615
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Williams syndrome and its cognitive profile: the importance of eye movements

Pages 143-151 | Published online: 03 Jun 2015

Abstract

People with Williams syndrome (WS), a rare neurodevelopmental disorder that is caused by a deletion on the long arm of chromosome 7, often show an uneven cognitive profile with participants performing better on language and face recognition tasks, in contrast to visuospatial and number tasks. Recent studies have shown that this specific cognitive profile in WS is a result of atypical developmental processes that interact with and affect brain development from infancy onward. Using examples from language, face processing, number, and visuospatial studies, this review evaluates current evidence from eye-tracking and developmental studies and argues that domain general processes, such as the ability to plan or execute saccades, influence the development of these domain-specific outcomes. Although more research on eye movements in WS is required, the importance of eye movements for cognitive development suggests a possible intervention pathway to improve cognitive abilities in this population.

Introduction

Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare neurodevelopmental disorder that results in a specific clinical, behavioral, and cognitive profile. This uneven cognitive profile has been of interest in order to unravel links between genetic makeup, brain, and behavioral outcomes.Citation1Citation5 Studies have often claimed that WS provides evidence for a modular cognitive theory in which certain abilities can be spared or impaired and that these impairments can be directly linked to the WS genotype.Citation6,Citation7 Yet, as argued elsewhere, modules observed in adults emerge as a result of development, and domain-specific behavioral outcomes are supported by domain general cognitive processes from infancy onward.Citation8,Citation9 In order to study the connections between genes, brain, and behavioral outcomes, it is, therefore, important to understand how the cognitive processes in WS differ from those in typically developing (TD) populations and how they develop over time.Citation8Citation11 In addition, a better understanding of what domain general abilities relate to performance in WS, including those that can explain areas of cognitive strength as well as a weakness, can further aid the development of ecologically valid training and intervention programs. Using evidence from two cognitive areas considered to be a strength (face and language processing) and two areas of weakness (visuospatial and number abilities), this review will evaluate how atypical looking behavior observed in eye-tracking as well as developmental studies in WS can explain some of the domain-specific outcomes in adulthood. Eye-tracking studies were identified through searches (through to January 2015) in PubMed, Web of Science, and Google Scholar using “Williams syndrome” and “eye tracking” as keywords (for a similar methodology, see Falck-YtterCitation12 applied to research on autism).

WS: an uneven cognitive profile

WS is a relatively rare neurodevelopmental disorder with a prevalence between one in 20,000 and one in 7,500 live births.Citation13,Citation14 It is caused by a microdeletion on the long arm of chromosome 7, affecting ~26–28 genes.Citation15,Citation16 WS is diagnosed phenotypically based on clinical features as well as genetically using the gold standard fluorescent in situ hybridization testing to confirm the deletion of genes on chromosome 7.

WS is a multisystem disorder with a specific clinical, behavioral, and cognitive profile.Citation17,Citation18 During the last decade, WS has been of interest to researchers because of its uneven cognitive profile. Despite average overall IQ scores of 55, which indicates a mild-to-moderate intellectual disability, face processing and language abilities in WS are generally better compared to drawing, visuospatial, memory, and number processing.Citation7,Citation19,Citation20 Because of the apparent discrepancy between the cognitive domains, especially between language abilities and spatial cognitive deficits, WS has been taken as evidence in favor of a modular theory in which specific, independent, and innate modules can be spared or impaired.Citation7,Citation19Citation21 However, this discrepancy between the strengths and weaknesses in WS only emerges over time with verbal abilities developing at a faster rate than nonverbal abilities.Citation22 In addition, considerable heterogeneity has been reported when it comes to discrepancies within the WS profile with some participants scoring very low on language measures while others scoring within the normal range on visuospatial tasks.Citation23,Citation24 Finally, studies that have examined how abilities change throughout the life span have demonstrated that behavioral outcomes, even those in which WS are proficient, rely on different underlying cognitive processes, and thus, performance in WS is often atypical rather than just impaired (see Karmiloff-Smith et alCitation25,Citation26 and Laing et alCitation27 for examples). For example, although participants with WS perform within the normal range on the Benton face recognition task, a task in which participants need to match unfamiliar faces,Citation25,Citation28,Citation29 in-depth studies looking at the underlying cognitive processes of performance revealed that people with WS tend to look more at individual features of faces compared to controls who are more likely to process faces holistically. This suggests that individuals with WS might have weak central coherence and thus process faces atypically.Citation25,Citation30

