68
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research

Functional MRI evidence for language plasticity in adult epileptic patients: Preliminary results

, , , &
Pages 235-246 | Published online: 08 Feb 2008

Abstract

The present fMRI study explores the cerebral reorganisation of language in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, according to the age of seizures onset (early or late) and the hippocampal sclerosis (associated or not). Seven right-handed control volunteers and seven preoperative adult epileptic patients performed a rhyme decision (language condition) and a visual detection (control condition) tasks in visually presented words and unreadable characters, respectively. All patients were left hemisphere dominant for language. Appropriate statistical analyses provided the following preliminary results: (1) patients compared with healthy subjects showed lower degree of hemispheric lateralization with supplementary involvement of the right hemisphere; (2) the degree of hemispheric specialization depends on the considered region; (3) patients with early seizures show signs of temporal and parietal reorganization more frequently than patients with late onset of seizures; (4) patients with early seizures show a tendency for intra-hemispheric frontal reorganisation; (5) associated hippocampal sclerosis facilitates the inter-hemispheric shift of temporal activation. Although our patients were left hemisphere predominant for language, the statistical analyses indicated that the degree of lateralization was significantly lower than in healthy subjects. This result has been considered as the indication of atypical lateralization of language.

Introduction

The term “focal epilepsy” is classically used for describing seizures generated by a specific cerebral region, the epileptogenic zone (CitationRosenow and Lüders 2001; CitationLüders et al 2006). The majority of focal epilepsies in adults are temporal lobe epilepsies (TLE) (CitationThomas and Genton 1992; CitationEngel 2001), meaning that the region responsible for seizures is situated in the temporal lobe. With respect to healthy subjects, the patients with chronic temporal epileptogenic show reorganization of temporal language networks (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationBreier et al 2005; CitationPataraia et al 2004, Citation2005). As the left temporal lobe is involved in language (CitationBillingsley et al 2001; CitationSeghier et al 2004; CitationGitelman et al 2005), an atypical representation (bilateral hemispheric representation or right hemispheric predominance) of the subjacent cerebral regions, could occur (CitationAdcock et al 2003; CitationDemonet et al 2005). Overall, when the epileptogenic zone is located within the predominant hemisphere for language, two types of cerebral reorganization have been generally described: (a) intra-hemispheric, ie, the nonaffected cerebral regions from the hemisphere where the epileptogenic zone is located in, take over language functions, for instance, the language function of the temporal region could “shift” within the frontal region of the same hemisphere, when the epileptogenic zone is situated in the temporal lobe (CitationBell et al 2002; CitationBrazdil et al 2003; CitationPataraia et al 2004, Citation2005; CitationKadis et al 2007) and (b) inter-hemispheric, ie, the homologues cerebral regions situated within the nonpredominant hemisphere for language become involved in language (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationPataraia et al 2004, Citation2005; CitationBreier et al 2005; CitationThivard et al 2005; CitationHelmstaedter et al 2006). The type of reorganization may depend on many factors such as the age of seizures onset (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationBerl et al 2005; CitationDemonet et al 2005; CitationGoldmann and Golby 2005; CitationThivard et al 2005), the considered language region, temporo-parietal or frontal (CitationKurthen et al 1992; CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationThivard et al 2005) or the associated hippocampal sclerosis (CitationKnecht 2004; CitationJanszky et al 2006; CitationWeber et al 2006).

The factor which has been often studied within this framework, was the age of seizures onset (CitationMuller et al 1999a; CitationBell et al 2002; CitationBrazdil et al 2003; CitationPataraia et al 2004). The onset of seizures before age of six (CitationGoldmann and Golby 2005; CitationYuan et al 2006) and originated in the left predominant hemisphere could induce either inter-hemispheric (bilateral or right hemispheric) (CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationMuller et al 1999a, Citation1999b; CitationVoets et al 2006) or intra-hemispheric (CitationBell et al 2002; CitationPataraia et al 2004) reorganization of language. Overall, it is generally accepted that the right shift of language tends to be stronger and concerns more numerous language regions in patients with early seizures, as compared to patients with late (after age of six) lesions (CitationMuller et al 1999a). The intra-hemispheric reorganization means that language operations depend on regions which are not originally dedicated to these operations. Thus, the incidence of the intra-hemispheric reorganization is significantly higher if seizures start early in life (CitationBell et al 2002; CitationPataraia et al 2004).

Epileptics with associated hippocampal sclerosis are more likely to show atypical reorganisation of language than patients without sclerosis (CitationJanszky et al 2003; CitationKnecht 2004; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationWeber et al 2006; CitationMaccotta et al 2007). It has been suggested that hippocampal pathology influences the intra- and the inter-hemispheric reorganization of language (CitationKnecht 2004). By using fMRI, it has been shown that patients with left hippocampal sclerosis show high involvement of the right medial temporal lobe during synonym judgement (CitationWeber et al 2006) or living categorization (CitationRichardson et al 2003). During performing a language task, epileptics with hippocampal sclerosis show evidence for supplementary ipsilateral recruitement of nontemporal areas such as frontal regions (CitationMaccotta et al 2007). These studies suggest inter-hemispheric temporal as well as intra-hemispheric reorganisation of language when the hippocampal sclerosis is associated.

