256
Views
135
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Gait analysis in demented subjects: Interests and perspectives

, , , , &
Pages 155-160 | Published online: 08 Feb 2008

Abstract

Gait disorders are more prevalent in dementia than in normal aging and are related to the severity of cognitive decline. Dementia-related gait changes (DRGC) mainly include decrease in walking speed provoked by a decrease in stride length and an increase in support phase. More recently, dual-task related changes in gait were found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and non-Alzheimer dementia, even at an early stage. An increase in stride-to-stride variability while usual walking and dual-tasking has been shown to be more specific and sensitive than any change in mean value in subjects with dementia. Those data show that DRGC are not only associated to motor disorders but also to problem with central processing of information and highlight that dysfunction of temporal and frontal lobe may in part explain gait impairment among demented subjects. Gait assessment, and more particularly dual-task analysis, is therefore crucial in early diagnosis of dementia and/or related syndromes in the elderly. Moreover, dual-task disturbances could be a specific marker of falling at a pre-dementia stage.

The main clinical hallmark of dementia is cognitive decline (CitationWaldemar et al 2007). Although not as prominent, motor disorders are commonly described in later stages of dementia (CitationCamicioli et al 1998; CitationMarquis et al 2002; CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationScarmeas et al 2005; CitationWaite et al 2005) and include gait apraxia, bradykinesia, extrapyramidal rigidity, resting tremor and various gait disorders such as cautious gait or gait slowing (CitationCamicioli et al 1998; CitationGoldman et al 1999; CitationMarquis et al 2002; CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationScarmeas et al 2005; CitationWaite et al 2005; CitationWaldemar et al 2007). Recently, a few studies have shown that motor disorders, and specifically gait disorders, may be present at an early stage of dementia (CitationCamicioli et al 1998; CitationVerghese et al 2002, Citation2007a; CitationScarmeas et al 2005; CitationWaite et al 2005). There is an increased interest in the study of these dementia-related gait changes (DRGC) since they could be used to improve early diagnosis and better understand risk of falling in subjects with dementia or even at a pre-dementia stage (CitationMorgan et al 2007).

Methods

The literature was searched using Medline database. General inclusion criteria included meta-analysis, randomized controlled trial, controlled clinical trial, guideline and review articles published in English in peer-reviewed priority journals. Keywords used in PubMed were the following: Aged; AND aged, 80 over; AND Gait disorders; AND Dementia. For completeness, additional key trials known to the authors that did not meet the initial search criteria were included.

A total of 40 papers met the criteria (116 were identified by PubMed, but 76 did not correspond to the topic of the paper), 23 additional papers already known by the authors were also reviewed.

Gait changes in demented subjects: Evidence for a relationship

Dementia and gait disorders represent a major public health issue because of their high prevalence, their related adverse outcomes leading to loss of autonomy and independence, and their high costs for public health and social services (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationAllan et al 2005; CitationBurn et al 2006). It has been shown that demented older adults present with greater gait impairments than those expected as a result of the normal aging process (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationAllan et al 2005; CitationBurn et al 2006; CitationScherder et al 2007). Different DRGC have been described, mostly when comparing subjects with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) versus healthy control subjects. The walking speed decreases in AD and further parallels severity of the disease (CitationVan Iersel et al 2004, Citation2007; CitationMorgan et al 2007; CitationScherder et al 2007). This change in velocity has been related to a decrease in stride length and an increase in support time (CitationVan Iersel et al 2004). Furthermore, subjects with vascular dementia (VaD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) walked more slowly and presented a reduced step length than AD subjects (CitationAllan et al 2005; CitationMerory at al 2007). DRGC are not specific to any type of dementia as similar changes in gait pattern have been reported with advanced age (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationScherder et al 2007).