Language is another area in which people with WS perform comparatively well, in that the speech of older individuals with WS is better than one would expect for their overall cognitive abilities with good auditory memory and vocabulary skills, while syntactic, morphological, and pragmatic abilities are lower than predicted by chronological age.Citation7,Citation31 However, a review of language abilities in WS has shown little evidence of language abilities being better than nonverbal abilities.Citation32 In addition, studies in infants with WS have found that not only the onset of language is delayedCitation27,Citation33 but also the performance on language tasks results from different underlying processes with language development following atypical pathways.Citation27 For example, in contrast to TD infants, those with WS do not use referential pointing before they start using referential language. Yet, this lack of referential pointing could not be attributed to delayed motor skills in WS.Citation27,Citation34 In addition, children with WS showed a reduced ability to follow the experimenter’s point. It has been argued that this impairment in pointing comprehension might affect their ability to learn vocabulary through parental pointing.Citation35

Visuospatial cognition, often assessed by drawing tasks, block building, or pattern construction tasks, has been found to be an extreme weakness in WS as performance is much lower with respect to age norms and overall IQ (for a review, see Farran and JarroldCitation36) and develops at a much slower rate than in TD controlsCitation22 with performance scores on pattern construction tasks often at the floor level.Citation37 There is evidence that even on visuospatial tasks where performance seems to be typical and in line with TD participants, participants with WS rely on atypical strategies. For example, visual illusions require participants to integrate a local part with surrounding elements into a coherent image. Behavioral studies have shown that participants with WS are susceptible to visual illusions to a similar extent as TD individuals.Citation38,Citation39 Yet, Grice et alCitation40 demonstrated that this performance is supported by atypical neural behaviors. The Kanizsa square illusion occurs when four Pacman disks are correctly aligned so that the contours of a white square are perceived. This illusion is not perceived when the disks are rotated, and it has been argued that this illusion depends on low-level visual processes. Participants with WS were able to perceive the contours of the Kanizsa square illusion to a similar extent as TD age-matched controls, which suggests that low-level visual processes are intact in WS. However, the N1 component is a negative deflection in the event-related potential waveform at about 145–180 ms poststimulus that has been shown to be particularly sensitive and reliable measure of processing of contour illusions. Although the N1 response itself was typical in WS, differences in amplitude of the N1 to the different stimuli were abnormal compared to controls. This suggests that the ability to perceive illusions is supported by atypical cognitive processes in WS.

Finally, studies that have investigated number abilities in children and adults with WS have revealed that arithmetic skills are severely impaired even in adulthood.Citation41,Citation42 Although children with WS are proficient at counting sequences,Citation43 they are impaired in their understanding of the meaning of counting or the cardinality principle.Citation44 Research in TD populations has provided evidence that number abilities rely upon two different systems: one for precise and accurate number abilities, such as counting, which relates to language and memory abilities, and the second is nonverbal magnitude system that relies upon the ratios presented and relates to people’s mental number line.Citation45 It has been argued that this magnitude system is predictive of mathematical abilities later on in life,Citation46,Citation47 and this system has been found to be impaired in WS.Citation43,Citation48Citation50 However, mathematical abilities are not only impaired, there is also evidence that they develop atypically: whereas non-verbal spatial abilities predicted the variance in TD controls, performance on counting tasks in the WS group related to their verbal abilities.Citation44 In addition, the developmental trajectory of the WS group was atypical as estimation abilities did not become faster and more accurate over time in contrast to TD children.Citation48

From domain general processes to domain specific outcomes

Although recent studies have investigated how performance in WS changes over time, in order to really understand the strengths and weaknesses of the WS cognitive profile and how these develop over time, one needs to not only trace these developmental trajectories back to infancy but also evaluate how domain general processes influence domain specific outcomes.Citation8,Citation35

Visual exploration is important during a number of learning processes, especially early in life as it allows infants to explore their environment before their motor abilities have developed sufficiently to explore through grasp and touch. Visual exploration can occur without moving one’s eyes or head (covert attention), but the greatest processing advantage happens by moving our eyes around (ie, through saccades) and fixating on places and objects within our environment (overt attention). Saccades do not happen at random but where and when the eye will move to is influenced by properties of the stimuli (bottom–up influences) as well as by the goals and interests of the viewer (top–down effects).Citation51 In addition, eye movements are coupled to attention processes, in that a position is fixated upon (stimulus orienting), then processed (sustained attention), and then disengaged from (attention disengagement). According to the oculomotor readiness hypothesis, attention and eye movements are strongly related to each other as where the eyes focus is generally also where the attention is shifted to, and attention and eye movements are controlled by the same brain structures that are responsible for oculomotor control.Citation52 Thus, in order to visually explore their environment, infants must learn to attend to objects and shift their attention to appropriate objects at the appropriate time. The ability to shift attention and make prompt saccades from a fixated target to a newer target under conditions of competition (ie, when both targets are present) develops from the age of 3–4 months onward in TD infants.Citation53 Failing to make appropriate saccades, whether through attention or oculomotor difficulties, would result in infants not being able to scan their environments properly, which impacts on the development of higher cognitive abilities. Indeed, recent studies in autism spectrum disorders have found evidence that the atypical scanning of faces and social scenes later on in life can be linked to saccadic eye movement deficits in infancy,Citation54 and thus, abnormal patterns of fixations can be used as a marker during early development of developmental disorders.Citation55

