Publication Cover
Stress
The International Journal on the Biology of Stress
Volume 17, 2014 - Issue 1
887
Views
45
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

High novelty-seeking rats are resilient to negative physiological effects of the early life stress

, &
Pages 97-107 | Received 13 Jun 2013, Accepted 06 Sep 2013, Published online: 06 Nov 2013

Abstract

Exposure to early life stress dramatically impacts adult behavior, physiology, and neuroendocrine function. Using rats bred for novelty-seeking differences and known to display divergent anxiety, depression, and stress vulnerability, we examined the interaction between early life adversity and genetic predisposition for high- versus low-emotional reactivity. Thus, bred Low Novelty Responder (bLR) rats, which naturally exhibit high anxiety- and depression-like behavior, and bred High Novelty Responder (bHR) rats, which show low anxiety/depression together with elevated aggression, impulsivity, and addictive behavior, were subjected to daily 3 h maternal separation (MS) stress postnatal days 1–14. We hypothesized that MS stress would differentially impact adult bHR/bLR behavior, physiology (stress-induced defecation), and neuroendocrine reactivity. While MS stress did not impact bHR and bLR anxiety-like behavior in the open field test and elevated plus maze, it exacerbated bLRs’ already high physiological response to stress – stress-induced defecation. In both tests, MS bLR adult offspring showed exaggerated stress-induced defecation compared to bLR controls while bHR offspring were unaffected. MS also selectively impacted bLRs’ (but not bHRs’) neuroendocrine stress reactivity, producing an exaggerated corticosterone acute stress response in MS bLR versus control bLR rats. These findings highlight how genetic predisposition shapes individuals’ response to early life stress. Future work will explore neural mechanisms underlying the distinct behavioral and neuroendocrine consequences of MS in bHR/bLR animals.

Introduction

Evidence in humans and experimental animals shows that early life adversity negatively impacts neurodevelopment and produces lifelong changes in stress reactivity, emotional behavior, and vulnerability to psychopathology (Heim et al., Citation2004; Ladd et al., Citation2000). The deleterious effects of early life stress are particularly pronounced in individuals genetically predisposed to psychiatric illness (Caspi et al., Citation2002, Citation2003), although there is much to learn about how gene-environment interactions shape brain development, emotional behavior, and vulnerability to disease.

Animal models offer useful tools to examine how early life adversity impacts brain and behavior. One popular neonatal stress paradigm is the rodent maternal separation (MS) stress model, where pups are deprived of maternal contact for 3-h periods during the first weeks of life (Lehmann et al., Citation2000; Sanchez et al., Citation2001). MS elicits myriad negative effects, including increased anxiety (Huot et al., Citation2001; Roth et al., Citation2009), depression-like behavior (Aisa et al., Citation2009; Franklin et al., Citation2010; Ladd et al., Citation2000), and exaggerated hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis stress responses (Holmes et al., Citation2005; Ladd et al., Citation2000; Plotsky & Meaney, Citation1993; Plotsky et al., Citation1998). The MS literature is somewhat inconsistent, with certain studies unable to confirm such behavioral and neuroendocrine findings. The conflicting results likely stem from a host of factors, including varied experimental procedures, gender, and rat strain (Hulshof et al., Citation2011; Lehmann & Feldon, Citation2000; Sterley et al., Citation2011). Importantly, rodents, like humans, exhibit a range of early life stress vulnerability, with some individuals particularly susceptible to its effects and others resilient (Lehmann & Feldon, Citation2000).