The origins of DRGC are not only associated to classic motor disorders in the basal ganglia, cerebellum and primary motor areas, but also to central misprocessing of information that is required to maintain gait safety and that include attention and executive functions (CitationGoldman et al 1999; CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationBeauchet et al 2006). Most studies exploring DRGC have focused on mean values of stride parameters (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationVan Iersel et al 2004). Recently, an increase in stride-to-stride variability while usual walking and dual-tasking has been shown to be more specific than any change in mean value in patients with dementia (CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationHausdorff 2004; CitationBeauchet et al 2005a; CitationWebster et al 2006; CitationAllali et al 2007; CitationVerghese et al 2007b). This stride-to-stride variability is a fine marker of gait control and, thus, highlights that gait should not be considered as a simple automatic motor behavior, but as a rather complex and higher level of cognitive functioning (CitationHausdorff 2004; CitationBeauchet et al 2005a; CitationAllali et al 2007). Exploring stride-to-stride variability in demented subjects represents a new way to access gait disorders related to higher levels of gait control impairment.

Early diagnosis of dementia: Gait analysis as a new method of expertise?

Whatever its etiology, treatments of dementia do not cure the disease, although its evolution may be slowed down (CitationGarcia et al 2007). Available treatments such as acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AchEIns) or memantine are efficient on cognitive symptoms and on global evolution for a limited period of time (CitationBirks 2006; CitationGarcia et al 2007), and their efficiency depends on early administration (CitationBirks 2006).

Alzheimer’s disease is the main cause of dementia (CitationFerri et al 2005; CitationRimmer et al 2005; CitationSmall et al 2007). Although operational criteria do exist, it has been shown that within subjects with AD-related cognitive decline only 50% are diagnosed with the disease, and only 30% at an early stage (CitationFerri et al 2005; CitationRimmer et al 2005). This may principally be explained by unclear boundaries between early dementia syndrome and normal aging. Neuropsychological deficits including memory, attention and executive functions are frequently affected first (CitationFerri et al 2005; CitationRimmer et al 2005; CitationSmall et al 2007), but heavily rely on psychometrics, which are time consuming and lack specificity in terms of predicting the occurrence of dementia syndrome. If defining pathological cognitive aging seems quite straightforward in the presence of dementia according to DMS-IV criteria, it becomes much more complex for intermediate cognitive impairment such as age-related cognitive decline (CitationFerri et al 2005) or mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as risk factors for AD (CitationRimmer et al 2005; CitationDi Carlo et al 2007; CitationVisser and Verhey 2007) but these are not specific to cognitive aging and hence cannot be used for early diagnosis (CitationRimmer et al 2005).

The question is then to determine whether the specificity of cognitive expertise in the early diagnosis of AD may be improved. Over the past ten years, a few studies demonstrated that cortical gait control impairment and certain motor deficits were one of the early signs of AD and non-Alzheimer dementia (CitationCamicioli et al 1998; CitationScarmeas et al 2005; CitationWaite et al 2005; CitationVerghese et al 2007a). Rhythmic motor tasks such as “finger tapping” and walking were performed more slowly in subjects developing cognitive decline than in those with no cognitive impairment whereas a one-second increase compared with the mean score of healthy subjects was associated with a risk of cognitive decline increased by a factor of 1.26 (OR = 1.26; IC = 1.01–1.60) (CitationCamicioli et al 1998). In the same study, a motor coordination impairment, ie, the capacity to alternately repeat movements of the lower limbs, was associated with a high risk of developing cognitive decline (OR = 6.10; IC = 1.40–26.30). In a longitudinal cohort of 630 community-dwelling participants aged 75 or over at recruitment, subjects with cognitive impairment associated with gait and motor slowing were the most likely to develop dementia (OR 5.6; 95% CI 2.5–12.6) (CitationWaite et al 2005). Recently, CitationVerghese and colleagues (2007a) presented similar findings in 427 subjects aged 70 and older, and concluded that quantitative gait measures might predict future risk of cognitive decline and dementia in initially non-demented older adults. These recent data suggest that an early diagnosis of dementia and/or related syndromes in the elderly should include DRGC analysis.

