88
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Early rising children are more active than late risers

Pages 959-963 | Published online: 15 Jan 2008

Abstract

Background

A low level of physical activity impacts mental as well as physical health. This study investigated the daily lifestyle habits that affect physical activity in young children.

Methods

The relationship between physical activity, assessed by means of a Mini-Mitter Actiwatch device, and observed daily lifestyle habits was analyzed for 204 children, aged 12 to 40 months (average: 22.6 months), for whom 6-consecutive-day data from both the Actiwatch and sleep log were obtained.

Results

An older age, male gender, and early waking time showed significant positive correlations with physical activity level. Multiple regression analysis revealed that these three variables were significant predictors of physical activity.

Conclusion

Promoting an early rising time is suggested to be an important element of cultivating good health in young children.

A British cohort study of more than 30 years’ duration (CitationViner and Hotopf 2004) has shown that sedentary behavior during childhood also increases the risk of chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis. In contrast, physical activity (PA) decreases the risk of obesity, which is an important risk factor for cardiovascular and metabolic disorders (CitationHaslam and James 2005). Moreover, PA is reported to enhance brain health in general (CitationCotman and Berchtold 2002). Exercise-derived benefits to brain function have been demonstrated at the molecular level (CitationBerchtold et al 2005), and PA has been reported to decrease the risk for Alzheimer’s disease (CitationFriedland et al 2001; CitationRovio et al 2005). PA is one of the key behavioral activities for promoting healthy brain function in animals, including humans. Thus, identification of the behavioral factors in childhood that increase the performance of PA is important for the promotion of both mental and physical health.

In 1979, 8.1% of children attending day nurseries in Japan were reported as yawning frequently in the morning, and 10.5% as becoming easily tired (CitationAbe 2005). In 2000, these numbers increased remarkably, to 53.2% and 76.6%, respectively (CitationAbe 2005). In accordance with these changes, the reported disturbance of basic daily life habits has progressively been increasing in young children in Japan; the rate of 3-year-old children who went to bed later than 10:00 pm was 22% in 1980, and 52% in 2000 (CitationKawai 2001). I also reported that the rate of late sleepers whose sleep onset time was 10:00 pm or later was 43.0% among 307 18-month-old infants living in Tokyo, and 53.7% among 151 36-month-old infants living in Tokyo, respectively (CitationKohyama et al 2000). Moreover, I found 49.6% of 3-year-old children living in a suburb of Tokyo fell asleep at 10:00 pm or later (CitationKohyama et al 2002). Based on these figures, I hypothesized that the number of inactive children is increasing in Japan, and that the habits of basic daily life influence the activity level of these children. The present study was a preparatory one, aiming to establish the effects of daily life habits on PA in young children.

Methods

Between July 2002 and November 2006, 285 children aged 6–49 months were studied after obtaining written informed consent from their guardians. All of the children were recruited through the Chuo Honcho Public Health Center in the Adachi District of Tokyo, Japan.

Daily PA was assessed using the Mini-Mitter Actiwatch (model AWLP-AM) (AW), a small device (37 × 29 × 11 mm) consisting of an omnidirectional accelerometer sensitive to 0.01 gravity (0.098 m/s2). The information on acceleration is altered into an electrical current that varies in magnitude, and is then stored as activity counts (CitationMinimitter Company Incorporated 1999). The maximum sampling frequency is 32 Hz (CitationMinimitter Company Incorporated 1999; CitationLopez-Alarcon et al 2004). The daily average activity counts per minute from waking in the morning until falling asleep in the evening [PA (counts/min/day)] were assessed.

According to previous studies (CitationNishihara et al 2002; CitationLopez-Alarcon et al 2004; CitationSulemana et al 2006), the AW device was placed on the ankle of each child for 7 consecutive days, during which period the guardians recorded sleep logs for the children. Times when the AW was removed were also recorded. They were also asked to describe the child’s behavior in as much detail as possible during this period. No other special instruction was made for guardians and their children, and they were asked to live their ordinary daily lives. Activity counts and time duration in minutes during the period when the AW was removed (eg, during bathing) as well as the period when nap was taken were omitted from the calculated PA. Times of sleep onset and waking, including napping, were confirmed by matching the records in the sleep logs based on the observation of guardians with the AW data (CitationBenson et al 2004). Since guardians were asked to record the time when their child fell asleep but not the time when the children went to bed, sleep latency could not be measured in the present study.