Also, the pattern of language plasticity depends on the language region which is considered. Within this framework, CitationThivard and colleagues (2005) have shown that the inter-hemispheric shift of language from the left to the right hemisphere is more likely to occur for temporal than for frontal regions in patients with TLE. Indeed, they showed that temporal lateralization indices indicate supplementary involvement of the right hemisphere, than frontal lateralization indices. These findings are in agreement with results provided by CitationBillingsley and colleagues (2001) showing larger inter-hemispheric shift of the activation for temporal than for frontal regions. These results suggest that language plasticity should be investigated at regional rather than at global hemispheric level.

The aim of this fMRI study was to assess the possible reorganization of language regions in epileptic patients with focal temporal lobe epilepsy when they are compared to healthy participants. The language reorganisation was explored at regional (frontal, temporal, parietal) level and the influence of two factors, the age of seizures onset and the associated hippocampal sclerosis, has been explored. All subjects performed a rhyme detection task. This task induces robust lateralized activation within the predominant hemisphere for language (CitationLurito et al 2000; CitationBaciu et al 2001; CitationBillingsley et al 2001; CitationSeghier et al 2004; CitationBaciu, Watson et al 2005; CitationCousin et al 2006) and particularly along the left perisylvian (frontal, temporal, parietal) regions (CitationPugh et al 1996; CitationBaciu, Juphard et al 2005) essential for language (CitationOjemann et al 1989). Thus, by using this task we could explore the possible reorganisation at global hemispheric level as well as at regional level.

Our hypotheses were the following:

  • At hemispheric level, the difference between left and right hemisphere activity should be greater in healthy participants than in epileptic patients due to significant involvement of the right hemisphere,

  • At regional level, the reorganisation would significantly concern temporo-parietal than frontal regions, as the epileptogenic zone is temporal,

  • The TLE associated with hippocampal sclerosis would induce higher degree of atypical temporal lateralization than TLE without hippocampal sclerosis.

Material and methods

Participants

The explored population included seven healthy right-handed (CitationOldfield 1971), native French speaker volunteers (3 women and 4 men, mean of age 28.28 years old) with no history of neurological disorders and seven preoperative adult patients (see for demographic data) with medically intractable TLE. Among them, 6/7 had left TLE, 4/6 had ipsilateral hippocampal sclerosis, 4/7 had early seizures (before age of six; mean of age 3.5) and 3/7 had late seizures (after age of six; mean of age 12.3). Although 4/7 patients were right-handed and 3/7 were left-handed, they were considered together in the analyses. Each patient included in this study was diagnosed in the neurology department and the type of seizures was determined based on the clinical features, video-EEG and stereo-EEG. None patient underwent surgery at the time of the present study. The temporal location of the epileptogenic zone was determined by VEEG and by using SEEG (5/2 patients). All patients had “functional” epileptogenic zone, meaning that no tumours or scars was detected to MRI, although some of them had hippocampal sclerosis. In all patients the left hemisphere was predominant for language (as determined by VEEG and SEEG and confirmed by fMRI examination, see ). This study has been reviewed by the appropriate ethics committee and all participants gave their informed consent.

Table 1 Shows demographic data for the epileptic patients examined in this study. For each of them the following information is mentioned

Stimuli

The stimuli consisted of couples of words presented visually one above the other in the middle of the screen. The words were written either in a readable (Times) or in an unreadable (Karalyn Patterson) font. Overall, 58 couples of readable and 58 couples of unreadable words were presented per scan. The stimuli were generated by means of Psyscope V.1.1 (Carnegie Mellon Department of Psychology) on a Macintosh computer (Power Macintosh 9600). They were transmitted into the magnet by means of a video projector (Eiki LC 6000), a projection screen situated behind the magnet and a mirror centred above the patient’s eyes. The duration of each stimulus (couple of readable and unreadable words) was 3 sec. Examples of stimuli presented during the experiment are shown in .

Figure 1 Examples of stimuli presented during fMRI paradigm, respectively “Task” (pairs of readable words) and “Control” (pairs of unreadable words written in Karalyn Patterson font) conditions. Concerning the “Task”, patients and controls were instructed to judge whether the words rhymed (rhyme detection task, “task epoch”). Concerning the “Control”, patients and healthy controls were instructed to judge whether the words contained at least one character which overshot the others.

Figure 1 Examples of stimuli presented during fMRI paradigm, respectively “Task” (pairs of readable words) and “Control” (pairs of unreadable words written in Karalyn Patterson font) conditions. Concerning the “Task”, patients and controls were instructed to judge whether the words rhymed (rhyme detection task, “task epoch”). Concerning the “Control”, patients and healthy controls were instructed to judge whether the words contained at least one character which overshot the others.

