225
Views
21
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review

Clinical treatment options for patients with homonymous visual field defects

, &
Pages 93-102 | Published online: 07 Mar 2008

Abstract

The objective of this review is to evaluate treatments for homonymous visual field defects (HVFDs). We distinguish between three treatments: visual restoration training (VRT), optical aids, and compensatory training. VRT is both the most ambitious and controversial approach, aiming to restore portions of the lost visual field. While early studies suggested that VRT can reduce the visual field defect, recent studies using more reliable means of monitoring the patients’ fixation could not confirm this effect. Studies utilizing modern optical aids have reported some promising results, but the extent to which these aids can reliably reduce the patients’ visual disability has yet to be confirmed. Compensatory approaches, which teach patients more effective ways of using their eyes, are currently the only form of treatment for which behavioral improvements have been demonstrated. However, with the exception of one study using a reading training, placebo-controlled clinical evaluation studies are lacking. It is also not yet clear whether the training benefits found in laboratory tasks lead to reliable improvements in activities of daily living and which of the various forms of compensatory training is the most promising. It is therefore too early to recommend any of the currently available treatment approaches.

Introduction

The visual field is the entire space in which visual stimuli can be perceived when the eyes are fixating. If someone has a visual field defect this means that they have lost the ability to see visual stimuli which are presented in one part of the visual field. With homonymous visual field defects (HVFDs), the same part of the visual field is affected for both the right and left eye. Approximately 20% of people with acquired brain injury may develop a HVFD (CitationKasten et al 1999), which occurs following damage to the neural visual pathway, specifically damage posterior to the optic chiasm. Homonymous hemianopia (see ), in which one half of the visual field is blind, occurs in approximately 75% of cases (CitationZihl 1995a).

Figure 1 Binocular visual field plot representing a left-sided hemianopia.

Figure 1 Binocular visual field plot representing a left-sided hemianopia.

HVFDs can be very disabling. They impair the patient’s ability to obtain a complete visual overview (CitationZihl 1995a), which can impact on their ability to interact with their environment. In addition, patients can experience further impairment in their daily life; for example, driving is prohibited in the majority of cases (CitationKooijman et al 2004) and HVFDs can lead to severe reading difficulties (CitationLeff et al 2001). Such difficulties arising from visual problems can have additional effects on the social and emotional functioning of the individual.

Traditionally HVFDs were considered untreatable, but recent advances in our understanding of the neural capacities for functional reorganization has led to an upsurge of attempts to achieve a reduction of the field loss through training. At the same time new rehabilitation procedures have been studied which might allow patients to more efficiently perceive the whole visual world and to improve reading performance despite persistent HVFDs. These procedures have been reviewed previously (CitationKerkhoff 2000; CitationPambakian et al 2005; CitationPelak et al 2007), including a recent systematic review (CitationBouwmeester et al 2007), and it was concluded that compensatory rehabilitation procedures can improve the visual searching skills and reading performance of patients with HVFDs. Despite these encouraging results, patients often do not receive specific rehabilitation for their HVFDs (CitationKerkhoff 1999; CitationPambakian et al 2005). Therefore the issue of how to increase rehabilitation needs to be addressed.

This review will examine what is needed to make the behavioral treatment of HVFDs more clinically relevant. Firstly, we will address why patients with HVFDs need treatment. Secondly, we will evaluate the efficacy of the different treatments which have been developed. Finally, we will discuss unresolved issues which may currently prevent clinicians from implementing the rehabilitation strategies.

HVFD: Impairment and disability

Reading problems have been objectively observed in 48% of hemianopic patients (CitationZihl 1999a) and are often cited as the most relevant behavioral difficulty (CitationKasten et al 1999). Hemianopic dyslexia is the term used to describe the particular pattern of reading problems that are associated with HVFDs, and the specific reading difficulties depend mainly on HVFD location (CitationTrauzettel-Klosinski and Brendler 1998). For example, in Western cultures where text is read from left to right, patients with right-sided defects experience more severe problems since the parafoveal information to the right is crucial for guiding appropriate reading eye-movements. Furthermore, reading is typically worse in patients who have less than 5° of visual sparing (CitationZihl 1995b), therefore specifically patients with macular splitting or a central scotoma. However, such patients are the minority of cases (CitationLeff 2004). Analysis of patients’ eye-movement data indicates that patients with HVFDs do not appear to compensate for their field loss when reading and show inefficient eye-movements (CitationTrauzettel-Klosinski and Brendler 1998; CitationMcDonald et al 2006), although this is not the case for all patients (CitationGassel and Williams 1963). CitationGassel and Williams (1963) also found that some patients with atypical eye-movements can still exhibit adequate reading speeds, demonstrating that atypical eye-movement patterns during reading do not necessarily lead to an impaired reading ability.

HVFDs can also lead to difficulties creating a complete visual overview, particularly in novel environments, thereby affecting the ability to find objects. This ability has been examined using visual search tasks, where the aim is to locate a target item amongst distracting elements. Patients typically take longer than healthy individuals to complete such tasks (CitationZihl 1995a), and can have similar difficulties performing other visual tasks such as identification and sorting (CitationZihl and Wohlfarth-Englert 1986; CitationZihl et al 1988). Many patients with HVFDs execute abnormally short eye-movements when looking into their blind regions and each movement is typically slower than that of a normally-sighted individual. This strategy is known as saccadic hypometria (CitationZihl 2000) and limits the patients’ ability to effectively search their environment, which contributes to their disorientation and obstacle avoidance problems. Approximately 70% of patients show such disorganized searching strategies (CitationKerkhoff 1999).

