3,696
Views
136
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Current perspectives on attachment and bonding in the dog–human dyad

, &
Pages 71-79 | Published online: 24 Feb 2015

Abstract

This article reviews recent research concerning dog–human relationships and how attributes that arise from them can be measured. It highlights the influence of human characteristics on dog behavior, and consequently, the dog–human bond. Of particular importance are the influences of human attitudes and personality. These themes have received surprisingly little attention from researchers. Identifying human attributes that contribute to successful dog–human relationships could assist in the development of a behavioral template to ensure dyadic potential is optimized. Additionally, this article reveals how dyadic functionality and working performance may not necessarily be mutually inclusive. Potential underpinnings of various dog–human relationships and how these may influence dogs’ perceptions of their handlers are also discussed. The article considers attachment bonds between humans and dogs, how these may potentially clash with or complement each other, and the effects of different bonds on the dog–human dyad as a whole. We review existing tools designed to measure the dog–human bond and offer potential refinements to improve their accuracy. Positive attitudes and affiliative interactions seem to contribute to the enhanced well-being of both species, as reflected in resultant physiological changes. Thus, promoting positive dog–human relationships would capitalize on these benefits, thereby improving animal welfare. Finally, this article proposes future research directions that may assist in disambiguating what constitutes successful bonding between dogs and the humans in their lives.

Introduction

Symbiotic relationships between dogs and humans are thought to date back at least 18,000 years.Citation1 Although it has been argued that the tendency for dogs to form close relationships with humans can be attributed to social dominance, with dogs seeing humans as surrogate pack leaders,Citation2 social and associative learning appear highly relevant to dog–human interactions.Citation3Citation5 Dogs seem to possess an ability to interpret and respond to human signaling that exceeds that of chimpanzees.Citation6Citation8 The proficiency of dogs and extensively socialized wolves at such tasks is thought to reflect their adeptness at social scavenging or cooperation and associating certain human gestures with the provision of food, both of which can facilitate rapid learning.Citation9,Citation10 These days, dogs are used in various contexts that exploit their responsiveness to human direction, such as security work, moving livestock, and assisting humans with disabilities. It may be argued that working dog–human relationships are unidirectional, as they depend only on the function the dog performs. However, given that relational factors can affect dog performance,Citation11 it is likely that, as with companion dog–owner relationships, these relationships are bidirectional.Citation12 In light of this, the current article will discuss dog–human relationships on a general level, with particular emphasis on companion dogs and their owners.

An attachment bond is a close, emotional relationship between two individuals.Citation13 The dog–human dyad is believed to involve attachment bonds similar to those that characterize human caregiver–infant relationships.Citation14 Dogs have shown behaviors indicative of an attachment relationship, defined according to Bowlby.Citation13 One such behavior is proximity seeking, where the animal will seek out the attachment figure as a means of coping with stress.Citation15 Conversely, the absence of an attachment figure can trigger behaviors indicative of separation-related distress in dogs.Citation16 The presence of a human can also attenuate the effect of a stressful event, thereby constituting the so-called safe haven effect of attachment theory.Citation17 Dogs have also demonstrated the so-called secure base effect, where the presence of an attachment figure allows dogs to more freely investigate novel objects.Citation18 Therefore, the dog–human attachment bond is characterized by all four features of attachment bonds that arise in human caregiver–infant relationships. Moreover, there is some evidence of interactions between owner and dog attachment patterns,Citation19 although this is disputed.Citation20 What remains unknown are the factors that influence the nature of attachment bonds dogs develop with their human handlers or owners. If certain attachment styles are beneficial in different working dog contexts, human behaviors could be tailored accordingly to produce more functional dyads.

Human factors that contribute to dog behavior and training outcomes are the focus of a growing body of research. Several of these factors are likely to influence dogs’ affective, or emotional, states and thereby influence their behavioral output. Many human interventions, such as use of positive reinforcementCitation21 and affiliative interactions,Citation22 are likely to produce a positive affective state in a dog, leading to more favorable behavioral responses, such as obedience during training. However, it is important to note that expert timing of these interventions is essential for training success.Citation23 Hence, the expert application of such attributes is suitable for encouraging certain behaviors in dogs and likely contributes to a positive emotional bond. Focusing on improving these characteristics offers a promising solution for dog owners with relatively suboptimal dog-handling ability, or dogmanship, defined as an individual’s ability to interact with and train dogs. However, the influence of human psychological characteristics, such as personality and attitudes, on dogmanship and the dog–human relationship remains unclear. Thus far, the tantalizing notion that certain personality dimensions may predispose an individual to interact skillfully with dogs remains unconfirmed.