Tasks

Patients and control subjects were instructed to judge whether the words rhymed (rhyme detection task, “task epoch”). With respect to “words” in unreadable font, patients and controls were instructed to judge if “words” contained at least one character which overshot the others (visual detection task, “control epoch”). The “yes” and “no” responses for the two epochs were transmitted by means of two keys pressed with the index and the middle finger of the dominant hand, respectively.

Paradigm and MR acquisition

A “block” paradigm was used during the fMRI examination, consisting of five periods, each composed of a “control” and a “task” epochs. Each epoch lasted 48 seconds. Each patient underwent a single fMRI scan. Before entering into the magnet, the subjects were positioned in front of a computer screen, and carefully instructed and trained to execute the tasks by using stimuli which were different from those used during the experiment.

Functional MR imaging was performed on a 1.5 Tesla MR imager (Philips NT) equipped with echo-planar (EPI) acquisition. Twenty five adjacent, axial slices (thickness 5 mm each) were imaged sequentially. The imaging volume was oriented parallel to the bicommissural (AC-PC) plane. It was measured sixteen times during each epoch. Positioning of the image planes was performed on scout images acquired in the sagittal plane. An EPI MR Pulse sequence was used. The major MR acquisition parameters of this sequence were: TR = 3000 ms, TE = 45 ms, flip angle = 90°, acquisition time per slice = 37 ms, field-of-view = 256 × 256 mm2, imaging matrix = 64 × 64, reconstruction matrix = 128 × 128. Subsequent to the functional scan, a high resolution 3D anatomical MR scan was obtained from the volume examined functionally.

Data processing

Data analysis was performed by using the SPM2 software (CitationFriston et al 1995) and based on the general linear model (boxcar function convolved with the hemodynamic response). The MR images were spatially pre-processed as follows. First, motion correction was applied and all images were realigned by means of a rigid body transformation with respect to the first acquired image. Subsequently, all images (patients and controls) were normalized, first the anatomical and then, the functional volumes. In order to avoid left-right morphological asymmetries that could bias the inter-hemispheric comparison, a symmetrical template was built by averaging the standard MNI template and its mirror with respect to the mid-sagittal plane. This average template was then used during for normalization of all participants including patients, as they did not have anatomical brain deformation, confirming by MRI. To conform to the underlying assumption in SPM that the data are normally distributed, the functional images were spatially smoothed by an 8 mm FWHM (Full Width at Half Maximum) Gaussian kernel. Time-series for each voxel were high-pass filtered (1/128 Hz cutoff) to remove low-frequency noise and signal drift.

The general linear model was then used to generate parameter estimates of activity at each voxel, for each condition, and each participant. Statistical parametric maps were generated from linear contrasts between the HRF parameter estimates for the two experimental conditions. At the individual level, we assessed the whole network of cerebral areas involved for each contrast (rhyme vs control; control vs rhyme). Subsequently, we performed in healthy participants a random-effect group analysis on the contrast images from individual analyses (CitationFriston et al 1998), using one-sample t tests. Clusters of activated voxels were then identified as significant at a voxel level of p < 0.001, T > 4.50, extended threshold of 15 voxels. The group analysis allowed identifying language regions significantly more activated within one hemisphere with respect to the other one. The brain regions were reported according to the stereotaxic atlas of CitationTalairach and Tournoux (1988) and the activation’s peak (x, y, z coordinates) of regions of interest (ROI) have been identified. The considered ROIs were the regions classically activated by previous studies for this task for healthy participants (CitationDemonet et al 1992; CitationLurito et al 2000; CitationBaciu et al 2001; CitationSeghier et al 2004; CitationBaciu, Juphard et al 2005). They were the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 44, 45), the inferior and middle temporal regions (IMTG, BA 37, 21) and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL, BA 39, 40). Subsequently, the peak of activation was used for drawing individual ROI in each participant (healthy controls and patients) by using the Marsbar tool (see http://marsbar.sourceforge.net). Each ROI was delineated as a sphere of 20 mm radius (CitationLiegeois et al 2004) centred on the peak (x, y, z) and symmetrically within hemispheres (one ROI to the left and the symmetrical to the right for each participant). Then, the parameter estimates (% of signal intensity variation for task and control conditions) were extracted from all left and right ROIs, in each participant.

Statistical analysis of the parameter estimates in patients

The parameter estimates values for all left ROIs and all right ROIs of individual participants were submitted to a three way ANOVA analysis (by using STATISTICA 5.0 software) with activated regions (IFG, IMTG, IPL) and hemispheres (Right and Left) as within-subjects factors, and pathological condition (Controls and Patients) as between-subject factor. We checked first if patients were significantly differently lateralized than healthy subjects. If significant difference in lateralization between the groups has been detected, we subsequently tested for each ROI if the factors of interest (age of seizures onset and associated hippocampal sclerosis) influenced plasticity. Their influence was checked for the intra- and inter-hemispheric reorganization of language. The parameter estimates for each left and the homologue right ROI were correlated with the two variables (age of onset and associated hippocampal sclerosis). Because of the small sample size, a nonparametric procedure was used (Spearman correlations). The critical significance threshold of correlations was considered at p < 0.05. Significant correlations suggested that these factors have an effect on the cerebral reorganization of language in epileptic patients.