Due to the enjoyment which can be gained from reading and other leisure activities requiring visual search skills, any impairment in these has obvious consequences for the emotional well-being of the patient (CitationStelmack 2001). Similarly, HVFDs can restrict many other activities such as driving, which can lead to a loss of independence, thereby affecting social and emotional functioning. The primary cause of HVFD is stroke (CitationHuber 1992) and accordingly most patients are elderly (CitationCairns 2004). Therefore, visual problems increase the risk of accidents such as falls, to which this age-group are already prone (CitationAnderson 2002). HVFDs also reduce the efficacy of other rehabilitation procedures which may be aimed at increasing the patient’s mobility (for example physiotherapy). Bearing these factors in mind many patients have low scores in activities of daily living (ADL) measures (CitationPatel et al 2000; CitationSànchez-Blanco et al 2002), and functional rehabilitation outcomes are poor (CitationReding and Potes 1988).

Spontaneous recovery

Some spontaneous visual field restoration is widely accepted to occur. However, the number of patients who experience any restoration is undetermined, with reports ranging from 7% to 85% of cases (CitationKasten et al 1999). The amount of field recovery that an individual can experience is similarly variable (CitationZihl and Kennard 1996). In an extensive review of spontaneous recovery, CitationZhang and colleagues (2006) observed that the degree of natural recovery decreased as the amount of time since the onset of the HVFD increased. The approximate maximal period of spontaneous recovery is typically 3 months (CitationPambakian and Kennard 1997). In summary, spontaneous visual field recovery does not occur in all patients and complete recovery is rare, therefore rehabilitation for such patients is important. Knowing the likely pattern of natural recovery is important for assessment and rehabilitation, and is useful for determining the time at which training will be maximally effective.

Patients may also try to adapt to their visual loss. The obvious way of compensating for HVFDs is to make larger and more frequent eye-movements, specifically into the blind areas. Unfortunately not all patients adopt this strategy (CitationKerkhoff 1999). Rather, several researchers have shown that the eye-movements of many HVFD patients are very small and their scan-paths (the pattern of eye-movements used to scan a complex visual representation) are disorganized (CitationMeienberg et al 1981; CitationZihl 1995a, Citation1999b; CitationPambakian et al 2000). Patients who have a chronic HVFD show more organized scanning strategies than those patients whose difficulties are a recent occurrence (CitationZihl 1995a; CitationPambakian et al 2000). However, many patients’ eye-movements are still disorganized 14 months after onset (CitationKerkhoff 1999). These abnormal eye-movements can also be observed during reading (CitationZihl 1995b).

Patients may also use other forms of behavioral compensation. One method is the use of eccentric fixation; the eye is rotated slightly towards the blind hemi-field rather than straight ahead (CitationGassel and Williams 1963). This means that the centre of the observed image does not fall exactly on the fovea, but instead the image falls further into the seeing field at a slightly eccentric position. This strategy is found in approximately 30% of cases and can increase reading speed (CitationTrauzettel-Klosinski 1997), although the impact of such a strategy on other activities has not been clearly determined. It is a strategy which may be of use for patients with little central vision, for example those with macular splitting or a central scotoma, which as already mentioned is the minority of patients (CitationLeff 2004). However, eccentric fixation is not a useful strategy for patients with an intact fovea. Another strategy witnessed in children involves to-and-fro rocking motions, to bring greater portions of the visual field into view (CitationBoyle et al 2005).

In summary, HVFDs are debilitating and the sensory loss is exacerbated by the adoption of slow and inefficient search strategies when exploring the blind field. The prognosis for spontaneous recovery appears poor, and although adaptation is possible many patients do not develop effective ways of compensating for their deficits. Specific interventions are required for these patients.

Intervention

Most research has focused on three treatment approaches: restorative training, optical aids, and compensatory training. The restorative approach is the most ambitious, aiming to reduce the visual field loss through prolonged training. The second approach uses optical aids to artificially expand the patient’s visual field such that parts of the visual world which would otherwise fall into the blind field now appear in the seeing field. The third approach is compensatory training. This therapy is based on the assumption that the visual field defect cannot be changed significantly, and therefore attempts to alleviate the resulting disability by teaching patients to make more efficient eye-movements. We will discuss each of these three rehabilitation strategies in separate sections.

Restorative training

Restorative training aims to restore vision (at least in part) to the blind visual field, based on evidence which supports plasticity in the visual system of both animals (CitationCowey and Weiskrantz 1963; CitationCowey 1967; CitationMohler and Wurtz 1977; CitationEysel and Schwiegart 1999) and humans (CitationDonoghue 1997). Whilst there was early promise for the potential of such training (CitationZihl and Von Cramon 1979; CitationZihl and Von Cramon 1985), some dismissed the approach as ineffective, with any supposed field increase being regarded as the product of eye-movements (CitationBalliet et al 1985).

Restorative training was later revived by Kasten, Sabel, and colleagues who introduced a computerized therapy called Vision Restoration Therapy (VRT; CitationKasten and Sabel 1995; CitationKasten et al 1997). During VRT patients fixate a central point whilst visual stimuli are repeatedly presented in the border region between the blind and seeing field (the transition zone). The training is typically conducted in daily one-hour sessions for 6 months. Placebo-controlled studies have suggested that VRT leads to significant increases in the visual field (CitationKasten et al 1998, Citation2000), although more so for patients with optic nerve as opposed to cortical damage. These field increases were still observed at least 6 months after the end of the treatment (CitationKasten and Sabel 1995; CitationKasten et al 2001).