The physiological and emotional benefits that ensue from a positive dog–human relationship extend to both members of the dyad. For dogs, humans seem to represent a social partner that, in addition to providing information pertinent to food acquisition, can be a source of emotional fulfilment and attachment.Citation16 Similarly, forming relationships with, or simply interacting with, dogs has been associated with several emotional and psychological health benefits for humans.Citation24,Citation25 Hence, fostering secure, positive emotional bonds between humans and dogs generally promotes well-being. This article aims to review current literature on the dog–human relationship, especially that regarding attachment and bonding. Assessing dog–human relationships through the use of a scientifically validated tool may reveal which dyads successfully capitalize on mutual benefits and those that may require intervention. This article will review existing tools designed to measure the dog–human bond. Including all possible measures of dog–human relationships, especially those that focus only on a singular aspect of these relationships, such as dog–human attachment, is well beyond the scope of this article (for reviews see Wilson and NettingCitation26 and AndersonCitation27). So, we will attempt to focus primarily on those measures that reflect a significant portion of the dog–human relationship.

A greater understanding of the mechanisms of well-matched dog–human dyads may foster the promotion of successful dyads through the skillful application of certain behaviors on the part of the human. Moreover, this may reduce the incidence of dysfunctional dog–human relationships, which can be harmful to both dyadic members,Citation28,Citation29 as well as the broader community.Citation30

Perceptions and attitudes of dog owners towards dogs

The influence of owner attitudes to or viewpoints on dog behavior and welfare represents a relatively recent avenue of research. Dogs belonging to those who regard their animals as social partners or meaningful companions have been shown to have relatively low salivary cortisol concentrations.Citation15 This suggests that positive owner attitudes may moderate stress in canine companions. Furthermore, Norwegian dog owners with more positive attitudes towards their dogs also had higher animal empathy scores, which correlated with how they rated pain in dogs.Citation31 Hence, empathetic dog owners with positive attitudes may be more aware of pain in their animal and readily respond to it, thus minimizing stress. Such handlers may have what Blouin described as humanistic views of their animals, regarding them as surrogate humans that offer affective benefits, or protectionistic views of their animals, regarding them as valuable companions with their own interests.Citation32 Blouin also identified a third perspective, dominionistic, whereby animals are viewed with low regard and valued only for their usefulness.Citation32 One would predict that dominionistic handlers would have less positive attitudes towards their pets, and consequently, the affective benefits to either dog or human may be limited.

Some sheepdog handlers reportedly regard dogs dominionistically, as tools that will eagerly please the pack leader (the human) by driving stock towards them.Citation33 More plausibly, the dogs in question drive stock chiefly because this is a self-rewarding behavior.Citation23 Similarly, it has been reported that dog handlers often misinterpret several aspects of their dog’s behavior or temperament, such as trainability,Citation34 play signals,Citation35 emotional arousal,Citation36 and acute stress.Citation37 A survey of 565 dog owners revealed that most participants overestimated the cognitive capabilities of dogs,Citation38 reflecting how widespread unrealistic expectations of companion animals can be. Such misunderstandings, in the absence of psychological evidence, such as believing certain dog behaviors to be indicative of the animal’s guilt, may be responsible for instances of conflict in dog–human relationships and contribute to relationship breakdowns.Citation39 These studies appear to be indicative of a general lack of understanding of dog behavior among dog owners and handlers that, if rectified, could improve dog handling, or dogmanship, on a broader scale.

Owner factors affecting dog–human relationships

The operant conditioning quadrant that a dog handler tends to use when training a dog can influence the dog’s affective state, relative arousal, and ultimately, its behavioral output.Citation40 Generally, producing a positive affective state and moderate arousal in a dog maximizes the probability of that dog demonstrating the desired behavior.Citation40 In a broader sense, human behavior can likewise influence dog behavior by changing emotional valence and arousal. In the literature, human behaviors that likely contribute to a more positive affective state and consequently more positive expectations in dogs are often those that provide the dog with resources of emotional value, such as affiliation,Citation22 human attention,Citation41 and safety.Citation17 However, the influence of human attachment on dog behavior remains ambiguous. An owner with an avoidant attachment to their dog is reported to have more negative expectations regarding the behavior of their dog.Citation42

As owner attitudes have been connected to dog behavior and stress,Citation15 insecure human–dog relationships may be related to poor stress coping in dogs, thereby compromising welfare and contributing to relinquishment. Aligning with this, owners relinquishing their dogs at animal shelters tend to score lower on companion animal attachment compared with existing dog owners.Citation43 Additionally, owners who are predisposed to view their interactions with their dog as negative may be more likely to fall victim to such miscommunication and then consider relinquishment.