PA, nap duration on the day when PA was calculated, waking time on the morning of the day, sleep duration of the previous night, and sleep onset time of the previous night were examined. The average values for these measures were calculated for each child, and these average values were used for calculating correlation coefficients. The fluctuation bands for sleep onset and waking times were calculated using the difference between the earliest and latest bedtimes within the recorded period. Correlation coefficients between PA and gender (male: 1, female: 2), age in months, sleep onset time of the previous night, sleep duration of the previous night, waking time in the morning, nap duration during the day, and the bands for sleep onset and waking times were assessed. Correlation coefficients of age in months with sleep onset time of the previous day, sleep duration of the previous night, waking time in the morning, and nap duration during the day were also calculated. Multiple linear regression analysis was used with PA as a dependent variable. The corresponding set of independent variables included age in months, gender, and daily life habits. p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

A complete 7-day data set from both the AW and sleep log were obtained for only 22 children. Fifty-nine children yielded data for <5 days. Consequently, 6-consecutive-day data from both the AW and sleep log were obtained from 204 children (103 male and 101 female). The data obtained from these 204 children were used for analysis. Their ages ranged from 12 to 40 months (mean 22.6 ± 7.3 months).

In the sleep logs, most guardians described behaviors of their children in detail. According to these records, I judged that these children lived their ordinary daily lives even during the study period with AW on their ankle.

The average sleep onset time was 21:24 (range: 19:03–24:08, standard deviation [SD]: 49.2 minutes) (). In 42 children (20.6%), the average sleep onset time was 22:00 or later. The average night sleep duration was 9.9 hours (range: 7.8–14.0, SD: 0.8), waking time was 7:19 (range: 5:05–10:29, SD: 49.8 minutes), nap duration was 1.7 hours (range: 0–3.3, SD: 0.6), the fluctuation band for the sleep onset time was 1.7 hours (range: 0–6.5, SD: 1.2) and that for the waking time was 1.5 hours (range: 0.2–7.0, SD: 0.9), respectively ().

Table 1 Average values for daily life habits

Among the correlation coefficients calculated (), significant positive correlations were obtained between older age and PA, as well as sleep onset time and waking time. Significant negative correlations were obtained between waking time and PA, and between age and nap duration. According to the correlation coefficient between PA and gender, males were found to increase PA significantly. PA also increased significantly with an older age and early waking time. Using 7:30 am as a cut-off time, and comparing PA, the average PA of children who woke up earlier than 7:30 (442.1, SD: 95.7, n = 124) was significantly higher than that of children who woke up at 7:30 am or later (402.9, SD: 96.9, n = 80) (p < 0.005).

Table 2 Correlation coefficients for the obtained data

Based on multiple linear regression analysis, a significantly predictable regression formula was obtained for PA. Significant regression coefficients with respect to PA was obtained for gender (p = 0.006), waking time (p = 0.008), and months of age (p = 0.010).

Discussion

In this study, daily PA was assessed by means of AW, which has been reported as a valid and useful device for the assessment of PA in children (CitationPuyau et al 2002). In adults, AW is worn on the wrist, but it is also worn on the belt or on the ankle in children and adolescents. Although it is hard to say that ankle activity is fully correlated with overall systematic muscular activity, several papers reported that PA measured by AW on the ankle could be an indicator of PA in infants (CitationNishihara et al 2002), children (CitationLopez-Alarcon et al 2004), and in adolescents (CitationSulemana et al 2006). According to these reports, I placed AW on the ankle, though its adequacy needs to be discussed. None of the children included in this study were too young to walk.

This study determined times of sleep onset and waking including napping by matching the records in the sleep logs with the AW data. CitationAcebo and colleagues (2005) reported the overall similarities between actigraph sleep measures and mother-reported measures, although actigraph-based nocturnal wake minutes were higher than maternal diary reports. Since the present study neglected nocturnal waking, an adequacy of determined data was considered as proper.

This study reveals PA to be significantly correlated with older age and male gender. These results are consistent with our experience that the activity of a child increases with age, and that male children are generally more active than female children. In addition, the present study shows a significant negative correlation between age and nap duration. This finding is also in accord with previous reports (CitationWeissbluth 1995).