However, despite the apparent success of VRT critics have challenged the claim that it is an effective treatment (CitationHorton 2005; CitationPlant 2005; CitationMcFadzean 2006). This is because the claims of significant field increases found in the VRT studies mentioned above were based on a method of assessing the visual field which was incorporated into the training device and did not allow a reliable way of controlling the patients’ fixation. The effect of VRT has been re-evaluated using techniques which allowed a much more reliable means of monitoring the patients’ fixation, specifically the use of a scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO; CitationJamara et al 2003) combined with microperimetry. In studies whereby they reliably monitored fixation during visual field assessment no significant visual field increases were obtained (CitationSabel et al 2004; CitationReinhard et al 2005; CitationSchreiber et al 2006).

Recently CitationKasten and colleagues (2006) responded to this challenge by publishing a study in which they used a video-based system to monitor their patients’ fixation and found a significant (but somewhat modest) field increase of 1.8 degrees. This finding is insufficient to re-establish the therapeutic value of VRT for two reasons. Firstly, because the employed fixation-control technique is inferior to that used by other studies which did not find significant field increases. Secondly, even if we accept the reported field increase, such an increase of 1.8° is clearly not enough to convey a clinically significant benefit to patients who have endured 3 months of daily training and invested a considerable amount of money. In this context it is important to note that these expenses are currently not covered by medical insurance companies (CitationPelak et al 2007).

Even if the claim that VRT produces a clinically-relevant field increase is dismissed, the widespread subjective improvements reported by many participants of this training cannot be disregarded (CitationMueller et al 2003; CitationSabel et al 2004). The fact that improvements are sometimes reported by patients in the absence of any training-induced field recovery (CitationMueller et al 2003) raises an interesting question: why do patients report that subsequent to the training they find it easier to find objects and avoid obstacles, if their visual field is unchanged? It might be that these reports simply reflect the patients’ desire to justify a training in which they have invested a lot of time, effort, and money. However, it is also possible that VRT leads to behavioral improvements. For example, it is possible that VRT cues patients to allocate more attention into their blind field, and such attentional cueing can improve target detection (CitationPoggel et al 2006). Although visual costimulation was found to be no more effective than single stimulation VRT at expanding the visual field, it is clear that VRT has beneficial effects on attentional performance (CitationKasten et al 2007). Further research into the role of attention for visual field rehabilitation is required.

In addition to possible effects on attention, VRT could also inadvertently lead to eye-movements being more frequently directed into the blind field, although this explanation is denied by CitationKasten and colleagues (2006). However, it could explain why restorative training leads to changes in cortical activity as reported by several small-sample (n = 1–5) imaging and electrophysiology studies (CitationJulkunen et al 2003, Citation2006; CitationPleger et al 2003), since saccadic changes due to compensatory mechanisms could influence widespread neural activity. Initially these changes in cortical activity were interpreted as evidence for the training-induced brain plasticity underlying the recovery of visual field loss. However, in the absence of reliable evidence for such visual field recovery this interpretation appears unlikely.

More encouraging results have been obtained with children. Werth and colleagues presented findings from randomized placebo-controlled trials with a restorative training used with children aged between 1 and 15 years (CitationWerth and Moehrenschlager 1999; CitationWerth and Seelos 2005). In some cases the visual field defect disappeared completely, and the mean increase was 65 degrees. Such dramatic increases coincide with evidence suggesting that there is greater potential for recovery from damage sustained early in life (CitationPayne et al 1996; CitationBoyle et al 2005), possibly because child and adult cases typically differ with regards to etiology and lesion location (CitationKedar et al 2006), or perhaps due to greater neuronal plasticity more generally in children. However, due to the age of these patients conventional perimetry could not be performed and instead the researchers had to rely on observed changes in target-directed eye-movements to estimate the extent of visual field recovery. Accordingly the visual field measurements and reported field increases are difficult to interpret and could reflect compensatory mechanisms rather than restorative ones. Given the large improvements which appear possible, this training would appear worthwhile pursuing with children who have HVFDs.

In conclusion, restorative training in adults has failed to fulfill its early promise. Recent studies suggest that VRT does not lead to significant increases in visual field size but consistently yields subjective improvements. The basis of these subjective improvements is still unclear but it is possible that VRT leads to compensatory changes in behavior which are as yet unconfirmed. However, even if we were to assume that VRT leads to significant behavioral improvements it would still be inferior to other forms of compensatory training (see below) which appear to produce effective behavioral compensation with significantly less effort, cost and time.

Optical aids

Optical aids such as prism glasses can be used to reduce the apparent visual field loss by shifting visual stimuli from the blind field into the patient’s seeing field. These prisms are fitted to spectacles but need to be restricted to just one half of each of the lenses (typically on the side of the blind field). If the prisms were fitted across the entire lens then the visual space corresponding to the unaffected side would be moved outside the field of view, thereby simply replacing one blind field with another. Such prisms can be fitted to just one eye (monocular sector prisms) or both eyes (binocular sector prisms). Whilst such aids appear to enhance visual functioning (CitationGottlieb et al 1998; CitationLee and Perez 1999; CitationSzlyk et al 2005) they have their limitations. Monocular prisms provide an expansion of the visual field but at the cost of creating central double-vision (diplopia) which patients experience as unpleasant. Binocular prisms lead to field relocation rather than field expansion. Such problems probably explain why so far these optical aids proved only moderately successful in HVFD rehabilitation.