A study investigating the influence of certain owner factors on the dog–human relationship found a significant negative correlation between owners using the dog for ‘company only’ and emotional closeness.Citation44 The authors defined ‘company only’ as non-participation in herding, hunting, agility, dog shows, or working dog training. Time spent as a dyad may have a critical influence on this observation, as the activities cited by Meyer and ForkmanCitation44 would arguably require more owner engagement with the dog, an attribute that has been reported as critical in the dog–human relationship.Citation11,Citation45 Additionally, humans using their dogs for company alone may arguably have a dominionistic viewpoint of their dogs and hence may be more likely to experience relationship dysfunction than those who are more willing to engage in activities with their animal.

Investigating the effect of human personality on dog–human relationships is of particular relevance when conceptualizing dogmanship as it holds promise of identifying specific characteristics of individuals who are outstanding with their dogs. More specifically, current research suggests the Big Five personality dimension of neuroticism may provide some preliminary indication of the dogmanship of an individual dog owner. High neuroticism scores in dog owners have been associated with poor canine performance in operational tasks,Citation15,Citation46 handlers’ use of excessive signaling during training,Citation47 and delayed responses to owner commands.Citation47 Taken together, these results suggest that high neuroticism in dog owners contributes to poor dyadic functionality and that individuals with good dogmanship are likely to score low on this trait. Nevertheless, owners with high neuroticism have been observed to be more socially attractive to their dogs,Citation48 with these dyads being rated as being more friendly than other dyads by experimental observersCitation46 and having lower salivary cortisol concentrations in dogs.Citation15 Additionally, owners in these dyads were more likely to consider their dogs as social supporters or partners.Citation15 These observations suggest that quality of life for both members of the dog–human dyad does not necessarily relate to performance in practical tasks. Future analyses should focus on examining various dog–human interactions with owners of different personality types and dog training experience levels, to clarify whether high neuroticism correlates with canine welfare and the implications this has for dog training.

Accounting for dog and human personalities when matching dogs with humans has potential to reduce behavioral conflict in the dog–human dyad by preventing mismatches. Significant correlations have been observed between the personality facets of openness and agreeableness and owner satisfaction with the dog–human relationship.Citation49 Similarly, Curb et alCitation50 reported that owner satisfaction correlated with dog-and-owner matching on certain behavioral traits, such as having an active lifestyle and creativity, which correlate with extraversionCitation51 and openness,Citation52 respectively. To further investigate dog–owner personality matching, future studies should use validated personality dimensions in their assessment, accompanied by direct behavioral observations of dog personality to prevent owner bias.

Dog perceptions of the dog–human relationship

To effectively assess dog–human relationships, canine factors must be considered. It has been hypothesized that dogs have been selected for increased deference to humans and that the dog–human relationship has a defined social hierarchy.Citation12,Citation53 Although intraspecific dominance relationships have been observed in dogs, evidence suggests that dogs do not generally view humans as surrogate dogs; thus, social dominance may not apply in the dog–human dyad.Citation54 Despite engaging in interactions with other dogs, intraspecific play does not suppress the motivation for dogs to interact with owners,Citation55 suggesting each interaction fulfils a different role. Furthermore, unlike the presence of a familiar dog, the presence of a familiar human has been shown to reduce plasma cortisol concentrations in dogs in a novel environment.Citation56 As such, it is likely dog–dog and dog–human interactions are motivationally as well as functionally distinct, and thus, it is unlikely that dog–human interactions operate as part of a dominance hierarchy. It may be that, rather than deference to humans, reduced fear of humans may have had a selection advantage, with these animals being more likely to scavenge from humans.

There are several hypotheses regarding the domestication of the dog and the particular behavioral traits that had a selection advantage. It has been argued that dogs have been selected for their ability to perceive human signals and cooperate with humans.Citation10 Dogs have been shown to outperform wolves in establishing eye contact with humans and adapting their behavior to human attitudes.Citation57,Citation58 However, given that socialized wolves do seem to interact with their human raisers as social partners, this cooperation may be more due to environment and ontogenic events than human-directed selection.Citation9 Additionally, wolves can outperform dogs in social-learning tasks with a conspecific demonstrator.Citation59 This suggests that wolves may be more cooperative with conspecifics than dogs. Consequently, it has been suggested that dog–human cooperation has evolved on the foundation of wolf–wolf cooperation, and during the domestication process, dogs have become less cooperative with each other.Citation12 This canine cooperation theory aligns with current research. However, given that existing dog–wolf comparisons compare companion dogs to wolves with limited socializationCitation7 or wolves that have been clicker-trained,Citation9 there is a need for more balanced comparisons.Citation60 Future studies should incorporate bigger, more diverse sample sizes of dogs and wolves to assess whether these variations exist between breeds and entire versus desexed animals. Moreover, the modern wolf is genetically distinct from the ancestor of the modern dog.Citation61 As such, given that modern wolves may not resemble their ancient counterparts, using dog–wolf comparisons to investigate the domestication of the dog may be of limited relevance.