Results

Healthy participants

Activated regions (ROI) during rhyming task

Brain areas involved in “task” (rhyme) condition relative to “control” (visual detection) and in “control” relative to “task” are summarized in . During rhyme, the activated regions of interest were exclusively located to the left: inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 44, x y z, −40 17 25), inferior parietal lobule (supramarginal gyrus, SMG, BA 40, x y z, −44 −41 35) and infero-medial temporal gyrus (IMTG, BA 37/21, x y z, −44 −63 −7). During visual detection, the activated regions were exclusively located to the right: middle frontal gyrus (BA 9, x y z, 51 10 36) and middle occipital gyrus (BA 19, x y z, 44 −40 −15).

Table 2 Cerebral activated regions obtained for “Task” versus “Control” (T > C) and for “Control” versus “Task” (C > T). The statistical significance threshold for individual voxels was set at p < 0.001 uncorrected (random-effect analysis). The Talairach peak coordinates (x, y, z) are indicated for each cluster

Epileptic patients

MR signal intensity variation (parameter estimates, magnitude) in left and right ROI in individual patients

The parameter estimates were extracted from the ROI identified in healthy subjects: IFG, IPL (SMG), and IMTG. Then, these values were subsequently analysed by using ANOVA analyses. First, when all participants (Controls and Patients) and all regions (IFG, IPL, IMTG) were considered, we obtained significantly greater activation of the left (F 1, 12 = 5.295, p < 0.05) than of the right hemisphere. Furthermore, ANOVA revealed an interaction () between hemispheres and pathological condition; (F1, 12 = 7.60, p < 0.01) with greater inter-hemispheric difference (ie, left hemispheric dominance) for healthy than for epileptic patients. This result suggests that right hemisphere is supplementary involved in patients than in controls during language, although patients keep left hemispheric predominance.

Figure 2 Significant interaction between hemisphere (Left and Right) and pathological condition (Control and Patient). The y-axis shows the degree of hemispheric lateralization (magnitude difference between conditions in left and right ROI for patients and controls (x-axis). It shows significantly left-right difference for controls with respect to patients.

Figure 2 Significant interaction between hemisphere (Left and Right) and pathological condition (Control and Patient). The y-axis shows the degree of hemispheric lateralization (magnitude difference between conditions in left and right ROI for patients and controls (x-axis). It shows significantly left-right difference for controls with respect to patients.

The effect of seizures onset and hippocampal sclerosis on plasticity

This effect was assessed for each individual left ROI and right ROI by performing correlations between the parameter estimates extracted from these regions and the two variables (age of seizures onset and associated hippocampal sclerosis). By using the WFUpickatlas (CitationMaldjian et al 2003, Citation2004) we illustrate in - only cerebral regions suggesting reorganization (“plastic” regions) with respect to control subjects, according to the age of seizure onset (IFG, IMTG, IPL) and the associated hippocampal sclerosis (IMTG). In the following section we describe each specific region.

short-legendFigure 3

Inferomedial temporal gyrus (IMTG, BA 37/21)

We obtained significant correlations between the parameter estimates extracted from the left IMTG and the age of seizures onset (rs = 0.83; p < 0.01), as well as between the parameters estimates extracted from the right IMTG and the age of seizures onset (rs = 0.89; p < 0.006). This result suggests that patients presenting seizures early in life have more bilateral activation of the IMTG (). Interestingly, we also obtained significant correlation between the parameters estimates extracted from the right IMTG and the associated left hippocampal sclerosis (rs = 0.77; p < 0.05). This result suggests that patients with associated hippocampal sclerosis are more likely to involve their homologue IMTG during language ().

Inferior parietal lobule (IPL, SMG, BA 40)

Significant correlation was obtained between the parameters estimate extracted from the right IPL and the age of seizures onset (rs = 0.89; p < 0.006). This result suggests that early age of seizures onset could induce supplementary activation of the right homologue parietal region in patients ().

Figure 4 Shows the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) activation in TLE patients according to the age of seizures onset. The Panel A shows bilateral SMG activation in patients with seizures starting before age of 6 years old (OC, SDi, PD, CG from ). The Panel B shows left SMG activation in patients with seizures starting after age of 6 years old (MR, FD, SD from ). The activation is projected onto 2D anatomical slices (left is right).

Figure 4 Shows the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) activation in TLE patients according to the age of seizures onset. The Panel A shows bilateral SMG activation in patients with seizures starting before age of 6 years old (OC, SDi, PD, CG from Table 1). The Panel B shows left SMG activation in patients with seizures starting after age of 6 years old (MR, FD, SD from Table 1). The activation is projected onto 2D anatomical slices (left is right).