CitationPeli (2000; Citation2001) has introduced a new set of spectacles with monocularly fitted sector prisms which extend across the entire width of the spectacle lens but which spare the central aspect. This is known as vision multiplexing and using this technique field expansion is achieved without central diplopia. CitationPeli (2000) reported field expansion of about 20° and noted on the basis of subjective reports that patients seemed to benefit from the spectacles. Vision multiplexing seems promising but randomized controlled trials using objective measures of functional improvement are required to evaluate the clinical potential of such a technique.

Compensatory training

Even if training cannot achieve a significant reduction in the visual field loss, it might still be possible to help patients to cope more effectively with their HVFD. Scanning the visual world systematically with large sweeping eye-movements would appear to be the most obvious form of compensation, however many patients do not spontaneously adopt this strategy (CitationKerkhoff 1999). In order to improve patients’ ability to compensate for their visual loss, several researchers have developed training schemes designed to teach patients more efficient strategies for visual scanning.

The compensatory training approaches typically use target-localization tasks to train patients to make large eye-movements and use visual search tasks to teach patients to use systematic scanning strategies when searching their visual world. Sometimes additional training is included which helps the patient to utilize the strategies in everyday situations such as crossing the street or finding objects around the home (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994; CitationPambakian et al 2004). With compensation training patients usually receive daily one hour training sessions for about four weeks making it much less demanding than VRT.

Compensatory training in general leads to improved search performance and efficiency (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994; CitationZihl 1995a; CitationNelles et al 2001; CitationPambakian et al 2004; CitationVerlohr and Dannheim 2007). Two studies confirmed that the improvements obtained after training were significantly greater than those observed during untrained periods and are thus training specific (CitationKerkoff et al 1994; CitationPambakian et al 2004). For example, CitationPambakian and colleagues (2004) found that 76% of patients had faster search times following training, while 14% remained unchanged, and 10% were actually slower. Those with slower search times showed an improvement in their detection rates. Compensatory training has also been found to significantly enlarge the search field (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994; CitationPambakian et al 2004). Additionally, studies using eye-tracking show that compensatory training leads to better organized scanning strategies and larger saccades (CitationZihl 1995a). Furthermore, it was found that the post-training search improvements could be maintained for at least one month, and in some cases up to 22 months (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992).

Not only does it appear from these studies that compensatory training can significantly improve search but it is possible that it may actually increase the visual field itself (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994). However, not all studies have found significant visual field increases after compensatory training (CitationZihl 1995; CitationNelles et al 2001; CitationPambakian et al 2004). Caution is required when interpreting these findings as the same limitations relating to fixation control during perimetry are present as in studies examining restorative approaches.

The reading performance of patients with HVFDs is also often impaired (CitationLeff et al 2001). Consequently, specific compensatory reading training procedures have been developed to directly address this deficit (CitationZihl 1995b; CitationHan et al 2004; CitationSpitzyna et al 2007). CitationZihl (1995b) showed that such training can improve reading accuracy and speed although unfortunately there was no control group and so it is not known to what extent the effects were due to the training provided. In contrast, a recent study by CitationSpitzyna and colleauges (2007) included a placebo training to assess the specific effects of a reading training for patients with hemianopic dyslexia. They tested 19 patients with right-sided hemianopia and divided them into two groups. Group 1 received a reading training for two 4-week blocks. During this training, patients practiced reading moving text which scrolled from right to left. In group 2 patients received a 4-week block of placebo training and a 4-week block of reading training. In the placebo-training patients received pairs of pictures which differed only in a number of minor features and they had to detect these differences. The reading training but not the placebo training induced significant improvements in reading speed.

The results surrounding compensatory training indicate the promise which such an approach holds for helping patients to adapt to their visual loss, and whilst controversy continues to surround the use of VRT, compensation would appear to be a viable rehabilitation option for the patients with HVFDs. However, it is important to note that for most compensatory training regimes a placebo-controlled study examining their efficacy is still missing.

Unresolved issues of HVFD rehabilitation

As described above the lack of placebo-controlled evaluation trials (for an exception see CitationSpitzyna et al 2007) means that the clinical efficacy has not yet been established for any of the described rehabilitation procedures. It is thus too early to give a firm recommendation for any of these approaches. However, the compensatory training approach is the only one for which behavioral improvements in the form of improved search times, increased reading speed and larger eye-movements have been demonstrated. In fact, for one form of compensatory reading training its superiority over a placebo-training has already been established (CitationSpitzyna et al 2007). In contrast the same is not true for either restorative training or optical aids. In the case of restorative training the early claims of increased visual field size following training have not been confirmed by studies using more reliable means of assessing the visual field size and there is currently no evidence that the reported subjective improvements correspond to measurable behavioral improvements. In the case of optical aids evidence for behavioral improvements are also lacking, in particular for their most promising forms, the vision multiplexing prisms. Thus on the basis of current evidence the compensatory approach appears to be in our view the most promising, and we will therefore now turn our attention to those aspects of the compensatory therapy which require further research.