There are early reports that social learning in the form of Do As I Do (DAID) training can be as successful as shaping and clicker-training methods in the training of simple commands and superior to them in the training of complex or sequential commands.Citation62 These results highlight that imitation can occur in dog–human dyads. However, it is important to note that the authors did not measure the behavior of the human; thus, it is possible that demonstrators may have inadvertently reinforced certain actions. That said, the protocol did involve control conditions, such as the ‘do it’ command being given by an individual who was unfamiliar with the demonstration, thus preventing a Clever Hans effect. However, to the authors’ knowledge, there is no documented evidence of imitation occurring naturally in dog–human relationships; thus, its relevance is questionable. Despite this, the ability of dogs to demonstrate social referencing, adapting their behavior according to human emotional signals,Citation57,Citation63 further reinforces the relevance of social learning in the dog–human relationship. Therefore, it is reasonable to postulate that dogs view humans as peers who often provide salient information about the surrounding environment but are distinct from conspecifics.

In addition to using humans as a social reference point, dogs have been shown to develop attachment bonds with humans.Citation14,Citation16 This relationship allows them to interact securely with their environment in the presence of the ownerCitation18 and show less distress in response to threatening events.Citation17 Interestingly, the secure base effect seems to operate regardless of whether the owner is encouraging or passive.Citation18 This suggests that social referencing does not always operate in the dog–human dyad. Indeed, dogs seem to struggle to distinguish between fearful and neutral emotional messages about certain objects and to respond appropriately to emotional messages given by a stranger.Citation63 As such, relational factors and attachment dimensions probably influence the degree of social referencing between dogs and owners.

The learning history of a dog may also be relevant to the attachment relationships it forms and its social referencing capabilities. For example, trained water rescue dogs have more difficulty than companion dogs in responding to the emotional messages of a stranger.Citation64 It remains unclear whether these results reflect habituation to strangers giving emotional cues in their training or the dogs’ strong attachment to their handlers. Further studies of dogs in various working and companion contexts may disambiguate the relevance of attachment and learning history in the ability of dogs to respond to human social cues.

Potential interactions between human and dog attachment patterns require clarification. Rehn et alCitation20 found no evidence of an interaction between perceived emotional closeness, assessed via the Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS), and dog attachment behaviors, assessed using the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP). In contrast, Siniscalchi et alCitation19 reported a relationship in which owner attachment (confident vs unconfident) was measured using the 9 Attachment Profile (9AP) and dog attachment was measured using the SSP. These authors reported that dogs of confident owners displayed more proximity-seeking behaviors and were more likely to interact with the owner when a stranger was present compared with dogs of owners lacking in confidence. Further studies using both the 9AP and the MDORS, in conjunction with standardized behavioral observations of both dogs and handlers, may assist in clarifying the existence of such an interaction.

Tools and methods to measure the dog–human relationship

Identification of high-risk pairings of dogs and humans offers a means of preventing dysfunction in the dog–human dyad. Additionally, outstanding dyads can provide models of dogmanship strategies that could then be applied in those dyads that tend to struggle. Therefore, a means of measuring the dog–human relationship has great potential for reducing disharmony. Although scales of this nature have been created,Citation26 there is no generally accepted tool to measure the dog–human bond. One questionnaire designed to measure the dog–human relationship was not given a name by researchers.Citation46 Accordingly, for convenience, the authors of the current review will refer to it as the Modified Person–Animal Wellness Scale (MPAWS).

Some dog–human relationship assessment tools tend to focus on the human factors of a dog–human relationship, particularly attachment. One such measure, the Dog Attachment Questionnaire (DAQ), was developed to measure human attachment to their canine companions.Citation65 Given that human factors generally have more influence on the dog–human bond than canine factors,Citation44 using measures such as the DAQ seems appropriate. However, such approaches may be overly simplistic, as attachment dimensions alone may fail to capture the influence of specific human behaviors, such as affiliation, and perceptions on the dog–human relationship. Furthermore, as we are defining the human–animal bond (HAB) as a symbiotic relationship, affective benefits to the dog, through attachment or otherwise, should be considered.

There are existing measures of the dog–human bond that consider canine factors. Schneider et alCitation66 created an internally consistent measure of the HAB that embraced human attachment and its potential to bias dog health ratings. While this measure did consider canine attachment, only one scale within it was devoted to it, while the remaining five related to human perceptions of the relationship. When testing the measure, the authors used a relatively homogenous sample. Hence, the results are not representative of the general population. Therefore, the researchers may have failed to capture some aspects of dog–human relationships. Moreover, given that the HAB has yet to be used in other studies, its overall applicability to examine dog–human relationships in general remains unclear.