Studies that have investigated eye movements in WS have shown that these are impaired from infancy onward. Using a double-step saccade paradigm, Brown et alCitation56 found that while toddlers with WS have sustained attention similar to chronological and mental age-matched TD controls, they had problems orienting to a target as well as making a second saccade. Also, adult participants with WS demonstrated difficulties with eye movements for targets that appeared suddenly during a backward step saccade-adaptation task.Citation5 In the backward step saccade task, participants were presented with a target dot on the left of the screen followed by a second dot on the right of the screen. However, during the test trials, the position of the second dot was moved slightly toward the middle of the screen while the participants were making a saccade toward it, therefore evoking a saccadic adaptation. Although 16 out of the 24 participants showed evidence of saccadic adaptation, all participants showed difficulties with moving their eyes accurately toward the second target. Best and Karmiloff-Smith,Citation57 Karmiloff-Smith et al,Citation58 and Van Herwegen and Karmiloff-SmithCitation59 have argued that these differences in saccadic movements early on in life may affect how infants with WS learn from their environment and can be linked to cognitive outcomes later on in life. In addition, impaired abilities to plan or execute eye movements might explain the sticky fixation, an inability to disengage attention from a previously fixated target to a new target, that has been observed in WS.Citation27,Citation60 For example, in an eye-tracking study, it was found that toddlers with WS produced fewer voluntary eye movements in an antisaccade task, in which participants have to ignore a cue that appears on the opposite side of where the stimulus will appear, due to difficulties disengaging from the central stimulus. However, they did not show any difficulties in a Posner cueing paradigm and automatically paid attention to cued targets. In addition, they were slower to orient to invalidly cued targets.Citation60 This shows that although toddlers with WS have issues with planning saccades, this is not caused by difficulties in orienting attention but rather by an inability to disengage from a previously fixed target. The following section will discuss how the inability to plan or execute eye movements, which results in sticky fixation, can partially explain cognitive performance, including both areas of strength and weakness in face processing, language learning, visuospatial abilities, and number development in WS.

People often report that individuals with WS show unusual eye contact, in that they often keep smiling and staring at people’s faces during conversations.Citation61 Indeed, eye-tracking studies have shown that participants with WS look longer at faces in both static as well as complex dynamic social scenes in contrast to TD controls.Citation62Citation67 In addition, participants with WS often focus longer on the eye region within the face.Citation62,Citation65 This fixation on faces has been argued to lead to an expertise for faces from an early age and is caused by hypersociability in WS.Citation66 However, eye-tracking studies have shown that individuals with WS are not faster to detect a hidden face within unrelated landscape scenes.Citation65 In addition, faces distract participants with WS no greater compared to controls in a visual search task and they display a similar face bias in a probe classification task.Citation64 Participants with WS do, however, show longer fixations on the faces once they have been fixated upon compared to controls.Citation63,Citation65 Furthermore, a more recent study found that prolonged gazes to faces were only present when the social information was presented in the middle of the screen near a central fixation point but not when the stimuli were shown noncentrally.Citation67 These studies demonstrate that the social bias in WS is likely to be caused by a difficulty to disengage from faces, which is caused by sticky fixation, rather than by hypersociability. There are currently no studies that have investigated scan paths while participants with WS were administered an upright/inversion task. Yet, a dysfunctional scanning ability whereby sticky fixation prevents backward and forward saccades between the different features of a face could explain the atypical local processing of features in WS. Indeed, research in TD and autism spectrum disorder populations have shown that eye gaze between facial features allows for a holistic processing of faces, which impacts on facial recognition.Citation68,Citation69

