J, Zhang F, Tian Z. Psychological and emotional recognition of preschool children using artificial neural Network. Front Psychol. 2022;12:762396. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.762396
  • Russell JD, Herringa RJ. Editorial: the preschool emotional brain. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60(1):29–31. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2020.08.012
  • Simon P, Nader-Grosbois N. Preschoolers’ empathy profiles and their social adjustment. Front Psychol. 2021;12:782500. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.782500
  • Milevsky A. Sibling Relationships in Childhood and Adolescence: Predictors and Outcomes. Columbia University Press; 2011.
  • Song JH, Volling B. Theory-of-mind development and early sibling relationships after the birth of a sibling: parental discipline matters. Infant Child Dev. 2018;27(1):e2053. doi:10.1002/icd.2053
  • Ward KP. The transition to siblinghood: an understudied yet influential process pertinent to social work research. Soc Work. 2022;67(2):175–183. doi:10.1093/sw/swac009
  • Leung AK, Robson WL. Sibling rivalry. Clin Pediatr. 1991;30(5):314–317. doi:10.1177/000992289103000510
  • Zhang W, Li Y, Li L, Hinshaw S, Lin X. Vicious cycle of emotion regulation and ODD symptoms among Chinese school-age children with ODD: a random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2023;17(1):47. doi:10.1186/s13034-023-00579-x
  • Volling BL. Family transitions following the birth of a sibling: an empirical review of changes in the firstborn’s adjustment [published correction appears in Psychol Bull. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(3):497–528. doi:10.1037/a0026921
  • Kramer L, Ramsburg D. Advice given to parents on welcoming a second child: a critical review. Fam Relat. 2002;51(1):2–14. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2002.00002.x
  • Stewart RB Jr. The Second Child: Family Transition and Adjustment. Sage Publications, Inc; 1990.
  • Gullicks JN, Crase SJ. Sibling behavior with a newborn: parents’ expectations and observations. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 1993;22(5):438–444. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6909.1993.tb01827.x
  • Stewart RB, Mobley LA, Van Tuyl SS, Salvador MA. The firstborn’s adjustment to the birth of a sibling: a longitudinal assessment. Child Dev. 1987;58(2):341–355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb01382.x
  • Dunn J, Kendrick C, MacNamee R. The reaction of first-born children to the birth of a sibling: mothers’ reports. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1981;22(1):1–18. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1981.tb00527.x
  • Field T, Reite M. Children’s responses to separation from mother during the birth of another child. Child Dev. 1984;55(4):1308–1316. doi:10.2307/1130000
  • Touris M, Kromelow S, Harding C. Mother-firstborn attachment and the birth of a sibling. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1995;65(2):293–297. doi:10.1037/h0079614
  • Harris MJ, John K, Sharp R. The effects of a mother’s second pregnancy on the firstborn child. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol. 1989;29(3 Pt 2):319–321. doi:10.1111/j.1479-828x.1989.tb01753.x
  • Behringer V, Berghänel A, Deschner T, Lee SM, Fruth B, Hohmann G. Transition to siblinghood causes a substantial and long-lasting increase in urinary cortisol levels in wild bonobos. Elife. 2022;11:e77227. doi:10.7554/eLife.77227
  • Volling BL, Gonzalez R, Oh W, et al. Developmental trajectories of children’s adjustment across the transition to siblinghood: pre-birth predictors and sibling outcomes at one year. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2017;82(3):1–215.
  • Howe N. Predicting normative and problematic family pathways to the transition to siblinghood: commentary on volling Et Al.’S monograph. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2017;82(3):184–195. doi:10.1111/mono.12319
  • Volling BL. The transition to siblinghood: a developmental ecological systems perspective and directions for future research. J Fam Psychol. 2005;19(4):542–549. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.542
  • Rong Y, Han Y, Dong L, Bi H. Differences in power acquisition between only and non-only children: the effects of cooperative orientation, competitive orientation, and dependency on parents. Front Psychol. 2022;13:778726. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.778726
  • Kramer L. The essential ingredients of successful sibling relationships: an emerging framework for advancing theory and practice. Child Dev Perspect. 2010;4(2):80–86. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00122.x
  • Trause MA, Voos D, Rudd C, Klaus M, Kennell J, Boslett M. Separation for childbirth: the effect on the sibling. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1981;12(1):32–39. doi:10.1007/BF00706671
  • Chen -B-B, Wang Y, Liang J, Tong L. And baby makes four: biological and psychological changes and influential factors of firstborn’s adjustment to transition to siblinghood. Adv Psychol Sci. 2016;24(6):863. doi:10.3724/sp.j.1042.2016.00863
  • Flouri E, Midouhas E, Ruddy A. Socio-economic status and family structure differences in early trajectories of child adjustment: individual and neighborhood effects. Health Place. 2016;37:8–15. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.11.005
  • Lee SJ, Pace GT, Lee JY, Knauer H. The association of fathers’ parental warmth and parenting stress to child behavior problems. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2018;91:1–10. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.05.020
  • Plamondon A, Browne DT, Madigan S, Jenkins JM. Disentangling child-specific and family-wide processes underlying negative mother-child transactions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46(3):437–447. doi:10.1007/s10802-017-0309-2
  • McDonald SW, Kehler HL, Tough SC. Risk factors for delayed social-emotional development and behavior problems at age two: results from the All Our Babies/Families (AOB/F) cohort. Health Sci Rep. 2018;1(10):e82. doi:10.1002/hsr2.82
  • Achenbach TM, Leslie AR. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. Vol. 30. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research center for children, youth, & families; 2000.