In recent years, our laboratory developed bred lines of Sprague-Dawley rats that display differences in novelty exploration and a host of emotional behaviors (Stead et al., Citation2006). Rats bred for high response to novelty (bred High Responders, bHRs) exhibit increased aggression (Kerman et al., Citation2011), impulsivity (Flagel et al., Citation2010), and proclivity to addictive behavior (Cummings et al., Citation2011; Davis et al., Citation2008; Flagel et al., Citation2010) compared to low novelty reactive rats (bred Low Responders, bLRs), which show high levels of anxiety- and depressive-like behavior (Clinton et al., Citation2008, Citation2011b; Perez et al., Citation2009; Stead et al., Citation2006; Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011). The distinct bHR/bLR phenotypes are likely driven by several factors, including distinct ontogeny of hippocampal circuits (Clinton et al., Citation2011b), HPA axis reactivity (Clinton et al., Citation2008; Kerman et al., Citation2012), and the Fibroblast Growth Factor (FGF) system (Perez et al., Citation2009; Turner et al., Citation2011). The bHR/bLR phenotypes are highly heritable (Stead et al., Citation2006), although some traits are influenced by environmental factors such as maternal style (Clinton et al., Citation2007; Kristiansen et al., Citation2007) and chronic stress (Clinton et al., Citation2008; Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011).

Considering bLRs’ anxious/depressive-like phenotype coupled with their chronic stress vulnerability, we hypothesized that bLRs would be susceptible to the effects of MS stress, while bHRs would be resilient. Thus, the present study subjected bHR/bLR pups to daily 3 h MS (or raised under normal conditions) during the first two postnatal weeks. Anxiety- and depression-like behavior and neuroendocrine stress responsivity were assessed in adult offspring.

Materials and methods

Animals

Animals were acquired from the University of Michigan in-house bHR/bLR colony. We previously published our breeding strategy and initial behavioral characterization of the bHR/bLR lines (Stead et al., Citation2006), and continue to examine the bHR-bLR behavioral phenotypes (Flagel et al., Citation2010; Kerman et al., Citation2011; Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011). Animals for the present experiments were taken from the 10th generation of the colony.

Males to be used for mating as well as male offspring used for later experiments were kept on a 12:12 light:dark cycle (lights on at 6 a.m.) with food and water available ad libitum. Female rats (as well as male-female mating pairs) were housed in a separate room kept on a 14:10 light:dark cycle to promote regular estrous cycles and fertility (Everett & Sawyer, Citation1949; Ying et al., Citation1973). The animal housing rooms and testing facilities are kept at 21–23 °C at 50–55% humidity. Litters were weaned on postnatal day 21 (P21). Only weaned male offspring were housed 3–4 per cage and kept for subsequent experiments. Procedures were conducted in accordance with the principles and procedures outlined in the National Institutes of Health guidelines for the care and use of animals and approved by the University of Michigan University Committee on the Use and Care of Animals.

Separation procedure

Male and female rats (N = 10 bHR male/female pairs, and N = 10 bLR male/female pairs) were housed together for 14 days. Male partners were then removed and females remained singly housed until giving birth. All litters were born within 4 days of one another, and all pups within a litter were treated in similarly (that is, all pups within a “maternal separation” experimental litter were removed from the mother daily 3-h per day from P1--14 and all pups from a control litter remained with their mother P1–14). At birth (P0), bHR/bLR litters were culled to 12 pups (6 males/6 females). Litters remained with their biological mothers and were randomly assigned to experimental groups: (a) MS stress or (b) non-separated control (n = 5 litters per bHR/bLR phenotype per stress treatment group). The following day, we initiated a MS stress procedure modeled after Plotsky & Meaney (Citation1993) and Card et al. (Citation2005). Non-separated control litters remained with their biological mothers continuously except for once weekly cage changes. Beginning on P1 (and continuing through P14), MS litters were separated from their dam for 3 h. An entire litter was moved with a handful of home bedding to a small box on a heating pad. The temperature of the nest was around 37 °C when the litter was transferred to the cage over the heating pad. The separation cages remained in the same room approximately 1.2 m from the home cage, where the dam remained. Littermates remained in close contact with one another throughout the MS period. At the conclusion of the MS period, pups and bedding were returned to the home cage. Pups were weaned on P21 and only males were chosen for subsequent tests performed in adulthood (75 + days). Each experimental group (n = 12/phenotype/treatment) consisted of no more than 3 littermates.

