671
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
ORIGINAL RESEARCH

Parental Attachment and Proactive-Reactive Aggression in Adolescence: The Mediating Role of Self-Control and Perspective Taking

, , , &
Pages 3437-3446 | Received 04 Jul 2023, Accepted 17 Aug 2023, Published online: 28 Aug 2023

Abstract

Introduction

Identifying risk and protective factors of aggressive behavior during adolescence is beneficial for the intervention and prevention treatments. Although studies show that attachment quality is closely related to aggression, the underlying psychological mechanisms remain unclear. This study aimed to investigate the links of parental attachment with proactive and reactive aggression and to examine the mediating role of perspective taking and self-control among Chinese adolescents.

Methods

A cluster sampling method was used to recruit participants from several high schools located in the central regions of China. A total of 2982 participants (Mage = 17.28, SD = 0.83, range 15~20 years; 1602 girls, 1380 boys) were included in the final analysis.

Results

Correlation analysis showed that participants possessing higher attachment security with parents were more likely to report lower levels of proactive and reactive aggression. And, self-control and perspective taking were positively associated with parental attachment, and negatively associated with both types of aggression. Moreover, structural equation models indicated that parental attachment directly, and indirectly predicted proactive and reactive aggression through self-control and perspective taking.

Discussion

Overall, this study contributes to a more comprehensive understanding of the explanatory mechanisms that link adolescent-parent attachment and aggression, and suggest that high quality of adolescent-parent interactions may promote adolescents’ self-control and perspectives taking, which further reduces their aggression propensity.

Introduction

Over the past decades, the study of aggressive behaviors has emerged as a prominent area of investigation within developmental psychology and psychopathology. Early-life persistent patterns of aggressive behavior are considered as potential precursors to subsequent issues such as substance abuse, violent crime, and health problems.Citation1,Citation2 In an attempt to understand the underlying mechanisms of aggression, researchers have classified aggression into two main types: proactive and reactive aggression. This dichotomous perspective holds the promise of illuminating distinct etiological pathways of aggression.Citation3,Citation4 Proactive aggression is described as instrumental and organized behavior aimed at gaining a reward or social dominance over others, whereas reactive aggression has been characterized as a response to provocation or a perceived threat.Citation5–7 Individuals exhibiting reactive aggression are more likely to react impulsively in the face of provocation, while those displaying proactive aggression tend to exhibit lower emotional reactivity and higher levels of instrumentality in their pursuit of benefits. Longitudinal research has provided evidence suggesting that proactive and reactive aggression are unique constructs with separate developmental trajectories and distinct associated factors.Citation8 Identifying the risk and protective factors of both types of aggressive behavior during adolescence is crucial in recognizing the emergence and cessation of aggression, thereby informs effective intervention and prevention strategies. Recently, abundant evidence has emphasized the significance of parental attachment in shaping aggressive behaviors. The present study aimed to investigate the associations between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression, while also examining the potential mediating roles of self-control and perspective taking in these relationships.

The Link Between Parental Attachment and Aggression

Parental attachment refers to the establishment of secure and nurturing bonds between children and their parents, which provides a foundation for the development of cognitive, social, and emotional competence from childhood throughout adulthood.Citation9–11 According to Bowlby’s attachment theory,Citation12 early experiences of relationships with parents shape the internal working models (ie, cognitive representations) of self and other, which are believed to influence and guide distinctive patterns of cognition, affect regulation, and behavior not only within family interactions but also in subsequent relationships with peers and close others.Citation13–15 Specifically, individuals who experience caring, responsive, and trustworthy relationships with parents are presumed to develop secure working models, and therefore adopting a similarly trusting, open, and collaborative approach toward peers and partners later in life.Citation16,Citation17 On the other hand, individuals who experience neglectful, rejecting, and intrusive relationships with parents are prone to develop insecure working models, potentially leading to problematic interpersonal functioning.

Empirical evidence suggests that adolescents higher in attachment insecurity are more likely to engage in psychological aggression, physical and verbal aggression, hostility, and proactive and reactive aggression.Citation10,Citation18–22 Conversely, secure attachment may play an important role in mitigating deleterious effects and promoting adjustment in adolescence. For instance, parental attachment quality was negatively associated with proactive and reactive aggression, and mitigated the adverse effect of emotion regulation difficulty on proactive and reactive aggression.Citation21 Hare et al found that attachment security moderated the association between paternal aggression and adolescent aggression, suggesting that attachment security may help attenuate the transmission of harmful conflict strategies across generations.Citation20,Citation23 Research also indicate that high-quality relationships with parents play a significant role in promoting adolescents’ psychosocial adjustment and life satisfaction.Citation24,Citation25 In summary, the quality of parental attachment was assumed to be negatively associated with proactive and reactive aggression.