Figure 5 Shows the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) activation in TLE patients according to the age of seizures onset. The Panel A shows left IFG activation in patients with seizures starting before age of 6 years old (OC, SDi, PD, CG from ). The Panel B shows left IFG activation in patients with seizures starting after age of 6 years old (MR, FD, SD from ). The first sub-group had significantly higher activation of the left frontal region than the second sub-group. The activation is projected onto 2D anatomical slices (left is right).

Figure 5 Shows the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) activation in TLE patients according to the age of seizures onset. The Panel A shows left IFG activation in patients with seizures starting before age of 6 years old (OC, SDi, PD, CG from Table 1). The Panel B shows left IFG activation in patients with seizures starting after age of 6 years old (MR, FD, SD from Table 1). The first sub-group had significantly higher activation of the left frontal region than the second sub-group. The activation is projected onto 2D anatomical slices (left is right).

No significant correlation has been obtained between the parameter estimates extracted from this region and the presence/absence of hippocampal sclerosis.

Inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 44)

The correlation between the parameter estimates extracted from the left IFG and the age of seizures onset failed to reach significance (rs = 0.71; p = 0.06) suggesting only a tendency of the intra-hemispheric reorganisation for this region when seizures start early in life ().

No significant correlation was observed between the parameter estimates extracted from the IFG and the presence/absence of hippocampal sclerosis.

Our patient population was not homogeneous with respect to the handedness: 4/7 of patients were right-handed and 3/7 were left-handed. This sample was too small for splitting patients in right and left-handed. Although the handedness was confounded because we did not explore the relationship between handedness and cerebral reorganisation, we checked the possible effect of this factor. On this purpose, Spearman correlations between handedness and left and right homologues ROI have been calculated. None significant effect was obtained (0.43 < rs < 0.14; 0.33 < p < 0.75) suggesting that the handedness did not significantly affect the cerebral reorganisation in our sample.

Discussion

This fMRI study explored the influence of two variables (age of seizures onset and associated hippocampal sclerosis) on the cerebral reorganisation of language in seven adult epileptic patients with respect to seven healthy subjects, while they performed a rhyme detection task. We first identified language ROI based on a random-effect group analysis for delineating their own left and symmetrical right ROI. From each ROI we extracted the parameters estimates (magnitude or % of MR signal variation) for each condition (“task” and “control”). We included these values in ANOVAs for detecting significant differences between patients and controls within these ROI. In patients, we also estimated (Spearman correlations) the effect of the two variables (age of seizures onset and hippocampal sclerosis) on the reorganization of language.

Regions of interest during rhyme detection

This task presupposes predominant grapho-phonemic conversion (phonological analysis) of words in order to detect rhyming but also automatic semantic analysis when a word is presented (CitationBaciu et al 2001; CitationSeghier et al 2004). That is why this task was used in the present study for assessing regions related to “language” (both, phonological and semantic aspects of language). The task induced activation of regions predominantly located in the left hemisphere, confirming results obtained by previous studies (CitationDemonet et al 1992; CitationBinder et al 1996, Citation1997; CitationLurito et al 2000; CitationPrice 2000). We obtained (a) left frontal activation (BA 44, x y z, −40 17 25) responsible for output phonology (CitationDemonet et al 1992), planning and execution of language (ie, articulation) and post-lexical processing (CitationGitelman et al 2005), (b) left temporal activation (BA 37/21, x y z, −44 −63 −7) related to semantic associations (CitationLurito et al 2000; CitationBillingsley et al 2001; CitationGitelman et al 2005; CitationSchaefer et al 2006) and (c) left parietal activation (BA 40, x y z, –44 −41 35) related to short term memory phonological storage (CitationGitelman et al 2005). We briefly remind that visual detection (“control”) induced very limited activation within the right hemisphere, related to visual attentional processes particularly involved for performing this task (CitationCorbetta et al 1998).

Hemispheric lateralization of language in patients with respect to control subjects

Our results show that patients have significantly reduced left hemispheric lateralization for language than right-handed healthy participants who are classically strongly left lateralized (CitationFiez 2001; CitationSeghier et al 2004; CitationBaciu, Juphard et al 2005). The reduced left hemisphere predominance suggests a tendency for bilateralization with supplementary involvement of the right hemisphere (CitationSpringer et al 1999; CitationAdcock et al 2003). Other studies have previously shown change of the hemispheric specialization degree or pattern in patients (CitationPataraia et al 2004, Citation2005; CitationBrazdil et al 2005; CitationThivard et al 2005; CitationYuan et al 2006) with respect to healthy subjects. However, in these studies, the assessment of the left-right hemispheric difference was not statistically robust as they generally used lateralization indices calculation. This method is known as being threshold-dependent and based on the number of activated voxels within hemispheres (CitationLiegeois et al 2002; CitationBaciu, Juphard et al 2005). In the present study, we brought robust evidence suggesting differences in the lateralization pattern in patients and in controls, by using robust statistical analyses on the MR signal intensity variation (parameter estimates) extracted from ROIs.