Behavioral analyses MS and control bHR/bLR offspring

When MS and control bHR/bLR male offspring reached adulthood (P75), they were subjected to a test battery to assess novelty-induced locomotion, anxiety- and depression-like behaviors (n = 12/phenotype/treatment as noted above). The order of the behavioral tests was as follows: (1) the locomotor response to novelty test; (2) the open field test; (3) the light-dark box test; (4) the elevated plus maze test; and (5) the forced swim test. Rats were given 2–3 days rest between each test to minimize the carry-over effect on behavior from test to test. Prior experiments with the selectively-bred bLR/bHR animals used a similar strategy, assessing bLR and bHR rats in multiple tests over a period of days to weeks. These prior studies demonstrated that bLR/bHR rats (a) exhibit reliable behavioral differences across several behavior tests and (b) behave consistently on a particular test (such as the light–dark box) whether they are exposed only to that single test, or subjected to it after completing a series of other behavior tests over time (Supplementary Figure 1). It took approximately 10 days to complete this test battery, so rats were P85 on the final test day.

Locomotor response to novelty

Rats were screened to assess novelty-induced locomotion as previously described (Stead et al., Citation2006). Rats were individually placed in standard clear acrylic cages (43 × 21.5 × 25.5 cm high) equipped with infrared photocell emitters mounted 2.3 and 6.5 cm above the floor to record horizontal and rearing movement, respectively. Test chambers were located in a room separate from housing quarters, and the rats were exposed to the test room for the first time on the test day. A computer monitored horizontal and rearing movements in 5-min intervals over 60-min. Testing was performed between 8 a.m. and 11 a.m. Total locomotor scores for each rat were calculated by adding the number of horizontal and rearing movements over the 60-min test period.

Anxiety behavior

Rats’ anxiety-like behavior was assessed using three classic rodent behavioral tests: the open field (OF) test, light–dark box (LDB) test, and elevated plus maze (EPM) test. Each test assessed novelty-induced locomotor activity, time spent in anxiogenic portions of the test apparatus (center of OF; light compartment in the LDB test, and open arms of the EPM), and latency to initially enter anxiogenic regions of the test apparatus. All testing was performed between 8:00–11:30 a.m.

Open field test

The OF apparatus was a 100 × 100 × 50 cm white Plexiglas box with black Plexiglas floor, and testing was conducted under dim light (30 lux). Behavior was recorded using a computerized videotracking system (Noldus Ethovision, Leesburg, VA). The experiment began by placing the rat into a corner of the open field. The tracking system recorded the latency to first enter the center of the open field, the amount of time spent in the center, periphery, or corner of the apparatus, and the total distance traveled during the 5-min test.

Light–dark box test

The test apparatus was a 30 × 60 × 30 cm Plexiglas shuttle-box divided into two equal-sized compartments by a wall with a 12-cm-wide open door. One compartment was white and brightly illuminated (100 lux), and the other compartment was black and dimly lit (5 lux). The experiment began by placing the rat into the dark compartment and the door between the two compartments was removed. Rows of photocells located 2.5 cm above the stainless steel grid floor monitored beam breaks (indicating locomotor activity) and time spent in each compartment. A microprocessor recorded the latency to first exit the dark compartment, the number of photocell beam breaks and time spent in each compartment during the 5-min test.

Elevated plus maze test

The apparatus was constructed of black Plexiglas, with four elevated arms (70 cm from the floor, 45 cm long, and 12 cm wide) arranged in a cross. Two opposite arms were enclosed by 45-cm-high walls (lighting approximately 3–5 lux) and the other two arms were open (lighting approximately 30 lux). A central square platform at the intersection of the open and closed arms provided access to all arms. The test room was dimly lit (approximately 30 lux), and behavior was monitored using a computerized videotracking system (Noldus Ethovision, Leesburg, VA). At the beginning of the 5-min test, each rat was placed in the central square facing a closed arm. The tracking system recorded latency to first enter the open arm, the amount of time spent in the open arms, closed arms, or center square, and the total distance traveled over the course of the 5-min test.