Gait disorders related to cortical gait control impairment: Interest in gait analysis under dual-task conditions

Walking is classically described as an automatic, rhythmic, and regular motor activity characterized by alternated, coordinated movements of crossed flexion-extension of the lower limbs while steady-state walking (CitationNutt et al 1993). It is considered as a simple motor activity in healthy subjects because of its predominant automatic character acquired during the simultaneous maturation of the locomotor and nervous systems. From a neurological viewpoint, the acquisition of such automatisms relies on motor procedural memory, allowing for the gradual appearance of motor processes enabling the automation of walking in healthy adults. Motor procedural memory handles information based on action rules (CitationBeaunieux et al 2006; CitationSquire et al 1990). Its expression is implicit, and cannot be dissociated from action.

In experimental psychology, the adaptative control of thought (ACT) by CitationAnderson and Fincham (1994) is considered as a reference. It distinguishes three successive phases during which the subject generates, combines, and improves a specific procedure for a given task. The first phase is the declarative phase, corresponding to the selection of relevant information for the requested task, and relies on intellectual abilities, as well as on important attentional resources. In the case of a motor action, it may be assimilated to the motor plan elaboration step. The second phase, called the knowledge compilation phase, corresponds to the transformation of declarative information into procedures, and may be assimilated to the elaboration of the motor program. Finally, the third and final phase is the procedural phase, corresponding to the adjustment and automation of the procedure.

The procedural learning of walking enables the automatic, unconscious triggering of underlying motor programs in healthy adults. Such an implicit character is predominant in walking and implies that walking only requires limited attentional resources in healthy subjects (CitationBeauchet et al 2005a; CitationDubost et al 2006, Citation2007). Although this assumption has been confirmed in young subjects (CitationBeauchet et al 2005b), several studies proved it was not the case in older adults and demented subjects, in which cortical gait control is involved while walking (CitationDubost et al 2006, Citation2007). Furthermore, the most ordinary walking conditions demonstrate that the implicit character of walking is not univocal: stride-to-stride time modifications, the presence of obstacles or directional changes constitute intentional parameters requiring explicit cognitive function.

The clinical test used to highlight cortical involvement in gait control is based on a dual-task paradigm, the principle of which is that the subject performs an attention-demanding task while walking and to observe any walking modification compared with the reference task, ie, usual walking (CitationAbernethy 1988; CitationWoollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002). Dual-task paradigms are based on the hypothesis that two simultaneously-performed tasks interfere if relying on identical functional and/or cerebral subsystems (CitationWoollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002). In the case of a paradigm including walking and an attention-demanding task, the interference is based on the hypothesis of a joint involvement of attention (CitationAbernethy 1988). The primary task therefore is the “attention-demanding” task, while the secondary task is represented by walking. The interferences observed are modifications of the performance in one or both tasks, which are measured by comparing the performances under single- and dual-task conditions (CitationAbernethy 1988; CitationPashler 1994; CitationWoollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002). Gait modifications are then interpreted as the involvement of attention while walking, and therefore attest of a level of cortical control ensuring the functionality of walking, while making control frailer.

Attention is a complex and multidimensional cognitive function that overlaps with executive functions and one dimension of which participate in improving the processing of information (CitationTreisman 1969). Attention is a limited resource in cognitive processing that may be overloaded by two competing tasks when performed simultaneously, or dual-tasking (CitationAbernethy 1988; CitationWoollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002), then consecutively leading to a decline of performance in one or both tasks. Two categories of interferences have been defined (CitationAbernethy 1988; CitationPashler 1994). The first one is called capacity interference and is the result of a central overload due to involvement of different information processes requiring attention. In contrast with capacity interference, structural interference is defined as a peripheral overload due to an inability to perform simultaneously two tasks involving the same category of information. The bottleneck and cross-talk models are based on this theoretical approach. Although similar tasks performed simultaneously provoke a decline in performance in the bottleneck model, the cross-talk model assumes that task similarity reduces interferences, thus leading to better performance.