The MPAWSCitation42 was developed from the Person–Animal Wellness Scale (PAWS)Citation67 and the Questionnaire for Anthropomorphic Attitudes.Citation68 This questionnaire features items concerning dog attachment and the owner–dog relationship, with four separate subscales for each of these sections. Additionally, the MPAWS asks owners about their attitude toward their dog. Significant associations have been observed within owner opinion items as well as the shared activities subscale. For example, neurotic owners reported less engagement in shared activities with their dogs.Citation46 Moreover, the MPAWS has been used in subsequent studies, identifying significant correlations between stress hormone concentrationsCitation15 and proximity-seeking behaviors.Citation48 However, aside from a principal component analysis, the authors reported no further statistical validation. Furthermore, the sample sizes for these studies were relatively small (n=22), such that any assumptions or generalizations from their results must be made with caution as they may not be applicable to dog–human dyads in general. Furthermore, many subscales had no significant relationship with dog or owner factors. Therefore, further refinement and validation of this questionnaire are required.

The MDORS has had widespread use.Citation20,Citation44,Citation69Citation71 It was also tested using an extensive, heterogeneous sample of participants, which indicates that the initial population was probably representative of dog owners in general. Unlike tools such as the MPAWS, this scale has been tested for both validity and reliability.Citation69 Despite this, it has been suggested that the MDORS focuses too much on the human member of the dog–human dyad and, thus, might overlook several aspects of the relationship that are pertinent to the emotional wellness of the dog.Citation20 A recent studyCitation44 reported that variance in MDORS scores correlated with only one dog personality subscale, as determined by Dog Mentality Assessment (DMA) results. Taken together, these results suggest that, while the MDORS is currently the most reliable measure of the dog–human relationship, it has potential to exclude canine factors. To address this, future studies should combine the MDORS with behavioral test batteries to categorize dog temperament effectively and establish its contribution to the dog–human bond.

Thus far, all tools discussed require owner reports of their behavior, the behavior of their dog, and their attitudes towards the relationship. While owner reports are arguably effective, interobserver reliability has been shown to vary depending on the particular trait being rated.Citation72,Citation73 Additionally, physical traits such as ear shape and coat color have been reported to affect ratings of dog behavior and personality.Citation74 Gosling discusses several causes for interobserver variation, such as familiarity with the animal and exposure to the species in question.Citation75 Furthermore, as previously discussed, there is ample evidence that owners may misinterpret their dog’s behavior and cognitive capacity. Taken together, this suggests that owner reports alone may not be a sufficient measure of dog–human relationships. Future studies should seek to combine owner reports with test batteries designed to measure dog–human cooperation and interaction styles. At the very least, studies using questionnaires should collect ratings from more than one individual and examine interobserver agreement.

There are several tools that assess the dog–human relationship from the perspective of dog attachment. The application of Ainsworth’s Strange Situation Test (SST), a measure originally designed to assess attachment of human infants to their mothers, has revealed several distinct attachment patterns in dogs.Citation16 This procedure has been used extensively to gauge canine attachment to humans,Citation76,Citation77 with some authors proposing, by extrapolation, that the dog’s behavior during the SST is a reflection of the bond it has developed with its owner.Citation19 Interestingly, human behavior during the SST has been shown to influence dog behavior and cortisol concentrations.Citation78 This indicates that owner behavior may bias dog behavioral observations during the SST to the extent that results may not reflect dog attachment alone. Future studies could examine how various owners differ in their behavior during the SST, such as during reunions, and how these variations affect dog behavior. Potentially, the SST may be more useful in assessing the dog–human bond than originally anticipated, if both dog and human behaviors are coded and analyzed simultaneously.

Biochemical and physiological effects of dog–human interactions

Dyadic interactions between humans and dogs can yield both mutual and individual positive effects. The dog–human relationship can be a more influential determinant of canine salivary cortisol concentrations than environmental stressors, such as a threatening stranger.Citation15 This is likely mediated by the safe haven effect or possible social referencing if the owner is not fearful of the environment. Likewise, human interaction has been demonstrated to reduce plasma cortisol concentrations in shelter dogs,Citation79 suggesting human–dog interactions may help dogs to cope with stress, almost regardless of relationship quality. Alternatively, the stressful shelter environment may have facilitated the rapid formation of an attachment bond. The specific nature of the interaction also seems to be relevant. Border guard dogs that had affiliative interactions with their handlers showed a more pronounced reduction in cortisol concentrations than police dogs subjected to authoritative interactions.Citation22 Owners kissing their dogs reportedly have higher oxytocin concentrations, as do their dogs, than owners who do not.Citation71 Oxytocin is believed to have a role in bond formation,Citation80 so frequent affiliative interactions between dogs and humans probably strengthen bond formation. This may provide a physiological explanation of why the amount of time that dogs and owners spend together is often reported to have a critical influence on both dogmanshipCitation11 and functional dog–human relationships.Citation46 These results emphasize the importance of affiliative interactions in the dog–human dyad, and their capacity to reduce stress and strengthen bond formation in both dogs and owners.