Transfer of training benefits to activities of daily living

The first issue relates to the question of whether the achieved training gains also lead to relevant improvements in activities of daily living (ADL), an aspect which is crucial to the clinical evaluation of any rehabilitation procedure. Unfortunately most studies examining compensatory search training either do not assess its impact on ADL tasks or rely solely upon subjective reports. Using questionnaires several researchers have found that patients do report improvements in such activities as finding objects in a room or on a table, and crossing the street (CitationKerkhoff et al 1994; CitationNelles et al 2001; CitationPambakian et al 2004), indicating that compensatory training produces functional improvements. It is however important to establish the functional benefits of the training with behavioral measures since subjective gains can be unreliable indicators of rehabilitation success.

There are a few studies which have used objective measures to assess the transfer of training benefits to ADL tasks. CitationKerkhoff and colleagues (1994) demonstrated that combined compensatory search training (ie, training which combines search tasks and exercises such as finding objects around the home) can yield improved search performance in more naturalistic forms of visual search (eg, searching for an item amongst distracters on a table) which use a wider field of view than that used during the training. Following training the patients in this study showed a 50% reduction in search time on the table test. Further to this, they also reported that 91% of the sample returned to some sort of part-time work after the training, indicating a positive functional outcome. CitationPambakian and colleagues (2004) reported training-related improvements in performance on activities representing ADL tasks, such as threading beads onto string. Whilst this does indicate the successful transfer to the visuomotor domain of the training benefits, there is a question of how much such tasks actually tell us about improvements in more commonplace everyday activities. Future work should examine more closely the efficacy of the training in relation to more relevant examples of ADL.

Driving is a major activity for which transfer of training gains would be beneficial since it is prohibited for the majority of patients with HVFDs. Unfortunately CitationKooijman and colleagues (2004) found that only 2 out of the 17 homonymous hemianopic patients who failed a test of practical driving fitness passed this test after a form of compensatory training. Given the social and emotional impact that the loss of driving has on patients with HVFDs the effect that compensatory training has on driving ability should be further examined. It is worth noting that patients with HVFDs may be able to drive as adequately as normal, healthy individuals (CitationSchulte et al 1999) and therefore perhaps driving guidelines should be modified such that an HVFD is not an automatic cause for license revocation.

CitationNelles and colleagues (2001) reported that patients’ subjective impression of their reading ability had improved after the compensatory search training. However, since reading performance has not been measured it is not possible to conclude if there is transfer from search training to reading tasks. Again reading is an activity which many patients report having difficulties with and which can severely impact on their quality of life, and as such should be considered an important outcome measure in future research. If it is found that general compensatory training can benefit reading (or that reading training can benefit other everyday activities) then only one type of training would be required.

Currently there is insufficient information about how the gains achieved with compensatory training transfer to other relevant activities like driving, reading, visuomotor control, and visual searching in natural surroundings. Furthermore, it is currently unclear whether some ADL tasks benefit more from training than others. If it is confirmed that some ADL tasks do not benefit sufficiently from compensatory training (for example reading), then training which addresses the specific requirements of those tasks will be needed.

Predictors of good training outcome

Not all patients benefit from compensatory training. In order to maximize its efficacy it is necessary to identify factors that contribute to the success or failure of this training. Conventional predictors of rehabilitation outcome, which include the cause of the HVFD (etiology) and the time since onset of the HVFD when training takes place have been examined. Etiology does not seem to have a significant effect on outcome (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994), although this is hardly surprising given that most patients share the same etiology, namely stroke. With respect to the timing of training, the results are mixed as some studies suggest that earlier training is more beneficial (CitationZihl 1995a), whilst others fail to find such an association (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994). Several studies have failed to find any association between age and training outcome (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994; CitationZihl 1995a). CitationKerkhoff and colleagues (1992) found that those patients who initially had the severest visual problems were those who showed the largest improvements after the training, thus pre-training impairment level could predict the possible success of the training.

A very plausible predictor for good training outcome is the degree of spared visual ability in the blind field. Some patients can respond quite accurately to visual stimuli presented to their blind field (for example by pointing to it) even though they insist that they cannot see it. This phenomenon has been called blindsight (CitationWeiskrantz et al 1974) and it is estimated that 15%–20% of HVFD patients show this (CitationBlythe et al 1987). It has been suggested that training patients to be aware of their blindsight capacity could be a useful rehabilitation strategy (CitationBoyle et al 2005) and repeatedly testing blindsight can lead to improved blindsight performance (CitationZihl 1980). It seems plausible that patients with blindsight are more successful in making accurate saccadic eye-movements to targets in their blind field and could therefore benefit more from compensatory training. However, this prediction has not yet been tested.

Cognitive variables, such as the patient’s ability to allocate visual attention or their spatial memory capacity, which might contribute to the success or failure of compensatory training, have not yet been examined. In summary, it seems clear that although compensatory treatments benefit the majority of participants, the factors which predict successful treatment outcome on an individual basis remain unclear.

Comorbidities

Some previous studies examining compensatory training have excluded people with HVFDs who suffered from additional disorders such as oculomotor deficits or hemispatial neglect. Disorder comorbidities are common (CitationAnderson and Rizzo 1995) and patients with multiple difficulties are typically more functionally impaired than those with a single defect (CitationPatel et al 2000). The impact that this may have on rehabilitation has not been fully established.

Hemispatial neglect is a disorder which typically occurs after right-hemispheric damage and leads to patients ignoring sensory information from the contralesional half of their body or surroundings. It might be predicted that patients with such additional disorders will gain less from compensatory training. However, some authors have argued that patients with both HVFD and hemispatial neglect can benefit from the training but require a more intensive schedule (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994). Similarly it has been shown in a case-study that a patient with multiple visual problems such as amblyopia and impaired form vision could still benefit from compensatory training (CitationHiramaya et al 2004).