The Mediating Role of Self-Control

Conceptually, self-control is defined as the ability to inhibit and alter dominant impulses or responses to support the pursuit of long-term and valued goals.Citation26,Citation27 According to the self-control theory, effective parenting, including monitoring, recognition, and discipline, facilitates the development of self-control, whereas negative parenting impedes self-control development, leading to an increased risk of externalizing and deviant behaviors.Citation28 Empirical and review studies demonstrate that effective parenting practices could promote the development of emotion and behavior regulation abilities among children and adolescents.Citation29–33 Specifically, warm and caring parent-adolescent relationships are positively associated with self-regulation behavior across early to late adolescence,Citation34–36 whereas insensitive and harsh parenting is related to lower inhibitory self-control and executive functioning.Citation31,Citation37,Citation38

Both empirical and theoretical research consistently indicate that self-control serves as a vital protective factor in preventing and restraining the formation and progression of aggressive behaviors.Citation39,Citation40 Adolescents with high self-control tend to exhibit more positive coping and prosocial behaviors,Citation41,Citation42 and have lower tendency to ruminate about angry events, which further contribute to lower levels of aggression.Citation43 Furthermore, self-control may be an important mediating mechanism that explains the effect of parental attachment on adolescents’ maladaptive responses and aggression. Specifically, secure parental attachment is linked to higher self-control, which in turn is associated with more prosocial behaviors and fewer rule-breaking behaviors.Citation42,Citation44 On the other hand, lower quality of adolescent-parent interactions coincides with dysfunctional self-regulation in adolescent, which in turn was associated with higher maladaptive peer relationships.Citation45,Citation46 Moreover, a cross-cultural study demonstrates that paternal attachment influenced adolescents’ adjustment difficulties directly, and indirectly through self-control.Citation47 Therefore, it is hypothesized that parental attachment quality would be positively associated with higher self-control, and negatively associated with aggression.

The Mediating Role of Perspective Taking

Perspective taking (ie, cognitive empathy), refers to the ability to recognize and understand other people’ s emotions, viewpoints, and situations, and plays an important role in social adaptation and interpersonal relationship.Citation48–51 Research has indicated that relationships with parents play a socializing role in the development of perspective taking abilities among adolescents.Citation52 More specifically, individuals who have a secure attachment with parents tend to exhibit higher levels of perspective-taking and empathic concern than their counterparts.Citation53,Citation54 Conversely, insecure-anxious individuals tend to focus on their own unregulated emotions when exposed to others in distress.Citation55 Taubner et al suggested that adolescents’ perspective-taking abilities play a moderating role in the relationship between psychopathic traits and proactive aggression,Citation56 suggesting that perspective-taking may serve as a protective factor in preventing proactive aggression in adolescence.

Perspective taking is acknowledged for its significance in comprehending the needs of others in distress and is considered a protective factor against aggression.Citation57,Citation58 Research shows that perspective taking facilitates prosocial actions and mitigates aggressive behaviors.Citation59 Adolescents with lower perspective taking are at a higher risk of engaging in various forms of bullying and aggressive behaviors, such as name calling, indirect bullying, and violent behaviors.Citation60 Indeed, children and adolescents who are more aggressive and less prosocial tend to have deficits in perspective taking and emotional empathy.Citation61 Previous work investigating the connection between parental attachment, perspective-taking, and aggression has demonstrated that adolescents’ attachment security with parents was positively associated with perspective taking, while both attachment security and perspective-taking were negatively related to aggressive tendencies.Citation62 Moreover, higher levels of attachment avoidance predicted lower perspective taking, which in turn, predicted reduced altruistic behavior.Citation63 However, controversy remains in the literature regarding the relationship between perspective taking and aggression. For instance, some studies revealed a lack of association between aggression and perspective taking in adolescents.Citation60,Citation64 Batanova and Loukas found that perspective taking predicted increased relational aggression one year later.Citation65 Researchers suggested that individuals may utilize perspective-taking skills for aggressive behaviors in order to maintain or enhance their social status and dominance.Citation64,Citation66 Considering these mixed findings, the relationship between perspective taking and aggression should be further clarified.