Conclusion and future directions

This review highlights growing evidence that human factors, including personality and attitudes, influence the dog–human relationship. In particular, both positive attitudes and affiliative behavior seem to contribute to a strong dog–human bond, as is apparently confirmed by hormonal changes that emerge in both dyad members. This illustrates the benefits that can ensue from successful dog–human relationships and should inspire the cultivation of such relationships. In contrast, negative attitudes, insecure attachment, and misunderstanding of dog behavior have the potential to disrupt relationships and even lead to dog relinquishment. Future studies should consider the influence of both owner attitudes and behavior on canine physiology and affective states. Such investigations may reveal a potential causal relationship between attitudes and behavior. Interestingly, although the human personality dimension of neuroticism may relate to poor dyadic functionality, it may not compromise the quality of the relationship. Assessing the personality of working dog handlers in a standardized setting may clarify whether neuroticism contributes to a given dyad’s struggle with practical tasks.

Recent studies have highlighted the importance of social and associative learning in the dog–human dyad. Indeed, given the ease with which dogs learn complex commands and behavioral sequences, training methods that exploit social learning, such as DAID, as a complement to shaping techniques may provide a means of further capitalizing on the dogmanship of handlers.

Importantly, the dog–human relationship and attachment relationships held by both humans and dogs may not be complementary. The MDORS is currently the most robust measure of the dog–human relationship, addressing primarily the human perceptions of the relationship. Future studies investigating the influence of dog temperament, measured using an internally consistent, validated scale, on the dog–human relationship may reveal how the MDORS should be refined to capture more information on canine members of the dyad. Moreover, to investigate the relationship between the dog–human bond and attachment, a measure of canine attachment, such as the SST, should also be included. The ability to produce successful dog–human dyads through the identification of factors contributing to the HAB promises to enhance the welfare of both species in this unique and ancient dyad.

Disclosure

None of the authors of this paper has a financial or personal relationship with other people or organizations that could inappropriately influence or bias the content of the paper.