In summary, HVFD patients with comorbidities such as hemispatial neglect may require more intensive training, but may still benefit from compensatory training. Having said that, the current evidence is scant and rigorous studies comparing the training benefits in large samples of patients with and without such comorbidities have yet to be conducted.

Parameters of effective compensatory training

Although it has been shown that compensatory training in general can lead to significant functional gains (CitationPambakian et al 2004), a number of different training regimens have been used and it is unclear which is the most effective. These regimens differ with respect to the required effort and cost, and it is important to establish whether the simpler and less costly forms are as effective as the more laborious ones. For example, training displays of various size have been used ranging from computer or television monitors, which train only the central 25 degrees of the visual field (CitationPambakian et al 2004), to displays which fill the entire visual field (CitationNelles et al 2001). It is obvious that training on a small screen is less costly because it means that the training can potentially be performed by the patient in their own home, as was done in the study by CitationPambakian and colleagues (2004), and as is the case with VRT (CitationKasten and Sabel 1995).

Treatment duration also differs significantly, ranging between 12 and 60 sessions (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992). In several studies (CitationKerkhoff et al 1992, Citation1994; CitationZihl 1995a) patients receive training until their performance plateaus or their search field increases by a specified amount. However in the other studies patients all receive a standardized amount of training. Research should attempt to determine what the maximal amount of training required is, and possible factors that may influence the amount of training required by a specific individual. This will ensure that time and resources are utilized to their best advantage.

Different types of training programs have not been compared directly and so we do not know yet whether the different regimens are equally effective. A confounding problem is that different outcome measures are also used in many of the studies making it difficult to compare them. Standardizing the outcome measures may aid this process, and CitationVerlohr and Dannheim (2007) recently proposed the visual performance test as a standardized outcome measure for the purpose of assessing search times. This task involves patients having to visually locate as quickly as possible a series of targets which can be at one of eleven positions on a screen. Reaction time is the main outcome measure.

Another version of compensatory search training combined auditory cues with visual search displays and reported significant improvements in exploratory eye-movements and transfer to ADL (CitationBolognini et al 2005). However, it is not known whether the achieved gains are superior to those observed with training using purely visual displays. A direct comparison between conventional and combined (ie, visual plus auditory stimulation) training is required to determine if adding auditory cues increases the efficacy of compensatory training.

In summary, there are different versions of compensatory training available, varying specifically in relation to the size of the training stimuli, the duration of the training, and the addition of attentional aids. Currently it is unclear which version produces the best clinical outcome. Standardizing outcome measures will make it easier to directly compare the benefits of different training techniques, which will allow researchers to develop the maximally effective training paradigm.

Conclusion

With the exception of one form of a compensatory reading training (CitationSpitzyna et al 2007) clinical efficacy has not been unequivocally established for any of the above described rehabilitation procedures. To establish clinical efficacy randomized placebo-controlled clinical evaluation trials are needed. Currently it is therefore too early to recommend any of the described rehabilitation procedures. However, we would like to argue that compensatory approaches have come further towards the aim of establishing their clinical efficacy than either the VRT approaches or the use of optical aids. In the case of compensatory approaches several studies found significant behavioral improvements following the training. The same can not be said for either VRT or optical aids. In both cases their claim of clinical efficacy currently rests on subjective patient reports and it is yet unknown whether these subjective reports of improvement correspond to measurable behavioral improvements. Apart from the need for placebo-controlled clinical evaluation trials we have also identified a number of other issues which need to be addressed by future research. These include the question of transfer, which is whether or not the compensatory training leads to improvements in relevant ADL tasks, the issue of outcome predictors and also which specific version of the compensatory training is the most effective form of treatment.

Disclosure

The researchers are supported by a grant from the Wolfson Research Institute and A. Lane is supported by a studentship jointly provided by the ESRC and MRC.