In reviewing the existing literature, it is evident that few studies have delved into the relationship between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression. Furthermore, no research has directly investigated the role of self-control and perspective taking underlying these associations. Thus, the present study aimed to examine the associations between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression, while simultaneously exploring the mediating role of self-control and perspective taking. Building upon the theoretical and empirical evidence, the present study proposed that self-control and perspective taking could play a mediating role in the association between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression in adolescence.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

This cross-sectional study employed a cluster sampling method to recruit 3200 participants from several high schools in Henan and Hubei Province located in the central regions of China. The researcher first introduced the purpose of the study and related questionnaires to the school manager before collectively administering the questionnaires to student on a class basis. Under the guidance of trained teachers, students were presented with an introduction to the purpose of the study and the confidentiality of their answers. Subsequently, guidelines were read out to the students, explaining the relevant instructions on how to complete the questionnaire and what aspects to consider during the process. The entire survey process lasted approximately 20 minutes, and the questionnaires were collected from the participants upon completion. Demographic characteristics were also collected: age, gender, family economic incomes (1 = 1000¥ and below; 2 = 1001~3000¥; 3 = 3001~5000¥; 4 = 5000~10,000¥; 5 = 10,001~20,000¥; 6 = 20,000¥ and above), residence (ie, rural or urban area), and parent education (1 = primary school and below; 2 = middle school; 3 = high school degree or special school degree; 4 = undergraduate degree; 5 = graduate degree or above). A total of 2982 participants (Mage = 17.28, SD = 0.83, range 15~20 years; 1602 girls, 1380 boys) were included in the final analysis. The effective collection rate of questionnaires was 93.19%. All adolescent participants and their parents signed an informed consent document and were offered an honorarium. Ethical approval of this study was granted by the Ethics Committee of Central China Normal University, and all procedures were in accordance with the Ethics of Declaration of Helsinki.

Measurements

Parental attachment was measured using the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (36-item),Citation67 which includes the cognitive-affective dimensions of trust in parent and peer, and accessibility and responsiveness. Two subscales (mother and father attachment, each containing 12 items) were used to assess three key aspects of attachment: trust (eg, “My mother/father accepts me as I am”), communication (eg, “My mother/father helps me to understand myself better”), and alienation (eg, “I don’t get much attention from my father/mother”, reversed). Participants were asked to rate their agreement on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Previous research has supported the reliability and validity of the Chinese version of this inventory.Citation68,Citation69 In the present study, Cronbach’ s alpha for mother attachment and father attachment was 0.85 and 0.86, respectively.

Proactive aggression and reactive aggression were assessed using a well-validated proactive-reactive aggression questionnaire,Citation3 which consists of 23 items. Specifically, 11 items assessed reactive aggression (eg, “Reacted angrily when provoked by others”), and 12 items assessed proactive aggression (eg, “Hurt others to win a game”). All items were rated on a 3-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, and 2 = often), with higher scores indicating a higher level of aggression. Research has indicated that this questionnaire has good reliability and validity in Chinese children and adolescents.Citation70,Citation71 In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85 for proactive aggression, 0.81 for reactive aggression, and 0.84 for total aggression.

Self-control measured with the Trait Self-Control Scale.Citation72,Citation73 Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agree with on 13 items (eg “I am good at resisting temptation”) with a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all like me, 5 = very much like me). Items were averaged so that higher scores indicated greater self-control ability. Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.82 in the present study.

Perspective taking was measured using five items from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index.Citation50 Respondents indicated the extent to which they agree with each item (eg, “I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective”) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all like me, 5 = very much like me), with higher scores indicating greater perspective taking. Items were averaged and higher scores reflect greater perspective taking. Previous research has supported the reliability and validity of this inventory in the context of Chinese culture.Citation62,Citation63 Cronbach’s alpha of this scale in this study was 0.81.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics and correlations analysis were conducted with SPSS 24.0. To assess the meditating effect of self-control and perspective taking underlying the associations between parental attachment and aggression outcomes, structural equation models were conducted using M-plus 7.0. Specifically, father attachment and mother attachment were treated as independent variables (X), self-control and perspectives taking as the mediator variables (M), and proactive aggression and reactive aggression as the separate independent variables (Y), with sex, age, income, residence, and parent education as covariates. The chi-square, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), root–mean–square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root–mean–square residual (SRMR) were used to evaluate the measurement model fit. The following criteria were used to indicate the goodness of fit: CFI ≥ 0.90, RMSEA ≤ 0.10, and SRMR ≤ 0.10.Citation74

Results

Common Methods Bias Analysis

To check and test common method bias, Harman’ s single-factor test using confirmatory factor analysis was conducted. A factor analysis was conducted on all items of the scales including parental attachment, self-control, perspectives taking, proactive aggression and reactive aggression, and a common factor from these items was extracted. The interpretation rate of the first factor was 16.77%, less than 40%, indicating that there was no common method bias in this study.

Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Analysis

presents the means, standard deviations, and t-test results on sex differences. Girls reported higher level of attachment to parents and perspective taking than boys. Boys exhibited higher levels of both proactive and reactive aggression compared to girls.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics of Sex Difference

Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix for model variables were shown in . In terms of aggressive tendencies, adolescents exhibited low to moderate levels of reactive aggression and low levels of proactive aggression. As mentioned above, proactive and reactive aggression were more prevalent in the boys. Correlation analysis showed that income and parental education were significantly and positively associated with parental attachment. Moreover, parental attachment was positively correlated with self-control and perspectives taking, and negatively correlated with proactive aggression and reactive aggression. Moreover, higher levels of self-control and perspectives taking were associated with lower proactive and reactive aggression. This suggests that participants with secure attachment to mother and father are associated with higher levels of self-control and perspectives taking, while also displaying lower levels of proactive and reactive aggressive behavior.

Table 2 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Variables (N = 2982)

Mediation Analysis

As portrayed in , the structural equation models were fitted was to examine the mediating effect of self-control with father/mother attachment as predictors and proactive and reactive aggression as outcomes. The model fit the data adequately, χ2 (9) = 70.77, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.96; TLI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.05, 90% [CI: 0.04, 0.06]; SRMR = 0.03. As predicted, mother attachment predicted proactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.11, 95% CI [−0.157, −0.071]) and reactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.07, 95% CI [−0.112, −0.016]) directly, and indirectly through self-control (indirect effect coefficient = −0.002, 95% CI [−0.004, −0.001] for proactive aggression; indirect effect coefficient = −0.029, 95% CI [−0.039, −0.019] for reactive aggression). Similarly, father attachment predicted proactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.07, 95% CI [−0.113, −0.029]) and reactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.08, 95% CI [−0.129, −0.036]) directly, and through self-control indirectly (indirect effect coefficient == −0.003, 95% CI [−0.006, −0.001] for proactive aggression; indirect effect coefficient = −0.042, 95% CI [−0.052, −0.034] for reactive aggression). Finally, we conducted a chi-square difference test to examine the potential differences between mother and father in their contributions to aggression outcomes. Results revealed no significant differences in the mediating effect of self-control between father attachment and mother attachment (proactive aggression: χ2 (1) = 2.39, p > 0.05; reactive aggression: χ2 (1) = 3.29, p > 0.05).

Figure 1 The model with standardized estimates is presented. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Figure 1 The model with standardized estimates is presented. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

A second structural equation model was established to examine the mediating effect of perspective taking with father/mother attachment as predictors and proactive and reactive aggression as outcomes (). The model fit the data adequately, χ2 (9) = 63.72, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.95; TLI = 0.90; RMSEA = 0.04, 90% [CI: 0.04, 0.06]; SRMR = 0.03. As hypothesized, mother attachment predicted proactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.11, 95% CI [−0.153, −0.068]) and reactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.09, 95% CI [−0.139, −0.043]) directly, and indirectly through perspective taking (indirect effect coefficient = −0.004, 95% CI [−0.006, −0.002] for proactive aggression; indirect effect coefficient = −0.016, 95% CI [−0.022, −0.010] for reactive aggression). Meanwhile, father attachment predicted proactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.08, 95% CI [−0.116, −0.036]) and reactive aggression (direct effect coefficient = −0.15, 95% CI [−0.196, −0.105]) directly, and indirectly through perspective taking (indirect effect coefficient = −0.002, 95% CI [−0.004, −0.001] for proactive aggression; indirect effect coefficient = −0.009, 95% CI [−0.015, −0.003] for reactive aggression). Results of chi-square difference test showed again revealed no significant differences in the mediating effect of perspective taking between father attachment and mother attachment (proactive aggression: χ2 (1) = 2.34, p > 0.05; reactive aggression: χ2 (1) = 2.55, p > 0.05).

Figure 2 Second model with standardized estimates is presented. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Figure 2 Second model with standardized estimates is presented. **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

The present study investigated the associations between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression in Chinese adolescents, and further uncovered the mediating role of self-control and perspective taking. Results showed that higher attachment security in adolescents was associated with lower proactive and reactive aggression. Additionally, both self-control and perspective taking were positively associated with parental attachment and negatively associated with both types of aggression. Furthermore, parental attachment not only directly predicted proactive and reactive aggression, but also exerted an indirect effect through self-control and perspective taking. Overall, the present findings the current study enhances our understanding of the complex relationships between parental attachment, self-control, perspective taking, and different types of aggression in adolescents. The results suggest that secure attachment with parents may serve as a protective factor against various forms of aggression by fostering better self-control and perspective-taking abilities in adolescents.