References

  • ThalmannOShapiroBCuiPComplete mitochondrial genomes of ancient canids suggest a European origin of domestic dogsScience2013342616087187424233726
  • LandsbergGMHunthausenWLAckermanLJHandbook of Behavior Problems of the Dog and Cat2nd edPhiladelphia, PAElsevier Health Sciences2003
  • BradshawJWBlackwellEJCaseyRADominance in domestic dogs-useful construct or bad habit?J Vet Behav200943135144
  • KubinyiEPongráczPMiklósiADog as a model for studying conspecific and heterospecific social learningJ Vet Behav2009413141
  • ElgierAMJakovcevicAMustacaAEBentoselaMPointing following in dogs: are simple or complex cognitive mechanisms involved?Anim Cogn201215611111119
  • MiklósiATopálJCsányiVComparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us?Anim Behav20046769951004
  • GácsiMGyoöriBVirányiZExplaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skillsPLoS One200948e658419714197
  • KirchhoferKCZimmermannFKaminskiJTomaselloMDogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointingPLoS One201272e3091322347411
  • UdellMRDoreyNRWynneCDWolves outperform dogs in following human social cuesAnim Behav200876617671773
  • ReidPJAdapting to the human world: dogs’ responsiveness to our social cuesBehav Processes200980332533319056474
  • LefebvreDDiederichCDelcourtMGiffroyJMThe quality of the relation between handler and military dogs influences efficiency and welfare of dogsAppl Anim Behav Sci20071041–24960
  • KaminskiJMarshall-PesciniSThe Social Dog: Behavior and CognitionBurlington, VTElsevier Science2014
  • BowlbyJThe nature of the childs tie to his motherInt J Psychoanal195839535037313610508
  • SerpellJAEvidence for an association between pet behavior and owner attachment levelsAppl Anim Behav Sci1996471–24960
  • SchoeberlIWedlMBauerBDayJMoestlEKotrschalKEffects of owner-dog relationship and owner personality on cortisol modulation in human-dog dyadsAnthrozoos2012252199214
  • TopálJMiklósiACsányiVDókaAAttachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation TestJ Comp Psychol199811232192299770312
  • GácsiMMarosKSernkvistSFaragóTMiklósiAHuman analogue safe haven effect of the owner: behavioural and heart rate response to stressful social stimuli in dogsPLoS One201383e5847523469283
  • HornLHuberLRangeFThe importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs – evidence from a manipulative problem-solving taskPLoS One201385e6529623734243
  • SiniscalchiMStipoCQuarantaA“Like owner, like dog”: correlation between the owner’s attachment profile and the owner-dog bondPLoS One2013810e7845524205235
  • RehnTLindholmUKeelingLForkmanBI like my dog, does my dog like me?Appl Anim Behav Sci20141506573
  • DeldalleSGaunetFEffects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog-owner relationshipJ Vet Behav2014925865
  • HorvathZDokaAMiklosiAAffiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directionsHorm Behav200854110711418353328
  • McGreevyPDBoakesRACarrots and Sticks: Principles of Animal TrainingSydney, AustraliaDarlington Press2007
  • BarkerSBWolenARThe benefits of human-companion animal interaction: a reviewJ Vet Med Educ200835448749519228898
  • SchneiderAARosenbergJBakerMMeliaNGrangerBBiringenZBecoming relationally effective: high-risk boys in animal-assisted therapyHum Anim Interact Bull201421118
  • WilsonCCNettingFEThe status of instrument development in the human-animal interaction fieldAnthrozoos201225Suppl 1S11S55
  • AndersonDCAssessing the Human-Animal Bond: A Compendium of Actual MeasuresWest Lafayette, INPurdue University Press2007
  • GuyNCLuescherUADohooSEA case series of biting dogs: characteristics of the dogs, their behaviour, and their victimsAppl Anim Behav Sci20017414357
  • PatronekGJGlickmanLTBeckAMMcCabeGPEckerCRisk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelterJ Am Vet Med Assoc199620935725818755975
  • MillerRHowellGVRegulating consumption with bite: building a contemporary framework for urban dog managementJ Bus Res2008615525531
  • EllingsenKZanellaAJBjerkåsEIndrebøAThe relationship between empathy, perception of pain and attitudes toward pets among Norwegian dog ownersAnthrozoos2010233231243
  • BlouinDDAre dogs children, companions, or just animals? Understanding variations in people’s orientations toward animalsAnthrozoos2013262279294
  • SavaloisNLescureuxNBrunoisFTeaching the dog and learning from the dog: interactivity in herding dog training and useAnthrozoos20132617791
  • MirkóEDókaAMiklósiAAssociation between subjective rating and behaviour coding and the role of experience in making video assessments on the personality of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)Appl Anim Behav Sci20131491–44554
  • TamiGGallagherADescription of the behaviour of domestic dog (Canis familiaris) by experienced and inexperienced peopleAppl Anim Behav Sci20091203–4159169
  • KerswellKJButlerKLBennettPHemsworthPHThe relationships between morphological features and social signalling behaviours in juvenile dogs: the effect of early experience with dogs of different morphotypesBehav Processes20108511720472041
  • MaritiCGazzanoAMooreJLBaragliPChelliLSighieriCPerception of dogs’ stress by their ownersJ Vet Behav201274213219
  • HowellTJToukhsatiSConduitRBennettPThe Perceptions of Dog Intelligence and Cognitive Skills (PoDIaCS) SurveyJ Vet Behav201386418424
  • HorowitzADisambiguating the “guilty look”: Salient prompts to a familiar dog behaviourBehav Processes200981344745219520245
  • StarlingMJBransonNJCodyDMcGreevyPDConceptualising the impact of arousal and affective state on training outcomes of operant conditioningAnimals201332300317
  • SchwabCHuberLObey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their ownersJ Comp Psychol2006120316917516893253