Dual-task related gait changes reflect the capacity to appropriately allocate attention between two tasks performed simultaneously and, therefore, are related to executive function efficiency (CitationPashler 1994; CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationAllali et al 2007). Alzheimer’s subjects with moderate dementia and executive dysfunction presented with high gait variability suggesting that this gait parameter could be a sensitive and specific marker of frontal cortical control of walking (CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationAllali et al 2007). In another sample, the degree of efficiency of the executive function was correlated to the degree of stride time variability (CitationHausdorff et al 2005). Recently, the disruption of stride-to-stride variability was shown to be the most significant gait parameter while dual-tasking in a sample of older subjects with a dysexecutive syndrome (CitationAllali et al 2007).

Dementia and the risk of falling: Gait analysis as the key test

Dementia, even at an early stage and without any other neurological signs, is a risk factor of falling that is constantly observed, irrespective of the subject’s living environment (CitationShaw 2002). The incidence of falls is significant in demented subjects and is approximately two or three times higher than in subjects without cognitive decline (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationShaw 2002). Additionally, cognitive decline, including dementia or not, has been associated with static balance or gait disorders directly related to the severity of cognitive decline (CitationAlexander 1996; CitationAllan et al 2005; CitationBurn et al 2006). When comparing subjects with mild to moderate AD to healthy subjects that were matched for age and sensory deficits, the performance of unipodal support or the tandem stance was associated to the severity of cognitive deficits (CitationFrassen et al 1999). Additionally, when testing the coordination of lower limbs in the sitting position while carrying out a test of the « Tapping » type, the performance observed during this test also decreased according to the degree of cognitive impairment. Depending on how severe cognitive decline is, associated neurological anomalies such as extrapyramidal syndrome have been observed, which explains the exponential increase in disorders and the incidence of falls (CitationShaw 2002).

The most frequently identified explanatory factor in demented subjects is postural instability, irrespective of the nature of dementia (CitationVisser 1983; CitationNakamura 1996; CitationO’Keeffe 1996; CitationBuchner et al 1997; CitationFrassen et al 1999). Subjects with VaD or DLB obviously are at an increased risk of falling compared to subjects with AD (CitationVisser 1983; CitationSattin 1992; CitationClemson et al 1996; CitationNakamura 1996; CitationO’Keeffe 1996; CitationBuchner et al 1997). The role of environment in the mechanism of falls in older demented subjects remains controversial, just as in older subjects without dementia. Few studies have reported an association between falls and the environment, while others were unable to demonstrate such relation (CitationSattin 1992; CitationClemson et al 1996; CitationCarter et al 1997; CitationClose et al 1999).

It has been shown that gait characteristics while usual walking could be related to an executive function deficit. Reduced walking speed was related to poor performances in the Trail Making Test A and B, a neuropsychological test evaluating executive function, in a group of 926 community-living older subjects, under obstacle avoidance conditions, that is considered as an attention-demanding task (CitationBle et al 2005). Previous studies were also able to establish such type of relation between executive dysfunction and poor performances in physical tests (CitationSheridan et al 2003; CitationHausdorff et al 2005; CitationAllali et al 2007), and explain how interesting it may be to use dual-task paradigms during gait analysis in subjects with dementia. Additionally, in a population of 325 community-living older subjects, a poor performance in the Trail Making Test part B (ie, with practice time over 180 seconds) was associated with a risk of severe trauma in the event of a fall that was 1.9 superior compared with subjects performing the Trail Making Test part B in less than 180 seconds (CitationNevitt et al 1991).