In this study, nocturnal sleep and nap duration did not exhibit a significant correlation with PA. If one presumes that adequate rest is indispensable for increasing PA, then children who sleep properly might have better PA. However, our findings did not support this assumption. The required sleep duration for an individual is very difficult to determine because the need for sleep is variable (CitationCarskadon and Dement 2005). One explanation may be that individual requirements for sleep vary, and thus there is no single specific sleep amount that is predictive of PA.

In the preliminary study (Yokomaku and Kohyama 2006), children who had smaller bands for sleep onset and waking times showed lower (= better) scores for some subscales of child behavior check list than those who had larger bands, and the distribution of clinical classification were significantly better for children with smaller band for sleep onset than those who had a larger one. However, in the current study, fluctuation bands for sleep onset and waking times did not affect PA. Effects of regularity of daily life habits on the child’s health remain to be discussed.

The current study does, however, show PA to be significantly correlated with earlier waking time, and late risers were found to be more inactive than early risers. PA has so far been discussed as a key behavioral activity for promoting healthy brain function. However, patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder are known to be pathologically overactive (CitationSimms 2004). In addition, these patients are also widely reported to be poor sleepers (CitationO’Brien et al 2003), and sleep disorders including obstructive sleep apnea syndrome frequently result in behavioral symptoms such as hyperactivity (CitationSimms 2004). Early risers who are sleep-deprived could exhibit pathological daytime over-activity, resulting in the increase of PA. It is hard to rule out this possibility, since the present device for calculating PA does not discriminate between normal and pathological activity. However, with the increasing numbers of children who go to bed late at night and wake up late in the morning (CitationKawai 2001), there is considerable concern among nursery staffs that the number of children who are exhausted or inactive is increasing (CitationSuzuki et al 2002). A currently obtained result that late risers were more inactive than early risers might be a clue to help these inactive children. But why were late risers more inactive than early risers? One possible explanation could be the involvement of morning light.

Morning light is known to be a powerful cue for shortening the circadian cycle of the biological clock, which is naturally longer than 24 h in most humans (CitationWever 1979). Consequently, morning light synchronizes the circadian cycle of the biological clock with the 24 h cycle of the earth (CitationMinors et al 1991). Without this time cue, our daily rhythms are apt to act abnormally causing tiredness, impaired alertness and performance, disorientation, gastrointestinal problems, loss of appetite, and inappropriate timing of defecation (CitationArendt et al 2005). In addition, light exposure in the morning has been reported to be effective for patients with depression (CitationGoel et al 2003; CitationBaghai et al 2006; CitationJorm et al 2006) and seasonal affective disorder (CitationTerman et al 1989; CitationTerman and Terman 2005; CitationMichalak et al 2007). Lack of exposure to morning light must produce an unfavorable condition for these patients, and might also be so for young late risers in Japan.

However, the current study did not measure the duration and intensity of light exposure for each child. This is a central issue which remains to be determined to test the current hypothesis on the role of morning light. This pilot study has another weakness, which is that no socio-economic or socio-educational data on the families were obtained. These factors could affect both basic daily life habits and PA, and should be assessed in future studies.

The average sleep onset and waking times obtained in the current study, 21:24 and 7:19, respectively, are earlier than those in the previous study from 2002: 21:44 and 7:48, respectively (CitationKohyama et al 2002). In addition, the rate of children who fell asleep at 22:00 or later was reduced in the current study than in our former studies (CitationKohyama et al 2000; Citation2002). A recent nationwide social promotion program to encourage children waking up early in the morning and going to bed early in the night, supported by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, might have contributed to this differing result. However, the current study also showed a positive significant correlation between age and both sleep onset and waking times. The social promotion of the favorable daily life habits on physical and mental health should be continued.

Acknowledgments

I thank the staff of Chuo Honcho Public Health Center in the Adachi District of Tokyo for their generous cooperation.