Research in TD children has revealed that joint attention, the ability to attend to an object or event vis-à-vis a communication partner, has an important role for early language development, in that it is thought to help children identify the intended referent of the parent’s language and aid word–object mappings.Citation70 Studies in infants with WS have shown that joint attention abilities are impaired,Citation71 in that they are less likely to initiate joint attention than mentally matched controls.Citation27 In addition, they were impaired in responding to joint attention, such as following where the examiner was pointing to, and this impairment predicted their language comprehension and production scores as measured by the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories.Citation27 Although this study did not directly record eye movements of the participants, it is possible that the failure to respond to pointing gestures is caused by an inability to disengage from the experimenter’s face due to sticky fixation. Eye gaze plays an important role in the ability to respond to joint attention as shifts in eye gaze trigger a shift in orientation in order to align attention between individuals.Citation72 There are only two studies thus far that have evaluated gaze behavior in WS, but both have identified that gaze following is impaired in this population.Citation73,Citation74 A study by Tsirempolou et alCitation74 tested eleven participants with WS and found that adults and adolescents are impaired in following eye gaze direction and rely longer on head orientation to identify where people are looking compared to TD children. A recent study examined eye movements while children and adults with WS aged 8–28 years old viewed pictures during a free-viewing condition as well as during a cued condition in which participants were asked to detect the target of an actor’s gaze.Citation73 Participants with WS followed gaze in a similar way to controls matched for nonverbal ability when explicitly cued for, but their atypical prolonged fixation on the faces prevented them from accurately identifying the correct target. Thus, the fact that participants with WS have difficulties in shifting their attention away from faces is likely to impair their gaze behavior, which impacts joint attention abilities with further cascading effects for their word–object mapping and early vocabulary acquisition abilities.

The studies discussed thus far have shown that participants with WS show longer fixations on faces due to sticky fixation. It can be argued that the failure to disengage from faces is caused by hypersociability rather than a problem with saccade movements per se (although see discussion above). However, participants with WS have also been found to show evidence of sticky fixation on tasks that do not involve any social aspects. For example, a study investigating the scanning patterns in a few infants with WS while they were looking at large numerical displays demonstrated that they only looked at a few dots and did not scan the entire display.Citation58 This suggests that the inability to plan eye movements in WS causes them to only scan individual dots rather than the entire quantity, and this leads to failure to discriminate between large numerosities and impacts on the development of the magnitude system and number line as well as the development of their precise mathematical abilities.Citation58,Citation59

As discussed above, individuals with WS show difficulties in visuospatial production tasks, such as drawing tasks and block construction tasks, and this could not be attributed to sensory vision problems.Citation75 For example, when asked in the Navon task to copy large letters (eg, H) that are made up of small letters (eg, z), participants with WS are more likely to copy a few of the small letters that make up the large letter rather than the large letter itself. Earlier explanations that this impairment is caused by a local-processing bias have been refuted as no such bias has been reported in tasks where participants had to recognize rather than reproduce the stimuli.Citation76 An alternative explanation could be that the inability to plan eye movements impairs the number of fixations people with WS make on a target as well as the gaze frequency (the switching between two targets), and this impairs task performance. Indeed, a recent study has illustrated that participants with WS looked less frequently at the model when copying a house compared to MA-matched controls.Citation77 This has been argued to be caused by poor switching between the copy and the model as a result of poor eye movement planning. Looking less frequently to the model results in a higher working memory load, as one has to remember more elements when drawing, which causes atypical disoriented drawings. However, this study used button presses as a proxy for fixations on the model instead of traditional eye-tracking techniques. Thus, it is unclear where participants were looking during the task and whether they showed any evidence of sticky fixation. To date, only one study has investigated eye movements during a block design task using eye-tracking methodology. This study also demonstrated that children with WS fixated on models as well as their partial solutions less frequently than IQ-matched control children and TD adults.Citation78 Again, this study failed to examine the average length of fixations and cannot provide any firm support for the suggestion that the infrequent fixations toward the model are caused by sticky fixation. In addition, Hoffman et alCitation78 argue that the fact that participants with WS still made errors once they had fixated the model provides evidence that atypical looking alone cannot explain the errors made. Instead of being the cause for low accuracy, atypical looking might be the result of participants’ prediction that they would fail the task anyway. As described below, task performance relies upon a number of cognitive, behavioral, and environmental factors, and thus, it is likely that poor eye movement planning alone cannot explain the performance on visuospatial block design tasks. Indeed, it cannot explain why difficulties with eye movement planning can produce difficulties in production but not perception tasks. Nevertheless, sticky fixation may also provide an explanation for the atypical neural performance observed during visual illusions. Studies in TD participants have illustrated that the N1 component is sensitive to illusory contours in event-related potential studies and that an increase in N1 amplitude relates to a global search relative to a local search.Citation79 In addition, TD participants often make a number of successive fixations at various spatial locations to enhance the visual illusion, and thus, it is important to combine the information from these different saccades.Citation80 As mentioned before, Grice et al have shown that, although participants with WS can perceive visual illusions similar to TD controls, the neural mechanisms that support this ability are atypical.Citation40 Currently, no eye-tracking studies have examined eye movements during illusion perception tasks in WS. However, it is possible that the fact that no differentiation was found in N1 amplitude between the different stimuli in WS, in contrast to controls, might suggest that participants with WS fail to use a global strategy, probably caused by the fact that they only make a single fixation.Citation40 If this is the case, it would show that although behavioral outcomes for perceptual tasks are similar to controls, the low-level visual processes that support higher level processing are impaired in WS.