References

  • AndersonSWEslingerPJ2002Visuoperceptual ImpairmentsNeuropsychological Interventions: Clinical Research and PracticeNew YorkThe Guildford Press16381
  • AndersonSWRizzoM1995Recovery and rehabilitation of visual cortical dysfunctionNeuroRehabilitation512940
  • BallietRBloodKMTBach-y-RitaP1985Visual field rehabilitation in the cortically blind?J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry481113244078577
  • BlytheIMKennardCRuddockKH1987Residual vision in patients with retrogeniculate lesions of the visual pathwaysBrain1108879053651800
  • BologniniNRasiFCocciaM2005Visual search improvement in hemianopic patients after audio-visual stimulationBrain12828304216219672
  • BouwmeesterLHeutinkJLucasC2007The effect of visual training for patients with visual field defects due to brain damage: a systematic reviewJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry785556417135455
  • BoyleNJJonesDHHamiltonR2005Blindsight in children: does it exist and can it be used to help the child? Observations on a case seriesDev Med Child Neurol4769970216174315
  • CairnsNJGoldsteinLHMcNeilJE2004Neuroanatomy and NeuropathologyClinical Neuropsychology: A Practical Guide to Assessment and Management for CliniciansChichesterJohn Wiley and Sons Ltd2356
  • CoweyA1967Perimetric study of field defects in monkeys after cortical and retinal ablationsQ J Exp Psychol19232454965358
  • CoweyAWeiskrantzL1963A perimetric study of visual field defects in monkeysQ J Exp Psychol1590115
  • DonoghueJP1997Limits of reorganization in cortical circuitsCereb Cortex79799087818
  • EyselUTSchweigartG1999Increased receptive field size in the surround of chronic lesions in the adult cat visual cortexCereb Cortex9101910220223
  • GasselMMWilliamsD1963Visual function in patients with homonymous hemianopia. Part II: Oculomotor mechanismsBrain8613613946752
  • GottliebDDFuhrAHatchWV1998Neuro-optometric facilitation of vision recovery after acquired brain injuryNeuroRehabilitation1117599
  • HanYCiuffredaKJKapoorN2004Reading-related oculomotor testing and training protocols for acquired brain injury in humansBrain Res Protocols14112
  • HiramayaKSakaiSYamawakiR2004Visual search training for a case of homonymous field defect with multiple visual dysfunctions [abstract]No To Shinkei564031315279198
  • HortonJC2005Vision restoration therapy: confounded by eye movementsBr J Ophthalmol89792415965150
  • HuberA1992Homonymous hemianopiaNeuro-Ophthalmology1235166
  • JamaraRJVan De VeldeFPeliE2003Scanning eye movements in homonymous hemianopia documented by scanning laser ophthalmoscope retinal perimetryOptom Vis Sci8049550412858085
  • JulkunenLTenovuoOJääskeläinenS2003Rehabilitation of chronic post-stroke visual field defect with computer-assisted trainingRestor Neurol Neurosci21192812808199
  • JulkunenLTenovuoOVorobyevV2006Functional brain imaging, clinical and neurophysiological outcome of visual rehabilitation in a chronic stroke patientRestor Neurol Neurosci241233216720948
  • KastenEBunzenthalUMüller-OehringE2007Vision restoration therapy does not benefit from costimulation: A pilot studyJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol295698417691030
  • KastenEBunzenthalUSabelBA2006Visual field recovery after vision restoration therapy (VRT) is independent of eye-movements: An eye-tracker studyBehav Brain Res175182616970999
  • KastenEMüller-OehringESabelBA2001Stability of visual field enlargements following computer-based restitution training – results of a follow-upJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol2329730511404808
  • KastenEPoggelDAMüller-OehringE1999Restoration of vision II: Residual functions and training-induced visual field enlargement in brain-damaged patientsRestor Neurol Neurosci152738712671238
  • KastenEPoggelDASabelBA2000Computer-based training of stimulus detection improves colour and simple pattern recognition in the defective field of hemianopic subjectsJ Cogn Neurosci1210011211177420
  • KastenESabelBA1995Visual field enlargement after computer training in brain-damaged patients with homonymous deficits: an open pilot trialRestor Neurol Neurosci811327
  • KastenEStrasburgerHSabelBA1997Programs for diagnosis and therapy of visual field deficits in vision rehabilitationSpat Vis104995039176962
  • KastenEWüstSBehrens-BaumannW1998Computer-based training for the treatment of partial blindnessNature Med41083879734406
  • KedarSZhangXLynnMJ2006Pediatric Homonymous HemianopiaJAAPOS1024952
  • KerkhoffG1999Restorative and compensatory therapy approaches in cerebral blindness – a reviewRestor Neurol Neurosci152557112671237
  • KerkhoffG2000Neurovisual rehabilitation: recent developments and future directionsJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry6869170610811691
  • KerkhoffGMünßingerUHaafE1992Rehabilitation of homonymous scotomata in patients with postgeniculate damage of the visual system: saccadic compensation trainingRestor Neurol Neurosci424554
  • KerkhoffGMünßingerUMeierEK1994Neurovisual rehabilitation in cerebral blindnessArch Neurol51474818179497
  • KooijmanACBrouwerWHCoeckelberghTRM2004Compensatory viewing training improves practical fitness to drive of subjects with impaired visionVis Impair Res6127
  • LeeAGPerezAM1999Improving awareness of peripheral visual field using sectorial prismJ Am Optom Assoc70624810561920
  • LeffA2004A historical review of the representation of the visual field in primary visual cortex with special reference to the neural mechanisms underlying macular sparingBrain Lang882687814967211
  • LeffAPCrewesHPlantGT2001The functional anatomy of single-word reading in patients with hemianopic and pure alexiaBrain1245102111222451
  • McDonaldSASpitsynaGShillcockRC2006Patients with hemianopic alexia adopt an inefficient eye movement strategy when reading textBrain1291586716317018
  • McFadzeanRM2006NovaVision: vision restoration therapyCurr Opin Ophthalmol1749850317065915
  • MeienbergOZangemeisterWHRosenbergM1981Saccadic eye movement strategies in patients with homonymous hemianopiaAnn Neurol9537447259115