Depression-like behavior

Depression-like behavior was assessed with Porsolt’s Forced Swim Test (FST) using the procedure described by Cryan et al (Citation2005). The water chambers were Plexiglas cylinders (40 cm high × 40 cm diameter) containing 30 cm deep water (25 °C). On FST day 1, rats were placed (one rat/cylinder) in the water for 15-min (pretest phase); 24 h later the rats returned to the water-filled cylinders and videotaped for 5-min (test phase). Water was changed after every swim session so that every rat was swimming in clean water. Each rat’s FST day 2 behavior was scored with a computerized program (Noldus Observer 5.0, Leesburg, VA), to assess the duration and frequency of events from the videotape. The person scoring videotapes was blind to experimental treatments and trained to observe: swimming, climbing, and immobility/floating during the test phase. Data are presented as percent time spent immobile, climbing or swimming during the 5-min Day 2 test.

Neuroendocrine studies

To assess impact of MS on bHR/bLR adult offsprings’ HPA axis, we measured adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) and corticosterone (CORT) secretion following the LDB anxiety test. Baseline blood samples (100 μl) were collected in EDTA-coated tubes via tail vein nick one day before LDB testing between 8:00–10:00 a.m. Animals were removed from their home cage, immobilized lightly, and a lateral tail vein was punctured with the corner of a razor blade to collect blood. Rats were then returned to their home cages. The next day, blood samples were similarly collected 5-, 20-, and 60-min after the beginning of the LDB test. Blood samples were separated by centrifugation (1000 × g, 10 min at 4 °C), and plasma was removed, frozen and stored at −80 °C until assay. Plasma ACTH and CORT were measured using commercially available radioimmunoassay kits (MP Biomedicals, Solon, OH) according to package instructions. The sensitivity of the CORT assay was 12.5 ng/ml and sensitivity of the ACTH assay was 15 pg/ml; intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were less than 5%.

Statistical analysis

Behavioral data were analyzed with 2-way ANOVA (bHR/bLR phenotype × MS condition as independent factors). Radioimmunoassay data were analyzed by 3-way ANOVA (bHR/bLR phenotype × MS group × blood collection timepoint as independent factors). ANOVAs were followed by Fisher's post-hoc comparisons when necessary. Data were analyzed using SPSS for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL), and for all tests α = 0.05.

Results

Early life MS stress does not impact novelty-induced locomotion in bHR/bLR rats

MS did not affect bHR and bLR offsprings’ adult locomotor response to novelty. Control bHRs (1083 ± SEM 44 activity counts) and MS bHRs (1263 ± SEM 42 activity counts) were dramatically more active in the novel environment compared to both bLR controls (185 ± SEM 16 activity counts) and bLR MS rats (252 ± SEM 13 counts; main effect of phenotype [F(1,121) = 903.36, p < 0.0001]). There was no effect of MS, and no MS x phenotype interaction. We observed the same effect in the other behavioral tests, with bHRs consistently being more active than bLRs in the OF test (main effect of phenotype [F(1,153) = 132.42, p < 0.0001]), and LDB (main effect of phenotype [F(1,90) = 113.07, p < 0.0001]; data not shown).

Early life MS stress does not impact typical bHR/bLR anxiety behavior differences

MS and control bLRs showed typically high levels of anxiety-like behavior across all three behavior tests – the OF test, the EPM and LDB. In the OF test, bLRs spent less time in the center of the OF (main effect of phenotype [F(1,153) = 23.24, p < 0.0001]; ), and showed greater latency to enter the center (main effect of phenotype [F(1,153) = 12.12, p < 0.001]; ) compared to bHRs. In the EPM, bLRs spent less time in the open arms (main effect of phenotype [F(1,91) = 309.37, p < 0.001]; ) and showed greater latency to enter the open arms (main effect of phenotype [F(1,91) = 6.32, p < 0.05]; ) relative to bHRs. Likewise, in the LDB test, bLRs spent less time in the light box (main effect of phenotype [F(1,90) = 61.66, p < 0.0001]; ) and longer latency to enter the light (main effect of phenotype [F(1,90) = 36.14, p < 0.0001]; ) compared to bHRs. A history of MS stress did not impact any of these measures (time spent in the anxiogenic portion of the test apparatus or the latency to enter that region) in either phenotype (no main effects of MS stress and no MS × phenotype interactions).