The effect of seizures onset and associated hippocampal sclerosis on language reorganisation

As suggested by other studies (CitationLehericy et al 2002; CitationRies et al 2004; CitationThivard et al 2005), the language reorganization should be explored at regional rather than at hemispheric level, because the reorganisation pattern depend on the region taken into account. Furthermore, the reorganisation of temporal regions occurs more frequently than the reorganisation of frontal regions. Several factors can influence the reorganisation pattern. In this study we explored two of them:

Age of seizures onset

The age of seizures influences cerebral plasticity (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationBrazdil et al 2003; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationThivard et al 2005). Althought it is difficult to distinguish between neuropathological changes and normal language development (CitationYuan et al 2006), it is largely accepted that the age is the primary factor for predicting recovery with prognosis so far better in children under age of six. Thus, under the age of six, the language plasticity is reflected by supplementary involvement of the homologue hemisphere and decrease of the degree of specialization (CitationMuller et al 1999a; CitationBrazdil et al 2003; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationDemonet et al 2005; CitationPataraia et al 2005; CitationThivard et al 2005; CitationHelmstaedter et al 2006; CitationVoets et al 2006; CitationYuan et al 2006). Is is also reflected by intra-hemispheric reorganization of cerebral networks (CitationLazar et al 1997; CitationBell et al 2002; CitationPataraia et al 2004), meaning that regions not originally involved in a specific language operation, become involved after reorganization. In the present study, the age of seizures onset was significantly correlated with the activity of each ROI taken into account. Next, we describe how the cerebral plasticity could occur in each of the “plastic” regions.

Frontal region

Our results suggest only a tendency of an intra-hemispheric reorganization of frontal activation with respect to healthy subjects. This tendency suggests that early seizures induce greater activation/involvement of frontal region than seizures starting late in life. This result is in agreement with CitationHertz-Pannier and colleagues’ (2002) study. Indeed, they showed that language reorganization occurs in pre-existing bilateral networks which could not be detected preoperatively but postoperatively. This pattern, also called “translocation of language function” (CitationLazar et al 1997), is interpreted as an over-activity of frontal region with probably taking into account the function of other language regions, such as the posterior (temporo-parietal) ipsilateral areas (CitationLehericy et al 2002). This pattern could also reflect lower involvement of temporal regions during language, due to their pathological condition (CitationRies et al 2004).

Parietal region

Our results indicate an inter-hemispheric reorganization for this region, correlated with the age of seizure onset: higher probability of reorganisation when patients had seizures early in life. These results are in agreement with previous studies which explored the temporo-parietal reorganization for language either by collapsing the two regions (CitationThivard et al 2005; CitationHelmstaedter et al 2006) or by considering them separately (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002).

Temporal region

Significant correlation obtained between the signal intensity variation for both, right and left IMTG and the age of seizures onset, suggests that early seizures induced bilateral representation of language for temporal regions. Furthermore, significant correlation of the left IMTG and the age of seizures onset, suggests that the injured hemisphere is still able to deal with language functions (CitationDeVos et al 1995), as the epileptogenic zone is located in the majority of our patients in the left temporal lobe. The correlation obtained between age and the right IMTG is consistent with previous studies showing similar result when the left temporal region is damaged (CitationHertz-Pannier et al 2002; CitationBrazdil et al 2003; CitationSabbah et al 2003; CitationLiegeois et al 2004; CitationThivard et al 2005; CitationJanszky et al 2006).

All together, these results suggest a possible segregation within the left temporal lobe in patients with TLE with seizures starting early in life. Some functional temporal sub-regions would not be able anymore to take in charge specific language operations. By consequent, their role could be assumed either by other intra-hemispheric language regions (such as frontal; intra-hemispheric “shift”) or homologue language regions (right temporal; inter-hemispheric “shift”). Some other functional temporal sub-regions could be over-activated by taking in charge the language functions of those dysfunctional temporal sub-regions.

Associated hippocampal sclerosis

Hippocampal sclerosis seems playing an important role on the cerebral plasticity for language. In our study, this factor was significantly correlated with the right IMTG activity. Several studies showed right lateralization of the temporal lobe activation in TLE with hippocampal sclerosis (CitationGrunwald et al 1999; CitationRichardson et al 2003; CitationSchaefer et al 2006; CitationWeber et al 2006). The right “shift” was interpreted either as a compensatory mechanism for verbal memory processes depending on the left damaged hippocampal structures (medial temporal regions) and/or as a compensatory mechanism for language as hippocampal structures could be involved in language lateralization development (CitationKnecht 2004).

Our population of epileptic patients was not homogenous for handedness. The hand preference should be taken into account (CitationPujol et al 1999; CitationKnecht et al 2000) when studying the language plasticity as linear relationship has been shown between handedness and atypical dominance, with increased incidence of the reorganisation in strong left-handed patients. In the present study, at a first glance, our results did not show correlation between handedness and MR signal intensity values measured in left and right ROIs. This result should be considered very cautiously as the patients sample was very small and the control subjects were not matched with patients for handedness. The influence of the hand preference on language reorganisation will be clearly explored in a greater population of epileptic patients and compared to appropriate and matched left and right-handed healthy subjects in future studies.