DRGC usually lead to falls and associated risk factors were recently reviewed from the most to the least important one (CitationOliver et al 2004): clinically abnormal walking, a history of falling and cognitive decline. Fall-related gait disorders associated with dementia may be prevented since several studies showed that prevention programs had resulted in a 20 to 25% reduction in falling rates (CitationTinetti et al 1998; CitationOliver et al 2003; CitationTinetti 2003). In addition, criteria have been established in order to better identify subjects at risk of falling (CitationOliver et al 2003). Consequently, an early identification of potential fallers through gait analysis is an indispensable first step of any efficient attempt to prevent falls in subjects with dementia.

The neuronal basis for dementia-related changes in gait

The automatic, implicit character is predominant in walking. It essentially relies on sub-cortical and spinal control, with a neuronal network localized in the lumbar area (CitationNutt et al 1993; CitationDietz 2003). These spinal neuronal networks that include motor neurons and inter neurons are called “central pattern generator” (CPG)for locomotion (CitationForssberg and Grillner 1973; CitationForssberg 1979; CitationDietz 2003). They allow the generation of automatic and rhythmic motor activity patterns, corresponding to alternate, coordinated movements of crossed flexion-extension of the upper limbs, even in the absence of sensory afferences or control by supraspinal structures (CitationDietz 2003). In humans, several clinical observations corroborate the existence of CPG, such as spontaneous locomotor activity in infants, or persistent rhythmic, coordinated movements of the lower limbs in paraplegic subjects after a medullar section in the dorsal region (CitationForssberg and Grillner 1973; CitationForssberg 1979). At the supramedullary level, a particularly important locomotor zone is the pediculopontine nucleus (PPN), localized at the mesencephalic level that is considered to be a site of termination for basal ganglia output, and hence probably plays a key role in the modulation of walking (CitationPahapill and Lozano 2000).

At the cortical level, there is increased evidence that dysfunction of temporal and frontal lobe may explain motor impairment among demented subjects. It has been shown that temporal atrophy, and specifically the hippocampus, is not only related to memory but also to motor dysfunction (CitationScherder et al 2007). Furthermore, both clinical studies based on dual-tasking and brain imaging studies have shown that frontal lobe dysfunction may be related to gait disorders in demented subjects. CitationNakamura and colleagues (1997) demonstrated that increased oscillations of the body in a static position, a reduced stride length and increased stride variability in subjects with AD could be related to a decreased cerebral perfusion in the frontal lobe.

Recommendations for future research

Visual observation is usually used for gait analysis by the clinicians but do not give sufficient measures of gait. Objective and accurate gait analysis of temporal and spatial characteristics by validated devices may provide useful information to improve early diagnosis and better understand risk of falling among subjects with cognitive decline. In particular, increase of stride time variability (ie, stride-to-stride variation of gait cycle duration) while dual-tasking has been specifically associated with impaired executive function (IEF) in demented older adults. Acquiring more information about dual-task related changes in stride time variability among demented subjects with and without IEF could add to our understanding of IEF-related gait instability.

Conclusion

Gait disorders and dementia are frequently associated. There is increase evidence that DRGC are not only attributable to motor disorders, but also associated to problems with the cortical processing of information what may be present at the early stage of dementia. Dysfunction of temporal and frontal lobe may in part explain gait disorders among demented subjects. The main clinical implications are that cortical related changes in gait characteristics could be used to improve the early diagnosis of dementia and detection of an increased risk of falling in demented subjects.