  • MohlerCWWurtzRH1977Role of striate cortex and superior colliculus in visual guidance of saccadic eye movements in monkeysJ Neurophysiol407494401874
  • MuellerIPoggelDAKenkelS2003Vision restoration therapy after brain damage: Subjective improvements of activities of daily life and their relationship to visual field enlargementsVis Impair Res515778
  • NellesGEsserJExksteinA2001Compensatory visual field training for patients with hemianopia after strokeNeurosci Lett3061899211406327
  • PambakianACurrieJKennardC2005Rehabilitation strategies for patients with homonymous visual field defectsJ Neuro-Ophthalmol2513642
  • PambakianALMKennardC1997Can visual function be restored in patients with homonymous hemianopia?Br J Ophthalmol8132489215064
  • PambakianALMMannanSKHodgsonTL2004Saccadic visual search training: a treatment for patients with homonymous hemianopiaJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry751443815377693
  • PambakianALMWoodingDSPatelN2000Scanning the visual world: a study of patients with homonymous hemianopiaJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry69751911080227
  • PatelATDuncanPWLaiSM2000The relation between impairments and functional outcomes poststrokeArch Phys Med Rehabil8113576311030501
  • PayneBRLomberSGMacneilMA1996Evidence for greater sight in blindsight following damage of primary visual cortex early in lifeNeuropsychologia34741748817506
  • PelakVSDubinMWhitneyE2007Homonymous hemianopia: A critical analysis of optical devices, compensatory training, and NovaVisionCurr Treat Opt Neurol9417
  • PeliE2000Field expansion for homonymous hemianopia by optically induced peripheral exotropiaOptom Vis Sci774536411014672
  • PeliE2001Vision multiplexing: an engineering approach to vision rehabilitation device developmentOptom Vis Sci783041511384008
  • PlantGT2005A work out for hemianopiaBr J Ophthalmol89215615734
  • PlegerBFoersterA-FWiddigW2003Functional magnetic resonance imaging mirrors recovery of visual perception after repetitive tachistoscopic stimulation in patients with partial cortical blindnessNeurosci Lett335192612531465
  • PoggelDAKastenEMüller-OehringEM2006Improving residual vision by attentional cueing in patients with brain lesionsBrain Res1097142816777076
  • RedingMJPotesE1988Rehabilitation outcome following initial unilateral hemispheric stroke: life table analysis approachStroke191354583188120
  • ReinhardJSchreiberASchieferU2005Does visual restitution training change absolute homonymous visual field defects? A fundus controlled studyBr J Ophthalmol8930515615742
  • SabelBAKenkelSKastenE2004Vision restoration therapy (VRT) efficacy as assessed by comparative perimetric analysis and subjective questionnairesRestor Neurol Neurosurg Neurosci22399420
  • Sànchez-BlancoIOchoa-SangradorCLópez-MunaínL2002Predictive model of functional independence in stroke patients admitted to a rehabilitation programmeClin Rehabil1346475
  • SchreiberAVontheinRReinhardJ2006Effect of visual restitution training on absolute homonymous scotomasNeurology67143516832095
  • SchulteTStrasburgerHMuller-Oehring1999Automobile driving performance of brain-injured patients with visual field defectsAm J Phys Med Rehabil781364210088588
  • SpitzynaGAWiseRJSMcDonaldSA2007Optokinetic therapy improves text reading in patients with hemianopic alexia: A controlled trialNeurology6819223017536049
  • StelmackJ2001Quality of life of low-vision patients and outcomes of low vision rehabilitationOptom Vis Sci783354211384011
  • SzlykJPSeipleWStelmackJ2005Use of prisms for navigation and driving in hemianopic patientsOphthal Physiol Opt2512835
  • Trauzettel-KlosinskiS1997Eccentric fixation with hemianopic field defects: A valuable strategy to improve reading ability and an indication of cortical plasticityNeuro-Ophthalmology1811731
  • Trauzettel-KlosinskiSBrendlerK1998Eye movements in reading with hemianopic field defects: the significance of clinical parametersGraefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol236911029498119
  • VerlohrDDannheimF2007The visual performance test: indications for compensational visual rehabilitation training and first resultsStrabismus1563817523048
  • WeiskrantzLWarringtonEKSandersMD1974Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablationBrain97709284434190
  • WerthRMoehrenschlagerM1999The development of visual functions in cerebrally blind children during a systematic visual field trainingRestor Neurol Neurosci152294112671235
  • WerthRSeelosK2005Restitution of visual functions in cerebrally blind childrenNeuropsychologia4320112315876440
  • ZhangXKedarSLynnMJ2006Natural history of homonymous hemianopiaNeurology66901516567709
  • ZihlJ1980“Blindsight”: Improvement of visually guided eye movements by systematic practice in patients with cerebral blindness”Neuropsychologia187177366825
  • ZihlJ1995aVisual scanning behavior in patients with homonymous hemianopiaNeuropsychologia332873037791997
  • ZihlJ1995bEye movement patterns in hemianopic dyslexiaBrain1188919127655887
  • ZihlJBrownJW1999aCerebral Disturbances of Elementary Visual FunctionsNeuropsychology of Visual PerceptionNew YorkLawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.3558
  • ZihlJ1999bOculomotor scanning performance in subjects with homonymous visual field disordersVis Impair Res12331
  • ZihlJ2000Rehabilitation of Visual Disorders After Brain InjuryEast SussexPsychology Press Ltd
  • ZihlJKennardCBrandtTCaplanLRDichgansJ1996Disorders of Higher Visual FunctionNeurological Disorders: Course and TreatmentCaliforniaAcademic Press20112
  • ZihlJRothWKerkhoffG1988The influence of homonymous visual field disorders on colour sorting performance in the FM 100-hue testNeuropsychologia26869763264058
  • ZihlJVon CramonD1979Restitution of visual function in patients with cerebral blindnessJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry4231222458478
  • ZihlJVon CramonD1985Visual field recovery from scotoma in patients with postgeniculate damage: a review of 55 casesBrain108335654005527
  • ZihlJWohlfarth-EnglertA1986The influence of visual field disorders on visual identification tasksEur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci2366143743589