  • Ohneberg C, Warmbein A, Stöbich N, et al. Study protocol for the implementation and evaluation of a digital-robotic-based intervention for nurses and patients in a hospital: a quantitative and qualitative triangulation based on the Medical Research Council (MRC) framework for developing and evaluating complex interventions. BMC Nurs. 2022;21(1):349. doi:10.1186/s12912-022-01088-6
  • Sáez Sánchez MB, Gil Madrona P, Martínez López M. Development psychomotor and its relationship with the motivation towards the learning and academic performance in Education Childish. Educ Mag. 2021;392:177–203. doi:10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2021-392-483
  • Bronfenbrenner U. Interacting systems in human development: research paradigms: present and future; 1988.
  • Qian G, Yang S, Li R, Dou G. The mediating effect of parenting style on the relationship between first-born children’s temperament and psychological adaptation. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):13594. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-17897-3
  • Karppanen AK, Hurtig T, Miettunen J, Niemelä M, Tammelin T, Korpelainen R. Infant motor development and physical activity and sedentary time at midlife. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2021;31(7):1450–1460. doi:10.1111/sms.13954
  • Helland SS, Røysamb E, Brandlistuen RE, Melby-Lervåg M, Gustavson K. A common family factor underlying language difficulties and internalizing problems: findings from a population-based sibling study. J Learn Disabil. 2020;53(5):399–409. doi:10.1177/0022219420911634
  • Sawicki JA. Sibling rivalry and the new baby: anticipatory guidance and management strategies. Pediatr Nurs. 1997;23(3):298–302.
  • Ng CS, Chiu MM, Zhou Q, Heyman G. The impact of differential parenting: study protocol on a longitudinal study investigating child and parent factors on children’s psychosocial health in Hong Kong. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1656. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01656
  • Gottlieb LN, Baillies J. Firstborns’ behaviors during a mother’s second pregnancy. Nurs Res. 1995;44(6):356–362. doi:10.1097/00006199-199511000-00007
  • Rubin KH, Burgess KB, Hastings PD. Stability and social-behavioral consequences of toddlers’ inhibited temperament and parenting behaviors. Child Dev. 2002;73(2):483–495. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00419
  • Feriante J, Torrico TJ, Bernstein B. Separation anxiety disorder. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2023.
  • Goh ECL. Grandparents as childcare providers: an in-depth analysis of the case of Xiamen, China. J Aging Stud. 2009;23(1):60–68. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2007.08.001
  • Nyland B, Zeng X, Nyland C, Tran L. grandparents as educators and carers in China. J Early Child Res. 2009;7(1):46–57. doi:10.1177/1476718x08098353
  • Richmond MK, Stocker CM, Rienks SL. Longitudinal associations between sibling relationship quality, parental differential treatment, and children’s adjustment. J Fam Psychol. 2005;19(4):550–559. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.550
  • Kuo PX, Volling BL, Gonzalez R. Gender role beliefs, work-family conflict, and father involvement after the birth of a second child. Psychol Men Masc. 2018;19(2):243–256. doi:10.1037/men0000101
  • Donithen R, Schoppe-Sullivan S. Correlates and predictors of parenting self-efficacy in new fathers. J Fam Psychol. 2022;36(3):396–405. doi:10.1037/fam0000910
  • Deng LY, Wang XT, Xiong YY, Li YT, Li BL. The relationship among paternal accompany, maternal emotion and psychosocial adjustment of primary school firstborn children in two-children families. J Clin Psychol. 2020;28(02):254–260. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.02.007
  • Jacobvitz D, Aviles AI, Aquino GA, Tian Z, Zhang S, Hazen N. Fathers’ sensitivity in infancy and externalizing problems in middle childhood: the role of coparenting. Front Psychol. 2022;13:805188. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.805188
  • Russell A, Hart CH, Robinson CC, Olsen SF. Children’s sociable and aggressive behavior with peers: a comparison of the US and Australia, and contributions of temperament and parenting styles. Int J Behav Dev. 2003;27(1):74–86. doi:10.1080/01650250244000038
  • Orr J. Piaget’s theory of cognitive development may be useful in deciding what to teach and how to teach it. Nurse Educ Today. 1991;11(1):65–69. doi:10.1016/0260-6917(91)90127-v
  • Teti DM, Sakin JW, Kucera E, Corns KM, Eiden RD. And baby makes four: predictors of attachment security among preschool-age firstborns during the transition to siblinghood. Child Dev. 1996;67(2):579–596. doi:10.2307/1131833
  • Kippen R, Walters S. Is sibling rivalry fatal?: siblings and mortality clustering. J Interdiscip Hist. 2012;42(4):571–591. doi:10.1162/jinh_a_00305
  • Whitworth A, Stephenson R. Birth spacing, sibling rivalry and child mortality in India. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(12):2107–2119. doi:10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00002-3
  • Dhamrait G, O’Donnell M, Christian H, Pereira G. Is early childhood development impeded by the birth timing of the younger sibling? PLoS One. 2022;17(5):e0268325. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0268325
  • McKay MT, Cannon M, Chambers D, et al. Childhood trauma and adult mental disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2021;143(3):189–205. doi:10.1111/acps.13268
  • Merrick MT, Ports KA, Ford DC, Afifi TO, Gershoff ET, Grogan-Kaylor A. Unpacking the impact of adverse childhood experiences on adult mental health. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;69:10–19. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.03.016
  • Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693–2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108
  • Pandey A, Hale D, Das S, Goddings AL, Blakemore SJ, Viner RM. Effectiveness of universal self-regulation-based interventions in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(6):566–575. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.0232
  • Taylor RD, Oberle E, Durlak JA, Weissberg RP. Promoting positive youth development through school-based social and emotional learning interventions: a meta-analysis of follow-up effects. Child Dev. 2017;88(4):1156–1171. doi:10.1111/cdev.12864