References

  • AbernethyBDual-task methodology and motor skills reseasch: somme applications and methodologies contraintsJ Hum Mov Studies19881410132
  • AlexanderNBGait disorders in older adultsJ Am Geriatr Soc199644434518636592
  • AllaliGKressigRWAssalFChanges in gait while backward counting in demented older adults with frontal lobe dysfunctionGait Posture200726572617344044
  • AllanLMBallardCGBurnDJPrevalence and severity of gait disorders in Alzheimer’s and non-Alzheimer’s dementiasJ Am Geriatr Soc2005531681716181166
  • AndersonJRFinchamJMAcquisition of procedural skills from examplesJ Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn1994201322407983466
  • BeauchetOBerrutGGait and dual-task: definition, interest, and perspectives in the elderlyPsychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil200642152516945851
  • BeauchetODubostVAminianKDual-task-related gait changes in the elderly: does the type of cognitive task matter?J Mot Behav2005a372596415967751
  • BeauchetODubostVHerrmannFRStride-to-stride variability while backward counting among healthy young adultsJ Neuroengineering Rehabil2005b226
  • BeaunieuxHHubertVWitkowskiTWhich processes are involved in cognitive procedural learning?Memory2006145213916754239
  • BleAVolpatoSZulianiGExecutive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI studyJ Am Geriatr Soc2005534101515743282
  • BirksJCholinesterase inhibitors for Alzheimer’s diseaseCochrane Database Syst Rev20061CD00559316437532
  • BuchnerDMCressMEDe LateurBJThe effect of strength and endurance training on gait, balance, fall risk, and health services use in community-living older adultsJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci199752M218249224433
  • BurnDJRowanENAllanLMMotor subtype and cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease, Parkinson’s disease with dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodiesJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry200677585916614017
  • CamicioliRHowiesonDOkenBMotor slowing precedes cognitive impairment in the oldest oldNeurology199850149689596020
  • CarterSECampbellEMSanson-FisherRWEnvironmental hazards in the homes of older peopleAge Ageing1997261952029223715
  • ClemsonLCummingRGRolandMCase-control study of hazards in the home and risk of falls and hip fracturesAge Ageing199625971018670535
  • CloseJEllisMHooperRPrevention of falls in the elderly trial (PROFET): a randomised controlled trialLancet199935393710023893
  • Di CarloALamassaMBaldereschiMCIND and MCI in the Italian elderly: frequency, vascular risk factors progression to dementiaNeurology20076819091617536047
  • DietzVSpinal cord pattern generators for locomotionClin Neurophysiol200311413798912888019
  • DubostVAnnweilerCAminianKStride-to-stride variability while enumerating animal names among healthy young adults: Result of stride velocity or effect of attention-demanding task?Gait Posture2007in press
  • DubostVKressigRWGonthierRRelationships between dual-task related changes in stride velocity and stride time variability in healthy older adultsHum Mov Sci2006253728216714067
  • FerriCPPrinceMBrayneCAlzheimer’s Disease International. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus studyLancet200536621121716360788
  • ForssbergHOn integrative motor functions in the cat’s spinal cordActa Physiol Scand Suppl1979474156401437
  • ForssbergHGrillnerSThe locomotion of the acute spinal cat injected with clonidine i.vBrain Res5018464690545
  • FranssenEHSourenLETorossianCLEquilibrium and limb coordination in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s diseaseJ Am Geriatr Soc199947463910203123
  • GarciaAThompsonKZanibbiKCholinesterase inhibitors and Alzheimer’s disease outcomesJ Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci20076257017522364
  • GoldmanWPBatyJDBucklesVDMotor dysfunction in mildly demented AD individuals without extrapyramidal signsNeurology1999539566210496252
  • HausdorffJMStride variability: beyond length and frequencyGait Posture20042030415531178
  • HausdorffJMYogevGSpringerSWalking is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a complex cognitive taskExp Brain Res2005164541815864565
  • MarquisSMooreMMHowiesonDBIndependent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly personsArch Neurol200259601611939895
  • MeroryJRWittwerJERoweCCQuantitative gait analysis in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s diseaseGait Posture2007264141917161601