Current limitations and future studies

Although deficient eye movement planning resulting in sticky fixation can explain a number of strengths and difficulties observed in the WS cognitive profile, research remains limited, in that the studies often included small sample sizes and there is a lack of longitudinal as well as developmental studies. Studies investigating eye movements from infancy onward across development are necessary, in that even when we know that the scanning patterns in WS infants are atypical for a certain type of stimuli, different developmental outcomes are possible. For example, individuals with WS might develop compensatory strategies. Alternatively, their scanning abilities might be merely delayed or remain atypical throughout development. Thus far, studies have shown that scanning paths remain atypical in WS for social stimuli,Citation63Citation65 yet research, including eye tracking with nonsocial stimuli, is still limited. In addition, most of the existent studies have included only a small number of participants, often from a wide age range, which might explain the large variability in the data and also why some studies did not find any evidence of atypical scanning or difficulties to disengage when using social stimuli in WS.Citation81Citation83 Comparing eye movements on social and nonsocial stimuli will further our understanding of how hypersociability and attention difficulties in general contribute to atypical scanning patterns.

In addition, there are methodological issues that make comparisons between studies difficult, including the different kinds and methods of eye tracking as well as different analyses (see HanleyCitation84 for a discussion). For example, while some studies tracked participants’ eyes at 250 Hz, others have used much lower frequencies (eg, 60 Hz or 120 Hz), therefore relying on fewer samples of where the eyes were positioned within a certain time frame. Also the type of stimuli, for example whether static versus dynamic scenes were used, has been found to affect scanning paths in WS.Citation64

It is also possible that abilities that seem to be unrelated in adulthood are related in infancy and can explain some aspects of the WS cognitive profile. For example, research has shown that infants with WS focus on auditory input.Citation34 Thus, upon hearing their mother’s voice, infants will turn to her upon which they will see their mother’s face. It is therefore possible that the fascination with faces in WS stems from a focus on auditory stimuli combined with problems with visual disengagement and a heightened social drive. Although it is possible that such domain specific abilities explain to an extent the face-processing and language abilities, there is limited evidence that hypersociability alone can explain the atypical behavior observed on nonverbal tasks. Recent studies have shown that the atypical scanning paths for social stimuli in WS are related to their anxiety and social reciprocity.Citation82 Thus, future studies should not only focus on eye movements but also on a number of domain general factors as well as environmental factors. For example, numerical abilities depend on verbal abilities, visuospatial abilities, attention, working memory, anxiety levels, and environmental influences. Therefore, any deficit in one or more of these domains from infancy onward can affect numerical abilities later on in life.

Finally, with exception of motion perception,Citation85,Citation86 very few studies have investigated the oculomotor system in WS directly, and it is unclear how stimulus-driven factors such as color,Citation87 luminance, and visual clutter influence fixation duration in WS. However, it has been shown that adults with WS have a less efficient oculomotor system that results in large saccadic variability.Citation5,Citation88 Yet, saccades did improve during a saccade-adaptation task, which suggests that individuals with WS would benefit from training programs aimed to improve saccadic control. Such training studies should be aimed at young participants with WS as a recent review has shown that, although there are not many training paradigms that improve attentional control with positive transfer to other cognitive abilities, training studies aimed at younger participants reported more widespread transfer of training effects.Citation89

Conclusion

Individuals with WS often show an uneven cognitive profile in which language and face-processing abilities are better in comparison to number and visuospatial abilities. However, developmental studies have shown that this uneven cognitive profile is the outcome of a number of atypical developmental processes. Specifically, atypical domain general processes, such as sticky fixation, which result from problems with saccade planning, influence and interact with specific cognitive developmental processes from infancy onward. There is probably a very complex relationship between attention and the cognitive processes described above, and it is certain that other domain general factors such as executive functioning, auditory processing, and low-level visual abilities play a role in the language, face processing, number, and visuospatial development in WS. Yet, the current overview has shown that differences in one such domain general ability, ie, scanpaths in infancy, can explain a number of behavioral outcomes observed in adults with WS. This is very promising for training studies, in that it was found that although participants with WS have problems with saccade planning, they demonstrated saccadic adaptation, which shows that they do have the capacity for saccadic motor learning and that their oculomotor system can be trained.Citation5