Conclusion

With respect to healthy subjects, the patients with focal TLE explored in the current study show significantly reduced hemispheric lateralization during performing a language task. The significant correlations obtained between the age of seizures onset and the magnitude of temporo-parietal regions suggest supplementary involvement of the right hemisphere (CitationThivard et al 2005). The tendentious correlation obtained between seizures onset and the magnitude in left frontal region could suggests a possible intra-hemispheric reorganisation of this region, but supplementary examinations should be performed in order to obtain robust significant results. These preliminary results show that cerebral reorganisation could be statistically assessed as a function of different regions and depending on several variables. The sample of patients was small and the results should be considered carefully at the individual level. However, this is encouraging evidence for using neuroimaging methods to study plasticity in further research.

References

  • AdcockJEWiseRGOxburyJMQuantitative fMRI assessment of the differences in lateralization of language-related brain activation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsyNeuroimage20031824233812595196
  • BaciuMJuphardACousinEEvaluating fMRI methods for assessing hemispheric language dominance in healthy subjectsEuropean Journal of Radiology20055522091816036149
  • BaciuMKahanePMinottiLFunctional MRI assessment of the hemispheric predominance for language in epileptic patients using a simple rhyme detection taskEpileptic Disorder20013311724
  • BaciuMWatsonJMMaccottaLEvaluating functional MRI procedures for assessing hemispheric language dominance in neurosurgical patientsNeuroradiology200547118354416142480
  • BellBHermannBSeidenbergMIpsilateral Reorganization of Language in Early-Onset Left Temporal Lobe EpilepsyEpilepsy Behav20023215816412609417
  • BerlMMBalsamoLMXuBSeizure focus affects regional language networks assessed by fMRINeurology2005651016041116301489
  • BillingsleyRLMcAndrewsMPCrawleyAPFunctional MRI of phonological and semantic processing in temporal lobe epilepsyBrain2001124612182711353737
  • BinderJRFrostJAHammekeTAHuman brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imagingJ Neurosci1997171353628987760
  • BinderJRSwansonSJHammekeTADetermination of language dominance using functional MRI: a comparison with the Wada testNeurology1996464978848780076
  • BrazdilMChlebusPMiklMReorganization of language-related neuronal networks in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy – an fMRI studyEur J Neurol20051242687515804243
  • BrazdilMZakopcanJKubaRAtypical hemispheric language dominance in left temporal lobe epilepsy as a result of the reorganization of language functionsEpilepsy Behav200344414912899862
  • BreierJICastilloEMSimosPGAtypical language representation in patients with chronic seizure disorder and achievement deficits with magnetoencephalographyEpilepsia2005464540815816948
  • CorbettaMAkbudakEConturoTEA common network of functional areas for attention and eye movementsNeuron1998214761739808463
  • CousinEPeyrinCBaciuMHemispheric predominance assessment of phonology and semantics: a divided visual field experimentBrain Cogn200661329830416600448
  • DemonetJFCholletFRamsaySThe anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjectsBrain199211561753681486459
  • DemonetJFThierryGCardebatDRenewal of the neurophysiology of language: functional neuroimagingPhysiol Rev2005851499515618478
  • DeVosKJWyllieEGecklerCLanguage dominance in patients with early childhood tumors near left hemisphere language areasNeurology1995452349567854538
  • EngelJJrMesial temporal lobe epilepsy: what have we learned?Neuroscientist2001743405211488399
  • FiezJANeuroimaging studies of speech an overview of techniques and methodological approachesJ Commun Disord200134644545411725857
  • FristonKJFletcherPJosephsOEvent-related fMRI: characterizing differential responsesNeuroimage19987130409500830
  • FristonKJFrithCDFrackowiakRSCharacterizing dynamic brain responses with fMRI: a multivariate approachNeuroimage199522166729343599
  • GitelmanDRNobreACSontySLanguage network specializations: an analysis with parallel task designs and functional magnetic resonance imagingNeuroimage20052649758515893473
  • GoldmannREGolbyAJAtypical language representation in epilepsy: implications for injury-induced reorganization of brain functionEpilepsy Behav2005644738715878308
  • GrunwaldTBeckHLehnertzKLimbic P300s in temporal lobe epilepsy with and without Ammon’s horn sclerosisEur J Neurosci1999116189990610336658
  • HelmstaedterCFritzNEGonzalez PerezPAShift-back of right into left hemisphere language dominance after control of epileptic seizures: evidence for epilepsy driven functional cerebral organizationEpilepsy Res2006702–32576216624525
  • Hertz-PannierLChironCJambaqueILate plasticity for language in a child’s non-dominant hemisphere: a pre- and post-surgery fMRI studyBrain2002125Pt 23617211844736
  • JanszkyJJokeitHHeinemannDEpileptic activity influences the speech organization in medial temporal lobe epilepsyBrain2003126Pt 920435112821521