  • MorganDFunkMCrossleyMThe potential of gait analysis to contribute to differential diagnosis of early stage dementia: current research and future directionsCan J Aging200726193217430801
  • NuttJGMarsdenCDThompsonPDHuman walking and higher-level gait disorders, particularly in the elderlyNeurology199343268798437689
  • NakamuraTMeguroKSasakiHRelationship between falls and stride length variability in senile dementia of the Alzheimer typeGerontology199642108139138973
  • NakamuraTMeguroKYamazakiHPostural and gait disturbance correlated with decreased frontal cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer diseaseAlzheimer Dis Assoc Disord19971113299305498
  • NevittMCCummingsSRHudesESRisk factors for injurious falls: a prospective studyJ Gerontol199146M164701890282
  • OliverDHopperASeedPDo hospital fall prevention programs work? A systematic reviewJ Am Geriatr Soc20034816798911129762
  • OliverDDalyFMartinFCRisk factors and risk assessment tools for falls in hospital in-patients: a systematic reviewAge Ageing2004331223014960426
  • O’KeeffeSTKazeemHPhilpottRMGait disturbance in Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical studyAge Ageing199625313168831878
  • PahapillPALozanoAMThe pedunculopontine nucleus and Parkinson’s diseaseBrain200012317678310960043
  • PashlerHDual-task interference in simple task: Data and theoryPsychol Bull1994116220447972591
  • RimmerEWojciechowskaMStaveCImplications of the Facing Dementia Survey for the general population, patients and caregivers across EuropeInt J Clin Pract Suppl2005146172415801187
  • SattinRWFalls among older persons: a public health perspectiveAnnu Rev Public Health1992134895081599600
  • ScarmeasNAlbertMBrandtJMotor signs predict poor outcomes in Alzheimer diseaseNeurology200564169670315911793
  • ScherderEEggermontLSwaabDGait in ageing and associated dementias; its relationship with cognitionNeurosci Biobehav Rev2007314859717306372
  • ShawFEFalls in cognitive impairment and dementiaClin Geriatr Med2002181597312180241
  • SheridanPLSolomontJKowallNInfluence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s diseaseJ Am Geriatr Soc200351633714687395
  • SmallBJGagnonERobinsonBEarly identification of cognitive deficits: preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairmentGeriatrics200762192317408315
  • SquireLRZola-MorganSCaveCBMemory: organization of brain systems and cognitionCold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol1990551007232132810
  • TinettiMESpeechleyMGinterSFRisk factors for falls among elderly persons living in the communityN Engl J Med1988319170173205267
  • TinettiMEClinical practice. Preventing falls in elderly personsN Engl J Med200334842912510042
  • TreismanAMStrategies and models of selective attentionPsychol Rev196976282994893203
  • Van IerselMBBenraadCERikkertMGValidity and reliability of quantitative gait analysis in geriatric patients with and without dementiaJ Am Geriatr Soc200755632417397453
  • Van IerselMBHoefslootWMunnekeMSystematic review of quantitative clinical gait analysis in patients with dementiaZ Gerontol Geriatr200437273214991293
  • VergheseJLiptonRBHallCBAbnormality of gait as a predictor of non-Alzheimer’s dementiaN Engl J Med20023471761812456852
  • VergheseJDerbyCKatzMJHigh risk neurological gait syndrome and vascular dementiaJ Neural Transm2007a11412495217541695
  • VergheseJWangCHoltzerRQuantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementiaJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry2007b789293517237140
  • VisserHGait and balance in senile dementia of Alzheimer’s typeAge Ageing1983122963016660138
  • VisserPJVerheyFRMild cognitive impairment as predictor for Alzheimer’s disease in clinical practice: effect of age and diagnostic criteriaPsychol Med200724110
  • WaldemarGDuboisBEmreMEFNSRecommendations for the diagnosis and management of Alzheimer’s disease and other disorders associated with dementia: EFNS guidelineEur J Neurol200714e12617222085
  • WaiteLMGraysonDAPiguetOGait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from the Sydney Older Persons StudyJ Neurol Sci2005229-230899315760625
  • WebsterKEMeroryJRWittwerJEGait variability in community dwelling adults with Alzheimer diseaseAlzheimer Dis Assoc Disord200620374016493234
  • WoollacottMShumway-CookAAttention and the control of posture and gait: A review of an emerging area of researchGait Posture20021611412127181