References

  • AbeSKodomoNihonMamorukaiWo2005Differences in physical status of children (in Japanese)Kodomo Hakusho. TokyoSoudo Bunka10810
  • AceboCSadehASeiferR2005Sleep/wake patterns derived from activity monitoring and maternal report for healthy 1- to 5-year-old childrenSleep2815687716408417
  • ArendtJStoneBSkeneDJKrygerMHRothTDementWC2005Sleep disruption in jet lag and other circadian rhythm-related disordersPrinciples and practice of sleep medicine4th edElsevier Saunders65972
  • BaghaiTCMollerHJRupprechtR2006Recent progress in pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment options of major depressionCurr Pharm Des125031516472142
  • BensonKFriedmanLNodaA2004The measurement of sleep by actigraphy: Direct comparison of 2 commercially available actigraphs in a nonclinical populationSleep27986915453559
  • BerchtoldNCChinnGChouM2005Exercise primes a molecular memory for brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein in the rat hippocampusNeuroscience1338536115896913
  • CarskadonMADementWCKrygerMHRothTDementWC2005Normal human sleep: An overviewPrinciples and practice of sleep medicine4th edElsevier Saunders1323
  • CotmanCWBerchtoldNC2002Exercise: A behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticityTrends Neurosci2529530112086747
  • FriedlandRPFritschTSmythKA2001Patients with Alzheimer’s disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group membersProc Natl Acad Sci USA983440511248097
  • GoelNTermanJSMacchiMM2003A placebo-controlled trial of light and negative ion treatment for chronic depression: Preliminary resultsChronobiol Int2012079
  • HaslamDWJamesWP2005ObesityLancet366119720916198769
  • JormAFAllenNBO’DonnellCP2006Effectiveness of complementary and self-help treatments for depression in children and adolescentsMed J Aust1853687217014404
  • KawaiH2001Investigation on the health of young children in 2000 (in Japanese)J Child Health6054387
  • KohyamaJShiikiTHasegawaT2000Sleep duration of young children is affected by nocturnal sleep onset timePediatr Int425899111059559
  • KohyamaJShiikiTOhinata-SugimotoJ2002Potentially harmful sleep habits of 3-year-old children in JapanJ Dev Behav Pediatr23677011943967
  • Lopez-AlarconMMerrifieldJFieldsDA2004Ability of the acti-watch accelerometer to predict free-living energy expenditure in young childrenObesity Res12185965
  • MichalakEEMurrayGWilkinsonC2007A pilot study of adherence with light treatment for seasonal affective disorderPsychiatry Res1493152017161464
  • Minimitter Company Incorporated1999Actiwatch 16/Actiwatch 64/Actiwatch-L Activity Monitors: Instruction ManualBend (Oregon, USA)Minimitter Company Corporated
  • MinorsDSWaterhouseJMWirz-JusticeA1991A human phase-response curve to lightNeurosci Lett13336401791996
  • NishiharaKHoriuchiSEtoHUchidaS2002The development of infants’ circadian rest-activity rhythm and mothers’ rhythmPhysiol Behav7791812213506
  • O’BrienLMIvanenkoACrabtreeVM2003Sleep disturbances in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorderPediatr Res542374312736394
  • PuyauMRAdolphALVohraFA2002Validation and calibration of physical activity monitors in childrenObesity Res101507
  • RovioSKareholtIHelkalaEL2005Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s diseaseLancet Neurol4690116239172
  • SimmsMDBehrmanREKliegmanRMJensonHB2004Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorderNelson textbook of pediatrics17th ed Saunders10710
  • SulemanaHSmolenskyMHLaiD2006Relationship between physical activity and body mass index in adolescentsMed Sci Sports Exerc381182616775561
  • SuzukiMTakahashiCNomuraY2002What are care workers concerned about regarding the relationship between young children and their parents? – On inquiring about sleep-wake rhythm of children and their parents’ attitude towards raising them (in Japanese)J Child Health615938
  • TermanMTermanJSKrygerMHRothTDementWC2005Light therapyPrinciples and practice of sleep medicine4th ed ElsevierSaunders142442
  • TermanMTermanJSQuitkinFM1989Bright light therapy for winter depression: A review of efficacyNeuropsychopharmachology2122
  • VinerRHotopfM2004Childhood predictors of self reported chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis in adults: National birth cohort studyBMJ329941315469945
  • WeverRA1979The circadian system of man: Results of experiments under temporal isolationNew YorkSpringer-Verlag
  • WeissbluthM1995Naps in children: 6 months-7 yearsSleep188277792496
  • YokomakuAKohyamaJEffect of sleeping habits on child behaviorNo To Hattatsu38S153

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.