Nonetheless, the number of eye-tracking studies providing concrete evidence about eye movements in WS is still limited and there is a lack of developmental studies examining the role of eye movements in cognitive processes from infancy onward. Such research is needed in order to fully appreciate the importance of saccadic movements in relation to the uneven cognitive profile in WS, especially as not all studies have found evidence for sticky fixation in WS. Therefore, large studies are required that allow the investigation of subtypes and examine the individual differences within the WS cognitive profile and how these relate to their fixation patterns.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Gaia Scerif and the reviewers for comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • EckertMAGalaburdaAMMillsDKBellugiUKorenbergJRReissALThe neurobiology of Williams syndrome: cascading influences of visual system impairment?Cell Mol Life Sci2006631867187516810457
  • KorenbergJRChenXNHirotaHGenome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndromeJ Cogn Neurosci20001218910710953236
  • Meyer-LindenbrugAMervisCBFaithKNeural mechanisms in Williams syndrome: a unique window to genetic influences on cognition and behaviourNat Rev Neurosci2006738039316760918
  • ReissALEckertMARoseFEAn experiment of nature: brain anatomy parallels cognition and behaviour in Williams syndromeJ Neurosci20042425009501515163693
  • Van der GeestJNLagers-van HaselenGCFrensMASaccadic adaptation in Williams-Beuren syndromeInvest Ophthalmol Vis Sci20064741464146816565380
  • BellugiUAdolphsRCassandryCChilesMTowards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndromeNeuroreport1999101510094123
  • BellugiULichtenbergerLJonesWLaiZSt-GeorgeMThe neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknessesJ Cogn Neurosci200012Suppl 172910953231
  • Karmiloff-SmithADevelopment itself is the key to understanding developmental disordersTrends Cogn Sci1998238939821227254
  • Karmiloff-SmithAModules, genes and evolution: what have we learned from atypical development?MunakataYJohnsonMProcesses of Change in Brain and Cognitive DevelopmentOxfordOxford University Press2006563583
  • Karmiloff-SmithANativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disordersDev Psychol2009451566319209990
  • ThomasMSCKarmiloff-SmithAAre developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelingBehav Brain Sci20022572778814598624
  • Falck-YtterYBölteSGredebäckGEye tracking in early autism researchJ Neruodev Disord20135128
  • MorrisCADemseySALeonardCODiltsCBlackburnBLNatural history of Williams syndrome: physical characteristicsJ Pediatr198811323183262456379
  • StrömmePBjornstadPGRamstadKPrevalence estimation of Williams syndromeJ Child Neurol20021726927112088082
  • TassabehjiMWilliams-Beuren syndrome: a challenge for genotype-phenotype correlationsHum Mol Genet2003122229237
  • DonnaiDKarmiloff-SmithAWilliams syndrome: from genotype through to the cognitive phenotypeAm J Med Genet20009716417111180224
  • MartensMAWilsonSJReutensDCResearch review: Williams syndrome: a critical review of the cognitive, behavioural, and neuroanatomical phenotypeJ Child Psychol Psychiatry20084957660818489677
  • CarrascoXCastilloSAravenaTRothhammerPAboitizFWilliams syndrome: pediatric, neurologic, and cognitive developmentPediatr Neurol200532316617215730896
  • BellugiUWangPPJerniganTLWilliams syndrome: an unusual neuropsychological profileBromanSHGrahamJAtypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain FunctionHillsdale, NJLawrence Erlbaum Associates19942356
  • MervisCBRobinsonBFBertrandJMorrisCAKlein-TasmanBPArmstrongSCThe Williams syndrome cognitive profileBrain Cogn20004460462811104544
  • BellugiUDissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndromeBishopDMVMogfordKLanguage Development in Exceptional CircumstancesEdinburghChurchill Livingstone1988177189
  • JarroldCBaddeleyADHewesAKPhillipsCA longitudinal assessment of diverging verbal and non-verbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotypeCortex200137342343111485066
  • PorterMAColthaertMCognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndromeDev Neuropsychol200527227530615753050
  • StojanovikVPerkinsMHowardSLinguistic heterogeneity in Williams syndromeClin Linguist Phon2006207&854755217056485
  • Karmiloff-SmithAThomasMAnnazDExploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: the importance of building developmental trajectoriesJ Child Psychol Psychiatry20044571258127415335346
  • Karmiloff-SmithAGrantJBerthoudIDaviesMHowlinPUdwinOLanguage and Williams syndrome: how intact is “intact”?Child Dev19976822462629180000
  • LaingEButterworthGAnsariDAtypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndromeDev Sci200252233246