  • JanszkyJMertensMJanszkyILeft-sided interictal epileptic activity induces shift of language lateralization in temporal lobe epilepsy: an fMRI studyEpilepsia2006475921716686658
  • KadisDSIidaKKerrENIntrahemispheric reorganization of language in children with medically intractable epilepsy of the left hemisphereJ Int Neuropsychol Soc20071335051617445300
  • KnechtSDoes language lateralization depend on the hippocampus?Brain2004127Pt 61217815166085
  • KnechtSDragerBDeppeMHandedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humansBrain2000123122512811099452
  • KurthenMLinkeDBElgerCELinguistic perseveration in dominant-side intracarotid amobarbital testsCortex1992282209191499307
  • LazarRMMarshallRSPile-SpellmanJAnterior translocation of language in patients with left cerebral arteriovenous malformationNeurology199749380289305344
  • LehericySBiondiASourourNArteriovenous brain malformations: is functional MR imaging reliable for studying language reorganization in patients? Initial observationsRadiology200222336728212034934
  • LiegeoisFConnellyACrossJHLanguage reorganization in children with early-onset lesions of the left hemisphere: an fMRI studyBrain2004127Pt 612293615069021
  • LiegeoisFConnellyASalmondCHA direct test for lateralization of language activation using fMRI: comparison with invasive assessments in children with epilepsyNeuroimage20021741861712498760
  • LüdersHONajmINairDThe epileptogenic zone: general principlesEpileptic Disord20068Suppl 219
  • LuritoJTKarekenDALoweMJComparison of rhyming and word generation with FMRIHum Brain Mapp20001039910610912589
  • MaccottaLBucknerRLGilliamFGChanging frontal contributions to memory before and after medial temporal lobectomyCereb Cortex20071724435616547345
  • MaldjianJLaurientiPBurdetteJKraft RAAn Automated Method for Neuroanatomic and Cytoarchitectonic Atlas-based Interrogation of fMRI Data SetsNeuroImage2003191233912880848
  • MaldjianJLaurientiPBurdetteJPrecentral Gyrus Discrepancy in Electronic Versions of the Talairach AtlasNeuroimage2004211450514741682
  • MullerRABehenMERothermelRDBrain organization for language in children adolescents and adults with left hemisphere lesion: a PET studyProg Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry1999b2346576810390724
  • MullerRARothermelRDBehenMELanguage organization in patients with early and late left-hemisphere lesion: a PET studyNeuropsychologia1999a3755455710340314
  • OjemannGOjemannJLettichECortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere. An electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patientsJ Neurosurg1989713316262769383
  • OldfieldRCThe assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventoryNeuropsychologia197191971135146491
  • PataraiaEBillingsley-MarshallRLCastilloEMOrganization of receptive language-specific cortex before and after left temporal lobectomyNeurology2005643481715699379
  • PataraiaESimosPGCastilloEMReorganization of language-specific cortex in patients with lesions or mesial temporal epilepsyNeurology2004631018253215557497
  • PriceCJThe anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimagingJ Anat2000197Pt 3335911117622
  • PughKRShaywitzBAShaywitzSECerebral organization of component processes in readingBrain199611941221388813285
  • PujolJDeusJLosillaJMCerebral lateralization of language in normal left-handed people studied by functional MRINeurology199952510384310102425
  • RichardsonMPStrangeBADuncanJSPreserved verbal memory function in left medial temporal pathology involves reorganisation of function to right medial temporal lobeNeuroimage200320Suppl 1S112914597304
  • RiesMLBoopFAGriebelMLFunctional MRI and Wada determination of language lateralization: a case of crossed dominanceEpilepsia200445185914692913
  • RosenowFLüdersHPresurgical evaluation of epilepsyBrain2001124Pt 9168370011522572
  • SabbahPChassouxFLevequeCFunctional MR imaging in assessment of language dominance in epileptic patientsNeuroimage20031824606712595199
  • SchaeferMHeinzeHJRotteMVerbal memory encoding in patients with left-sided hippocampal sclerosisNeuroreport2006171112192316837858
  • SeghierMLLazeyrasFPegnaAJVariability of fMRI activation during a phonological and semantic language task in healthy subjectsHum Brain Mapp20042331405515449358
  • SpringerJABinderJRHammekeTALanguage dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: a functional MRI studyBrain19991221120334610545389
  • TalairachJTournouxPCo-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain.3-dimensional proportional system: An approach to cerebral imaging1988New YorkThieme
  • ThivardLHombrouckJdu MontcelSTProductive and perceptive language reorganization in temporal lobe epilepsyNeuroimage20052438415115652319
  • ThomasPGentonPEpilepsies1992ParisMasson
  • VoetsNLAdcockJEFlitneyDEDistinct right frontal lobe activation in language processing following left hemisphere injuryBrain2006129Pt 37546616280351
  • WeberBWellmerJReuberMLeft hippocampal pathology is associated with atypical language lateralization in patients with focal epilepsyBrain2006129Pt 23465116330504
  • YuanWSzaflarskiJPSchmithorstVJfMRI shows atypical language lateralization in pediatric epilepsy patientsEpilepsia200647359360016529628