Figure 1. Impact of MS stress on adult offspring’s anxiety-like behavior. Adult bHR and bLR offspring that were maternally separated (MS) or remained undisturbed (CON) in early life were tested in the Open Field (OF), the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM), and the Light Dark Box (LDB). bLRs consistently showed greater anxiety compared to bHRs, spending less time in the center of the OF (A), less time in the open arms of the EPM (C), and less time in the light compartment in the LDB (E). bLRs also showed greater latency to initially enter these anxiogenic regions compared to bHRs (B, D, F). A history of MS had no impact on any of these measures. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05; ***indicates p < 0.0001.

Figure 1. Impact of MS stress on adult offspring’s anxiety-like behavior. Adult bHR and bLR offspring that were maternally separated (MS) or remained undisturbed (CON) in early life were tested in the Open Field (OF), the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM), and the Light Dark Box (LDB). bLRs consistently showed greater anxiety compared to bHRs, spending less time in the center of the OF (A), less time in the open arms of the EPM (C), and less time in the light compartment in the LDB (E). bLRs also showed greater latency to initially enter these anxiogenic regions compared to bHRs (B, D, F). A history of MS had no impact on any of these measures. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05; ***indicates p < 0.0001.

MS stress leads bLRs to exhibit exaggerated stress-induced defecation in adulthood

As an additional index of anxiety-like behavior, we also counted the number of fecal boli following each OF and EPM test. Interestingly, this analysis revealed marked differences between bHR/bLR rats as well as an effect of MS stress (). bLR rats generally produced more fecal boli than bHRs in both the OF test (main effect of phenotype [F(1,116) = 242.81, p < 0.0001]; ) and the EPM (main effect of phenotype [F(1,116) = 41.88, p < 0.0001]; ). A history of MS stress also increased defecation in the OF and EPM (main effect of MS in the OF ([F(1,116) = 4.59, p < 0.05]) and a stress × phenotype interaction in the OF ([F(1,116) = 4.26, p < 0.05]) and EPM ([F(1,116) = 5.46, p < 0.05]). Post hoc analysis showed that MS specifically impacted bLRs, leading MS-bLR adults to exhibit greater stress-induced defecation compared to control bLRs in the OF and EPM. MS and control bHR rats showed similarly low levels of stress-induced defecation in these tests ().

Figure 2. Impact of MS on adult offspring’s stress-induced defecation. Anxiety was also assessed via the amount of stress-induced defecation in bHR/bLR maternally separated (MS) and undisturbed (CON) offspring following the Open Field (OF; A); and the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM; B) tests. In general, bLRs exhibited far greater stress-induced defecation compared to bHRs in both tests. Interestingly, this difference was further magnified by a history of MS, as MS-bLR animals showed greater defecation in both tests compared to control bLRs. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; **indicates p < 0.01; ***<0.0001.

Figure 2. Impact of MS on adult offspring’s stress-induced defecation. Anxiety was also assessed via the amount of stress-induced defecation in bHR/bLR maternally separated (MS) and undisturbed (CON) offspring following the Open Field (OF; A); and the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM; B) tests. In general, bLRs exhibited far greater stress-induced defecation compared to bHRs in both tests. Interestingly, this difference was further magnified by a history of MS, as MS-bLR animals showed greater defecation in both tests compared to control bLRs. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; **indicates p < 0.01; ***<0.0001.

Early life MS does not impact typical bHR/bLR depression-like behavior differences in the FST

We used Porsolt’s FST to assess depression-like behavior in bHR/bLR MS and control offspring. Both MS and control bLRs showed a high level of immobility (main effect of phenotype [F(1,119) = 28.38, p < 0.0001]; ), and less climbing (main effect of phenotype [F(1,119) = 18.35, p < 0.0001]; ) and swimming (main effect of phenotype [F(1,119) = 8.69, p < 0.01]; ) compared to bHRs. There were no effects of MS on any measure, and no stress × phenotype interactions.