  • Zilcha-ManoSMikulincerMShaverPRAn attachment perspective on human-pet relationships: conceptualization and assessment of pet attachment orientationsJ Res Pers2011454345357
  • KwanJYBainMJOwner attachment and problem behaviors related to relinquishment and training techniques of dogsJ Appl Anim Welf Sci201316216818323544756
  • MeyerIForkmanBDog and owner characteristics affecting the dog-owner relationshipJ Vet Behav201494143150
  • ArhantCBubna-LittitzHBartelsAFutschikATroxlerJBehaviour of smaller and larger dogs: effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dogAppl Anim Behav Sci20101233–4131142
  • KotrschalKSchöberlIBauerBThibeautAMWedlMDyadic relationships and operational performance of male and female owners and their male dogsBehav Processes200981338339119520238
  • KisATurcsánBGácsiMThe effect of the owner’s personality on the behaviour of owner-dog dyadsInteract Stud2012133373385
  • WedlMSchöberlIBauerBDayJKotrschalKRelational factors affecting dog social attraction to human partnersInteract Stud2010113482503
  • CavanaughLALeonardHAScammonDLA tail of two personalities: how canine companions shape relationships and well-beingJ Bus Res2008615469479
  • CurbLAAbramsonCIGriceJWKennisonSMThe relationship between personality match and pet satisfaction among dog ownersAnthrozoos2013263395404
  • de BruijnGJde GrootRvan den PutteBRhodesRConscientiousness, extraversion, and action control: comparing moderate and vigorous physical activityJ Sport Exerc Psychol200931672474220384009
  • SungSYChoiJNDo Big Five personality factors affect individual creativity? The moderating role of extrinsic motivationSoc Behav Pers2009377941956
  • SchilderMBVinkeCMvan der BorgJADominance in domestic dogs revisited: useful habit and useful construct?J Vet Behav201494184191
  • ÁkosZBeckRNagyMVicsekTKubinyiELeadership and path characteristics during walks are linked to dominance order and individual traits in dogsPLoS Comput Biol2014101e100344624465200
  • RooneyNJBradshawJWRobinsonIHA comparison of dog-dog and dog-human play behaviourAppl Anim Behav Sci2000663235248
  • TuberDSSandersSHennessyMBMillerJABehavioral and glucocorticoid responses of adult domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to companionship and social separationJ Compar Psychol19961101103108
  • GácsiMVasJTopálJMiklósiAWolves do not join the dance: sophisticated aggression control by adjusting to human social signals in dogsAppl Anim Behav Sci20131453–4109122
  • VirányiZ1GácsiMKubinyiEComprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris)Anim Cogn200811337338718183437
  • RangeFVirányiZWolves are better imitators of conspecifics than dogsPLoS One201491e8655924489744
  • RangeFVirányiZSocial learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogsFront Psychol2013486824363648
  • FreedmanAHGronauISchweizerRMGenome sequencing highlights the dynamic early history of dogsPLoS Genet2014101e100401624453982
  • FugazzaCMiklósiAShould old dog trainers learn new tricks? The efficiency of the Do as I do method and shaping/clicker training method to train dogsAppl Anim Behav Sci20141535361
  • MerolaIPrato-PrevideELazzaroniMMarshall-PesciniSDogs’ comprehension of referential emotional expressions: familiar people and familiar emotions are easierAnim Cogn201417237338523955027
  • MerolaIMarshall-PesciniSD’AnielloBPrato-PrevideESocial referencing: water rescue trained dogs are less affected than pet dogs by the stranger’s messageAppl Anim Behav Sci20131471–2132138
  • ArcherJIrelandJLThe development and factor structure of a questionnaire measure of the strength of attachment to pet dogsAnthrozoos2011243249261
  • SchneiderTRLyonsJBTetrickMAAccorttEEMultidimensional quality of life and human-animal bond measures for companion dogsJ Vet Behav201056287301
  • JohannsonEEHuman-Animal Bonding: An Investigation of Attributes [doctoral thesis]Edmonton, ABUniversity of Alberta1999
  • TopálJMiklósiACsányiVDog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dogAnthrozoos1997104214224
  • DwyerFBennettPCColemanGJDevelopment of the Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS)Anthrozoos2006193243256
  • BennettPCCooperNRohlfVIMornementKFactors influencing owner satisfaction with companion-dog-training facilitiesJ Appl Anim Welf Sci200710321724117645407
  • HandlinLNilssonAEjdebäckMHydbring-SandbergEUvnäs-MobergKAssociations between the psychological characteristics of the human-dog relationship and oxytocin and cortisol levelsAnthrozoos2012252215228
  • LeyJMBennettPCColemanGJA refinement and validation of the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ)Appl Anim Behav Sci20091162–4220227
  • RooneyNJGainesSABradshawJWPenmanSValidation of a method for assessing the ability of trainee specialist search dogsAppl Anim Behav Sci20071031–290104
  • FratkinJLBakerSCThe role of coat color and ear shape on the perception of personality in dogsAnthrozoos2013261125133
  • GoslingSDFrom mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research?Psychol Bull20011271458611271756
  • Prato-PrevideECustanceDMSpiezioCSabatiniFIs the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth’s strange situationBehaviour2003140225254
  • PalmerRCustanceDA counterbalanced version of Ainsworth’s Strange Situation Procedure reveals secure-base effects in dog-human relationshipsAppl Anim Behav Sci20081092–4306319
  • RehnTHandlinLUvnäs-MobergKKeelingLJDogs’ endocrine and behavioural responses at reunion are affected by how the human initiates contactPhysiol Behav2014124455324471179
  • ShiverdeckerMDSchimlPAHennessyMBHuman interaction moderates plasma cortisol and behavioral responses of dogs to shelter housingPhysiol Behav2013109757923234768
  • CarterCSDeVriesACTaymansSERobertsRLWilliamsJRGetzLLPeptides, Steroids, and Pair BondingAnn N Y Acad Sci19978072602729071356