Theoretically, the present findings indicate that attachment security could inhibit aggressive tendencies, and are in accordance with attachment theory, suggesting that individuals who have positive relationships with parents tend to develop secure working models and exhibit healthy interpersonal interactions with peers and partners.Citation14,Citation16 Converging evidence has indicated that that securely attached adolescents experience less conflict with family and peers and demonstrate a smoother transition to high school than insecurely attached adolescents.Citation75–77 Similarly, studies have revealed that adolescent who had a secure attachment were lower in aggressive behaviors compared to those who had an insecure attachment.Citation18–20,Citation22 These findings suggest that attachment security may act as a protective factor for adolescents’ development of social adaptation and interpersonal relationships.

The present study adds to the existing literature on the relationship between attachment security and aggression by further highlighting the mediating role of self-control and perspective taking which elucidates the connection between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression. The mediating effect of self-control is in accordance with the self-control theory, which suggests that effective parenting fosters adolescents’ self-control ability,Citation28–30,Citation32,Citation33 which then contribute to lower aggressive tendency.Citation39,Citation40 Indeed, empirical studies have shown that caring and warm family relationships have positive effects on self-regulation behaviors among adolescents,Citation34–36 which in turn are associated with fewer rule-breaking behaviors.Citation44

On the other hand, the mediating role of perspective taking is in line with the attachment theory, which proposes that the quality of the parent-child relationship shapes an individual’s social development.Citation14,Citation16 Individuals who have a secure attachment with parents are more likely to exhibit greater perspective-taking and empathic concern than their counterparts.Citation53,Citation54 Although findings on the association between perspective taking and aggression were mixed,Citation64,Citation65 accumulating evidence suggests that understanding the viewpoint of others facilitates prosocial actions and mitigate aggressive behaviors.Citation59,Citation62,Citation78–80 Conversely, children and adolescents who lack perspective taking skills are more likely to display callous-unemotional traits and aggressive behaviors.Citation61,Citation63,Citation81 Based on existing evidence, self-control and perspective taking may be important psychological mechanisms underlying the associations between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression. Specifically, attachment security can promote adolescents’ self-regulation and perspective taking, thereby playing a crucial role in reducing aggression.

Several limitations in this study should be acknowledged. First, the recruitment of participants solely from Chinese high schools, specifically from two provinces in the central regions, may limit the generalizability of the present findings. Future research should assess this finding in diverse samples encompassing various age groups and countries with different cultural backgrounds. For instance, one study in Polish adolescents found that attachment to father, but not attachment to mother, predicted aggressive victimization through self-control.Citation82 Second, the utilization of a cross-sectional design and self-report measurements in this study present challenges to establish the causal associations among variables. Multiple forms of measurement and designs with ecological validity and predictive effect, such as third-party reports (eg, parents, teachers), experience sampling (eg, daily diary), and longitudinal tracking should be considered in future research.

Conclusions

The present study investigated the relationship between parental attachment and proactive and reactive aggression and revealed the mediating role of self-control and perspective taking in Chinese adolescents. Attachment security not only directly predicted proactive and reactive aggression, but also indirectly predicted both types of aggression through self-control and perspective taking. This study contributes to a deeper understanding of the links between parental attachment and aggression by revealing its psychological mechanisms and suggesting that secure attachment with parents may decrease adolescent’s aggression through the promotion of self-control and perspective taking. The current findings hold important implications for future research on alleviating aggression through intervention programs focused on fostering family education and relationships, as well as promoting self-control and perspective taking among adolescents.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior (Central China Normal University), Ministry of Education (CCNUCYPSYLAB2022B12, CCNUCYPSYLAB2022B08), and National Natural Science Foundation of Hubei Province of China (2023AFB276).