Figure 3. Impact of MS stress on adult offspring’s depression-like behavior. Adult bHR and bLR offspring that were maternally separated (MS) or remained undisturbed (CON) in early life were subjected to the Forced Swim Test (FST) to assess depression-like behavior. bLRs exhibited greater depression-like behavior in the FST compared to bHRs, showing greater immobility (A), and less climbing (B) and swimming (C) during the 5-min test. A history of MS had no impact on any of these measures. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05; ***indicates p < 0.0001.

Figure 3. Impact of MS stress on adult offspring’s depression-like behavior. Adult bHR and bLR offspring that were maternally separated (MS) or remained undisturbed (CON) in early life were subjected to the Forced Swim Test (FST) to assess depression-like behavior. bLRs exhibited greater depression-like behavior in the FST compared to bHRs, showing greater immobility (A), and less climbing (B) and swimming (C) during the 5-min test. A history of MS had no impact on any of these measures. Groups were compared using a 2-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05; ***indicates p < 0.0001.

MS stress produces exaggerated neuroendocrine reactivity in bLR offspring

We examined the effect of MS on HPA axis reactivity in bHR/bLR offspring by measuring ACTH and CORT secretion following the LDB test. Across all groups, ACTH and CORT levels sharply increased following the LDB, and then gradually decreased back to near-baseline by 60-min post-test (main effect of time for ACTH [F(1,108) = 24.20, p < 0.0001] and CORT [F(1,108) = 43.22, p < 0.0001]; ). For ACTH levels, there were no main effects of bHR/bLR phenotype or MS exposure, but there was a timepoint × phenotype × MS exposure interaction [F(1,108) = 5.19, p < 0.001]. For CORT levels, there were main effects of bHR/bLR phenotype [F(1,108) = 5.55, p < 0.05] and stress group [F(1,108) = 6.39, p < 0.05] as well as a timepoint × phenotype × MS exposure interaction [F(1,108) = 6.32, p < 0.001].

Figure 4. Impact of MS stress on novelty-induced adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) and corticosterone secretion. Basal blood samples were collected 24 h prior to the Light Dark Box (LDB) test, and additional blood samples were taken 5-, 20-, and 60-min after the beginning of the test. Black bar indicates the 5-min LDB test period. bLR controls, compared to bHR controls, exhibited a blunted stress-induced ACTH (A) and corticosterone (B) secretion following the LDB test. A history of MS shifted animals’ neuroendocrine responsivity, with the most striking effects in bLR. Maternally separated (MS) bLRs exhibited a distinctly “bHR-like” neuroendocrine response, showing enhanced ACTH (C) and corticosterone (D) secretion compared to bLR controls. MS only subtly influenced bHRs’ hormonal stress response, producing a dampened peak ACTH response (E), with no apparent impact on corticosterone (F). Groups were compared using a 3-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05 a significant difference between experimental groups at a particular timepoint.

Figure 4. Impact of MS stress on novelty-induced adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) and corticosterone secretion. Basal blood samples were collected 24 h prior to the Light Dark Box (LDB) test, and additional blood samples were taken 5-, 20-, and 60-min after the beginning of the test. Black bar indicates the 5-min LDB test period. bLR controls, compared to bHR controls, exhibited a blunted stress-induced ACTH (A) and corticosterone (B) secretion following the LDB test. A history of MS shifted animals’ neuroendocrine responsivity, with the most striking effects in bLR. Maternally separated (MS) bLRs exhibited a distinctly “bHR-like” neuroendocrine response, showing enhanced ACTH (C) and corticosterone (D) secretion compared to bLR controls. MS only subtly influenced bHRs’ hormonal stress response, producing a dampened peak ACTH response (E), with no apparent impact on corticosterone (F). Groups were compared using a 3-way ANOVA and Fisher’s post hoc comparisons where necessary; *indicates p < 0.05 a significant difference between experimental groups at a particular timepoint.

Post hoc analyses showed that bHR controls exhibited exaggerated ACTH and CORT secretion following the LDB test compared to bLR controls (), which parallels our previous work in the bred lines (Clinton et al., Citation2008) and other studies in commercially purchased HR/LR rats (Kabbaj et al., Citation2000; Piazza et al., Citation1991, Citation1993; Rouge-Pont et al., Citation1998). Specifically, bHR controls had greater ACTH levels at the 5-min timepoint () and greater CORT levels at the 20-min timepoint compared to bLR controls (ure 4B; p < 0.05 for both).