  • Plesa-SkwererDFajaSSchofieldCVerbalisATager-FlusbergHPerceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in individuals with Williams syndromeAm J Ment Retard20061111152616332153
  • Tager-FlusbergHPlesa-SkwererDFajaSJosephRMPeople with Williams syndrome process faces holisticallyCognition200389112412893122
  • DeruelleCManciniJLivetMOCassé-PerrotCde SchonenSConfigural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndromeBrain Cogn19994127629810585239
  • LawsGBishopDVMPragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairmentInt J Lang Commun Disord2004391456414660186
  • BrockJLanguage abilities in Williams syndrome: a critical reviewDev Psychopathol2007199712717241486
  • MervisCBRobinsonBFRoweMLBecerraAMKlein-TasmanBPLanguage abilities in Individuals with Williams syndromeAbbedutoLInternational Review of Research in Mental RetardationOrlando, FLAcademic Press20033581
  • MervisCBMorrisCABertrandJRobinsonBFWilliams syndrome: findings from an integrated program of researchTager-FlusbergHNeurodevelopmental DisordersCambridgeMIT Press199965110
  • Karmiloff-SmithAAtypical epigenesisDev Sci2007101848817181704
  • FarranEKJarroldCVisuo-spatial cognition in Williams syndrome: reviewing and accounting for the strengths and weaknesses in performanceDev Neuropsychol2003231–217320012730024
  • Van HerwegenJRundbladGDavelaarEJAnnazDVariability and standardised test profiles in typically developing children and children with Williams syndromeBr J Dev Psychol20112988389421995742
  • FarranEKColeVLPerceptual grouping and distance estimates in typical and atypical development: Comparing performance across perception, drawing and construction tasksBrain Cogn20086815716518508173
  • PalomaresMOgbonnaCLandauBEgethHNormal susceptibility to visual illusions in abnormal development: evidence from Williams syndromePerception20093818619919400429
  • GriceSJHaanMDHalitHERP abnormalities of illusory contour perception in Williams syndromeNeuroreport2003141773177714534418
  • UdwinODaviesMHowlinPA longitudinal study of cognitive and education attainment in Williams syndromeDev Med Child Neurol199638102010298913183
  • O’HearnKLandauBMathematical skill in individuals with Williams syndrome: evidence from a standardized mathematics batteryBrain Cogn20076423824617482333
  • PatersonSJGirelliLButterworthBKarmiloff-SmithAAre numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down’s syndromeJ Child Psychol Psychiatry200647219020416423150
  • AnsariDDonlanCThomasMEwingSKarmiloff-SmithAWhat makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number developmentJ Exp Child Psychol200385506212742762
  • FeigensonLDehaeneSSpelkeESCore systems of numberTrends Cogn Sci20048730731415242690
  • BonnyJWLourencoSFThe approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool yearsJ Exp Child Psychol201211437538823201156
  • MazzoccoMMMFeigensonLHalberdaJPreschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performancePLoS ONE201169e2374921935362
  • AnsariDDonlanCKarmiloff-SmithATypical and atypical development of visual estimation abilitiesCortex20074375876817710827
  • KrajcsiALukacsAIgacsJRacsmanyMPlehCNumerical abilities in Williams syndrome: dissociating the analogue magnitude system and verbal retrievalJ Clin Exp Neurospychol2009314439446
  • Van HerwegenJAnsariDXuFKarmiloff-SmithASmall and large number processing in infants and toddlers with Williams syndromeDev Sci200811563764318801117
  • RaynerKEye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of researchPsychol Bull19981243724229849112
  • HoffmanJSubramaniamBThe role of visual attention in saccadic eye movementsPercept Psychophys1995577877957651803
  • AtkinsonJHoodBWattam-BellJBraddickOJChanges in infants’ ability to switch visual attention in the first three months of lifePerception1992216436531488267
  • NeumannDSpezioMLPivenJAdoplhsRLooking you in the mouth: abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attentionSoc Cogn Affect Neurosci20061319420218985106
  • WassSJonesEJHGiglaTShorter spontaneous fixation durations in infants with later emerging autismNature201558284
  • BrownJHJohnsonMHPatersonSJGilmoreRLonghiEKarmiloff-SmithASpatial representation and attention in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndromeNeuropsychologia2003411037104612667539
  • BestGKarmiloff-SmithAWhy development matters in neurodevelopmental disordersVan HerwegenJRibyDNeurodevelopmental Disorders: Research Challenges and SolutionsHovePsychology Press20141933
  • Karmiloff-SmithAD’SouzaDDekkerTGenetic and environmental vulnerabilities: the importance of cross-syndrome comparisonsProc Natl Acad Sci U S A20121092172611726523045661
  • Van HerwegenJKarmiloff-SmithAGenetic developmental disorders and numerical competence across the lifespanCohen KadoshRDowkerAOxfordHandbook of Numerical CognitionOxfordOxford University PressIn press2015