References

  • Fite PJ, Raine A, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Loeber R, Pardini DA. Reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent males: examining differential outcomes 10 years later in early adulthood. Crim Justice Behav. 2010;37(2):141–157. doi:10.1177/0093854809353051
  • Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, et al. Female and male antisocial trajectories: from childhood origins to adult outcomes. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20(2):673–716. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000333
  • Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, et al. The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress Behav. 2006;32(2):159–171. doi:10.1002/ab.20115
  • Voulgaridou I, Kokkinos CM. Relational aggression in adolescents: a review of theoretical and empirical research. Aggress Violent Behav. 2015;23:87–97. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2015.05.006
  • Marsee MA, Frick PJ. Exploring the cognitive and emotional correlates to proactive and reactive aggression in a sample of detained girls. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35(6):969–981. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9147-y
  • Romero-Martínez Á, Sarrate-Costa C, Moya-Albiol L. Reactive vs proactive aggression: a differential psychobiological profile? Conclusions derived from a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;136:104626. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104626
  • Smithmyer CM, Hubbard JA, Simons RF. Proactive and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: relations to aggression outcome expectancies. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000;29(1):86–93. doi:10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_9
  • Vaughan EP, Speck JS, Frick PJ, et al. Proactive and reactive aggression: developmental trajectories and longitudinal associations with callous–unemotional traits, impulsivity, and internalizing emotions. Dev Psychopathol. 2023:1–9. doi:10.1017/S0954579423000317
  • Allen JP, McElhaney KB, Land DJ, et al. A secure base in adolescence: markers of attachment security in the mother–adolescent relationship. Child Dev. 2003;74(1):292–307. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00536
  • Buist KL, Deković M, Meeus W, van Aken MAG. The reciprocal relationship between early adolescent attachment and internalizing and externalizing problem behaviour. J Adolesc. 2004;27(3):251–266. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.012
  • Laursen B, Collins WA. Parent-child relationships during adolescence. In: Handbook of Adolescent Psychology: Contextual Influences on Adolescent Development. 3rd ed. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2009:3–42.
  • Bowlby J. Developmental psychiatry comes of age. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:1–10. doi:10.1176/ajp.145.1.1
  • Ammaniti M, Van Ijzendoorn MH, Speranza AM, Tambelli R. Internal working models of attachment during late childhood and early adolescence: an exploration of stability and change. Attach Hum Dev. 2000;2(3):328–346. doi:10.1080/14616730010001587
  • Lopez FG, Brennan KA. Dynamic processes underlying adult attachment organization: toward an attachment theoretical perspective on the healthy and effective self. J Couns Psychol. 2000;47(3):283–300. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.47.3.283
  • Collins NL. Working models of attachment: implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71(4):810–832. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.810
  • Michiels D, Grietens H, Onghena P, Kuppens S. Parent–child interactions and relational aggression in peer relationships. Dev Rev. 2008;28(4):522–540. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2008.08.002
  • Buist KL, Deković M, Meeus W, van Aken MAG. Developmental patterns in adolescent attachment to mother, father and sibling. J Youth Adolesc. 2002;31(3):167–176. doi:10.1023/A:1015074701280
  • Bonab BG, Koohsar AAH. Relation among quality of attachment, hostility and interpersonal sensitivity in college students. Procedia. 2011;30:192–196. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.038
  • Grych JH, Kinsfogel KM. Exploring the role of attachment style in the relation between family aggression and abuse in adolescent dating relationships. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. 2010;19(6):624–640. doi:10.1080/10926771.2010.502068
  • Hare AL, Miga EM, Allen JP. Intergenerational transmission of aggression in romantic relationships: the moderating role of attachment security. J Fam Psychol. 2009;23(6):808–818. doi:10.1037/a0016740
  • Kokkinos CM, Algiovanoglou I, Voulgaridou I. Emotion regulation and relational aggression in adolescents: parental attachment as moderator. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(11):3146–3160. doi:10.1007/s10826-019-01491-9
  • Savage J. The association between attachment, parental bonds and physically aggressive and violent behavior: a comprehensive review. Aggress Violent Behav. 2014;19(2):164–178. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2014.02.004
  • Cyr M, Pasalich DS, McMahon RJ, Spieker SJ. The longitudinal link between parenting and child aggression: the moderating effect of attachment security. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2014;45(5):555–564. doi:10.1007/s10578-013-0424-4
  • Li J-B, Guo Y-J, Delvecchio E, Mazzeschi C. Chinese adolescents’ psychosocial adjustment: the contribution of mothers’ attachment style and adolescents’ attachment to mother. J Soc Pers Relat. 2020;37(8–9):2597–2619. doi:10.1177/0265407520932667
  • Zhang R, Wang L-X, Datu JAD, et al. High Qualities of relationships with parents and teachers contribute to the development of adolescent life satisfaction through resilience: a three-wave prospective longitudinal study. J Happiness Stud. 2023;24(4):1339–1365. doi:10.1007/s10902-023-00647-1
  • Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM. The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16(6):351–355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
  • Inzlicht M, Werner KM, Briskin JL, Roberts BW. Integrating models of self-regulation. Ann Rev Psychol. 2021;72(1):319–345. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-061020-105721
  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T. A General Theory of Crime. Redwood City: Stanford University Press; 1990.
  • Altenburger LE, Schoppe-Sullivan SJ. Contributions of parenting quality and coparenting relationship quality to the development of child executive functioning. Early Child Res Q. 2021;57:133–143. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.05.010
  • Burt CH, Simons RL, Simons LG. A longitudinal test of the effects of parenting and the stability of self-control: negative evidence for the general theory of crime. Criminology. 2006;44(2):353–396. doi:10.1111/j.1745-9125.2006.00052.x
  • Heylen J, Vasey MW, Dujardin A, et al. Attachment and effortful control: relationships with maladjustment in early adolescence. J Early Adolesc. 2015;37(3):289–315. doi:10.1177/0272431615599063
  • Sosic-Vasic Z, Kröner J, Schneider S, Vasic N, Spitzer M, Streb J. The association between parenting behavior and executive functioning in children and young adolescents. Front Psychol. 2017;8:472. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00472
  • Valcan DS, Davis H, Pino-Pasternak D. Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: a meta-analysis. Educ Psychol Rev. 2018;30(3):607–649. doi:10.1007/s10648-017-9411-9
  • Farley JP, Kim-Spoon J. The development of adolescent self-regulation: reviewing the role of parent, peer, friend, and romantic relationships. J Adolesc. 2014;37(4):433–440. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.03.009
  • Li JB, Willems YE, Stok FM, Deković M, Bartels M, Finkenauer C. Parenting and self-control across early to late adolescence: a three-level meta-analysis. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2019;14(6):967–1005. doi:10.1177/1745691619863046
  • Moilanen KL, Shaw DS, Fitzpatrick A. Self-regulation in early adolescence: relations with mother–son relationship quality and maternal regulatory support and antagonism. J Youth Adolesc. 2010;39(11):1357–1367. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9485-x
  • Halse M, Steinsbekk S, Hammar Å, Belsky J, Wichstrøm L. Parental predictors of children’s executive functioning from ages 6 to 10. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(3):410–426. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12282
  • Lam CB, Chung KKH, Li X. Parental warmth and hostility and child executive function problems: a longitudinal study of Chinese families. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1063. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01063
  • Denson TF, DeWall CN, Finkel EJ. Self-control and aggression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):20–25. doi:10.1177/0963721411429451
  • DeWall CN, Finkel EJ, Denson TF. Self-control inhibits aggression. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5(7):458–472. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00363.x
  • Li JB, Delvecchio E, Lis A, Nie YG, Di Riso D. Positive coping as mediator between self-control and life satisfaction: evidence from two Chinese samples. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;97:130–133. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.042
  • Nie YG, Li JB, Vazsonyi AT. Self-control mediates the associations between parental attachment and prosocial behavior among Chinese adolescents. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;96:36–39. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.077
  • Li JB, Dou K, Situ QM, Salcuni S, Wang Y-J, Friese M. Anger rumination partly accounts for the association between trait self-control and aggression. J Res Pers. 2019;81:207–223. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2019.06.011
  • Sun Y, Li JB, Oktaufik MPM, Vazsonyi AT. Parental attachment and externalizing behaviors among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of self-control. J Child Fam Stud. 2022;31(4):923–933. doi:10.1007/s10826-021-02071-6
  • Liu F, Chui H, Chung MC. The effect of parent–adolescent relationship quality on deviant peer affiliation: the mediating role of self-control and friendship quality. J Soc Pers Relat. 2020;37(10–11):2714–2736. doi:10.1177/0265407520937358
  • Özdemir Y, Vazsonyi AT, Çok F. Parenting processes and aggression: the role of self-control among Turkish adolescents. J Adolesc. 2013;36(1):65–77. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.09.004
  • Mancinelli E, Liberska HD, J-B L, et al. A cross-cultural study on attachment and adjustment difficulties in adolescence: the mediating role of self-control in Italy, Spain, China, and Poland. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(16):8827. doi:10.3390/ijerph18168827
  • Anderson C, Keltner D. The role of empathy in the formation and maintenance of social bonds. Behav Brain Sci. 2002;25(1):21–22. doi:10.1080/14616730010001587
  • Blair RJR. Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(4):698–718. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004
  • Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44:113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
  • Decety J, Svetlova M. Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2(1):1–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003
  • Batanova MD, Loukas A. What are the unique and interacting contributions of school and family factors to early adolescents’ empathic concern and perspective taking? J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41(10):1382–1391. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9768-5
  • Henschel S, Nandrino J-L, Pezard L, Ott L, Vulliez-Coady L, Doba K. The influence of attachment styles on autonomic correlates of perspective-taking. Biol Psychol. 2020;154:107908. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107908