  • CornishKScerifGKarmiloff-SmithATracing syndrome-specific trajectories of attention across the life-spanCortex20074367268517710820
  • JonesWBellugiUChilesMReillyJLincolnAAdoplhsRHyper-sociability in Williams syndromeJ Cogn Neurosci200012Suppl 1304610953232
  • RibyDMHancockPJBViewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autismNeuropsychologia2008462855286018561959
  • RibyDMHancockPJBLooking at movies and cartoons: eye-tracking evidence from Williams syndrome and autismJ Intellect Disabil Res200953216921819192099
  • RibyDMJonesNBrownPHAttention to faces in Williams syndromeJ Autism Dev Disord20114191228123921125323
  • RibyDMHancockPJBDo faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movementsJ Autism Dev Disord200939342143118787936
  • PorterMAShawTMarshPJAn unusual attraction to the eyes in Williams-Beuren syndrome: a manipulation of facial affect while measuring face scanpathsCogn Neuropsychiatry201015650553020432078
  • WilliamsTAPorterMALangdonRViewing social scenes: a visual scan-path study comparing fragile X syndrome and Williams syndromeJ Autism Dev Disord2013431880189423224515
  • HendersonJMWilliamsCCFalkRKEye movements are functional during face learningMem Cognit200533198106
  • WilsonCEPalermoRBrockJVisual scan paths and recognition of facial identity in autism spectrum disorder and typical developmentPLoS One201275e3768122666378
  • MoralesMMundyPDelgadoCEFResponding to joint attention across the 6- to 24-month age period and early language acquisitionJ Appl Dev Psychol200021283298
  • BertrandJMervisCRiceCEAdamsonLDevelopment of joint attention by a toddler with Williams syndromePaper presented at: the Gatlinberg Conference on Research and Theory in Mental Retardation and Developmental DisabilitiesGatlinberg1993
  • LangtonSRHBruceVReflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of othersVis Cogn19996541567
  • RibyDMHancockPJBJonesNHanleyMSpontaneous and cued gaze-following in autism and Williams syndromeJ Neurodev Disord2013511323663405
  • TsirempolouELawrenceKLeeKEwingSKarmiloff-SmithAUnderstanding the social meaning of the eyes: is Williams syndrome so different from autism?World J Pediatr200624288296
  • AtkinsonJAnkerSBraddickONokesLMasonAVisual and visuospatial development in young children with Williams syndromeDev Med Child Neurol20014333033711368486
  • FarranEKJarroldCGathercoleSEDivided attention, selective attention and drawing: processing preferences in Williams syndrome are dependent on the tasks administeredNeuropsychologia20034167668712591025
  • HudsonKDFarranEKLooking around houses: attention to a model when drawing complex shapes in Williams syndrome and typical developmentRes Dev Disabil2013343029303923827984
  • HoffmanJELandauBPaganiBSpatial breakdown in spatial construction: evidence from eye fixations in children with Williams syndromeCogn Psychol20034626030112694695
  • ConsciMTollnerTLeszczynskiMMullerHJThe time-course of global and local attentional guidance in Kanizsa-figure detectionNeuropsychologia2011492456246421549722
  • LiinasuoMRovamoJKojoIEffects of spatial configuration and number of fixations on Kanizsa triangle detectionInvest Ophthalmol Vis Sci199738255425659375575
  • HanleyMRibyDMCaswellSRooneySBackELooking and thinking: how individuals with Williams syndrome make judgments about mental statesRes Dev Disabil2013344466447624139712
  • KirkHHockingDRibyDMCornishKLinking social behaviour and anxiety to attention to emotional faces in Williams syndromeRes Dev Disabil2013344608461624210355
  • Doherty-SneddonGWhittleLRibyDMGaze aversion during social style interactions in autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndromeRes Dev Disabil201334161662623123875
  • HanleyMEye-tracking and neurodevelopmental disorders: evidence from cross-syndrome comparisonsVan HerwegenJRibyDNeurodevelopmental Disorders: Research Challenges and SolutionsHovePsychology Press2014219240
  • AtkinsonABraddickOFrom genes to brain development to phenotypic behavior: “dorsal-stream vulnerability” in relation to spatial cognition, attention, and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental disordersProg Brain Res201118926126321489394
  • Castelo-BrancoMMendesMSebastiãoARVisual phenotype in Williams-Beuren syndrome challenges magnocellular theories explaining human neurodevelopmental visual cortical disordersJ Clin Invest2007117123720372918037993
  • FarranEKCranwellMBAlvarezJFranklinAColour discrimination and categorization in Williams syndromeRes Dev Disabil201334103352336023911545
  • van der GeestJNLagers-van HaselenGCvan HagenJMSaccade dysmestria in Williams-Beuren syndromeNeuropsychologia200442556957614725795
  • WassSVScerifGJohnsonMHTraining attentional control and working memory – is younger-better?Dev Rev201232360387