  • Laghi F, D’Alessio M, Pallini S, Baiocco R. Attachment representations and time perspective in adolescence. Soc Indic Res. 2009;90(2):181–194. doi:10.1007/s11205-008-9249-0
  • Wei M, Liao KY, Ku TY, Shaffer PA. Attachment, self-compassion, empathy, and subjective well-being among college students and community adults. J Pers. 2011;79(1):191–221. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00677.x
  • Taubner S, White LO, Zimmermann J, Fonagy P, Nolte T. Attachment-related mentalization moderates the relationship between psychopathic traits and proactive aggression in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2013;41(6):929–938. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9736-x
  • Gómez-Leal R, Megías-Robles A, Gutiérrez-Cobo MJ, Cabello R, Fernández-Berrocal P. Personal risk and protective factors involved in aggressive behavior. J Interpers Violence. 2020;37(3–4):1489–1515. doi:10.1177/0886260520926322
  • Loudin JL, Loukas A, Robinson S. Relational aggression in college students: examining the roles of social anxiety and empathy. Aggress Behav. 2003;29(5):430–439. doi:10.1002/ab.10039
  • Carlo G, Mestre MV, Samper P, Tur A, Armenta BE. Feelings or cognitions? Moral cognitions and emotions as longitudinal predictors of prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Pers Individ Dif. 2010;48(8):872–877. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.010
  • Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? J Adolesc. 2011;34(1):59–71. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.001
  • Carlo G, Mestre MV, Samper P, Tur A, Armenta BE. The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parenting styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviors. Int J Behav Dev. 2010;35(2):116–124. doi:10.1177/016502541037592
  • Li X, Bian C, Chen Y, et al. Indirect aggression and parental attachment in early adolescence: examining the role of perspective taking and empathetic concern. Pers Individ Dif. 2015;86:499–503. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.008
  • Pan Y, Liang S, Shek DTL. Attachment insecurity and altruistic behavior among Chinese adolescents: mediating effect of different dimensions of empathy. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(16). doi:10.3390/ijerph191610371
  • Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggress Behav. 2006;32(6):540–550. doi:10.1002/ab.20154
  • Batanova MD, Loukas A. Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. J Youth Adolesc. 2011;40(11):1534–1543. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9634-x
  • Cillessen AHN, Borch C. Developmental trajectories of adolescent popularity: a growth curve modelling analysis. J Adolesc. 2006;29(6):935–959. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.05.005
  • Armsden GC, Greenberg MT. The inventory of parent and peer attachment: individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 1987;16(5):427–454. doi:10.1007/BF02202939
  • Li JB, Delvecchio E, Miconi D, Salcuni S, Di Riso D. Parental attachment among Chinese, Italian, and Costa Rican adolescents: a cross-cultural study. Pers Individ Dif. 2014;71:118–123. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.036
  • Teng Z, Griffiths MD, Nie Q, Xiang G, Guo C. Parent–adolescent attachment and peer attachment associated with internet gaming disorder: a longitudinal study of first-year undergraduate students. J Behav Addict. 2020;9(1):116–128. doi:10.1556/2006.2020.00011
  • Fung AL, Raine A, Gao Y. Cross-cultural generalizability of the Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ). J Pers Assess. 2009;91(5):473–479. doi:10.1080/00223890903088420
  • Xu Y, Raine A, Yu L, Krieg A. Resting heart rate, vagal tone, and reactive and proactive aggression in Chinese children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014;42(3):501–514. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9792-2
  • Li Q, Xiang G, Song S, Huang X, Chen H. Examining the associations of trait self-control with hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. J Happiness Stud. 2021;23(2):667–687. doi:10.1007/s10902-021-00418-w
  • Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
  • Lt H, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Ducharme J, Doyle AB, Markiewicz D. Attachment security with mother and father: associations with adolescents’ reports of interpersonal behavior with parents and peers. J Soc Pers Relat. 2002;19(2):203–231. doi:10.1177/0265407502192003
  • Duchesne S, Ratelle CF, Poitras S-C, Drouin E. Early adolescent attachment to parents, emotional problems, and teacher-academic worries about the middle school transition. J Early Adolesc. 2009;29(5):743–766. doi:10.1177/0272431608325502
  • Laible D. Attachment with parents and peers in late adolescence: links with emotional competence and social behavior. Pers Individ Dif. 2007;43(5):1185–1197. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.010
  • Song P, Zhang Z, Wang B, et al. The influence of trait empathy on reactive aggression: an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2018;133:102–110. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.08.002
  • Tamnes CK, Overbye K, Ferschmann L, et al. Social perspective taking is associated with self-reported prosocial behavior and regional cortical thickness across adolescence. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(9):1745–1757. doi:10.1037/dev0000541
  • van Hazebroek BCM, Olthof T, Goossens FA. Predicting aggression in adolescence: the interrelation between (a lack of) empathy and social goals. Aggress Behav. 2017;43(2):204–214. doi:10.1002/ab.21675
  • van der Zouwen M, Hoeve M, Hendriks AM, Asscher JJ, Stams GJJM. The association between attachment and psychopathic traits. Aggress Violent Behav. 2018;43:45–55. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2018.09.002
  • Li JB, Liberska H, Salcuni S, Delvecchio E. Aggressive perpetration and victimization among Polish male and female adolescents: the role of attachment to parents and self-control. Crime Delinq. 2018;65(3):401–421. doi:10.1177/0011128718787472