To illustrate the impact of MS on bHR and bLR neuroendocrine reactivity, we separately present data for bLR and bHR rats in and , respectively. MS markedly changed bLRs’ HPA axis output, leading to enhanced ACTH secretion at the 5-min timepoint (p < 0.05) and increased CORT secretion at the 20-min timepoint compared to bLR controls (). On the other hand, bHR’s stress reactivity was only subtly impacted by MS, with MS-bHR animals showing reduced ACTH secretion at the 5-min timepoint (p < 0.05) (a pattern that was indistinguishable from control bLRs), but no CORT differences compared to bHR controls ().

Discussion

Early life adversity has well-known deleterious effects on brain development, emotional behavior, and stress reactivity (Bos et al., Citation2011; Sanchez et al., Citation2001; Sheridan et al., Citation2012), although certain individuals appear to be particularly vulnerable to these effects (Caspi et al., Citation2003; Petersen et al., Citation2012; Veenstra-Vanderweele et al., Citation2004). The present study shows that rats genetically predisposed to a high anxiety/depressive-like phenotype (bLRs) are vulnerable to early life MS stress, leading to enhanced physiological responses to stress, in terms of exaggerated HPA axis stress responsiveness, and greater stress-induced defecation. bHR rats, on the other hand – naturally prone to high novelty-seeking, aggression, impulsivity, addiction, low anxiety and depression – are largely unaffected by the early-life MS experience.

Individual differences in MS stress vulnerability

Our findings support previous reports of bLRs’ vulnerability to chronic stress. A 4-week chronic mild stress paradigm enhanced bLRs’ anxiety- and depressive-like behavior and elicited cardiovascular abnormalities reminiscent of those observed in depressed humans (Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011). Likewise, prenatal stress exacerbated bLRs’ anxiety-like behavior and increased their HPA axis responsiveness (Clinton et al., Citation2008). In both cases, bHRs were minimally affected by chronic stress. Here we found that early-life MS selectively impacted physiological parameters in bLR offspring (i.e. increasing stress-induced defecation and HPA stress axis reactivity) without impacting classic anxiety- and depression-like behavioral measures. It is important to note that while we examined bHR/bLR MS and control animals in multiple tests of anxiety-like behavior (i.e. the OF test, EPM, and LDB), we only examined possible depression-like behavior effects in the FST, which may have limited our ability to detect effects of MS on depression-like measures. Indeed, inclusion of tests to examine additional depression-like behavioral parameters, such as learned helplessness, novelty-suppressed feeding, sucrose preference or sexual interest (Cryan & Leonard, Citation2010), may have yielded additional findings and should be considered in future studies.

Our results are also consistent with prior studies evaluating the effects of MS in other rodent models of a high- or low-anxiety phenotype (Neumann et al., Citation2005; Sterley et al., Citation2011). For example, MS stress had opposing effects on Wistar rats selectively-bred for either high anxiety behavior (HAB) or low anxiety behavior (LAB) in the EPM. MS stress reduced HAB rats’ anxiety behavior and neuroendocrine stress responsiveness, but increased anxiety in LAB rats (Neumann et al., Citation2005). Another study found that Wistar-Kyoto rats, a common rat depression model (Braw et al., Citation2006; Dugovic et al., Citation2000; Pardon et al., Citation2002; Pare, Citation1989; Redei et al., Citation1994), were susceptible to MS while control animals, Spontaneously-Hypertensive (SHR) rats, were resistant (Sterley et al., Citation2011).

As noted above, MS stress selectively impacted bLRs’ physiological responses to stress while failing to change bLR or bHR behavior. Regardless of MS condition, bHRs consistently showed high novelty exploration, low anxiety/depression-like behavior relative to bLR groups. This is not surprising considering the strong genetic underpinning of the bHR/bLR traits (Stead et al., Citation2006). Moreover, considering bLR-control animals’ already high levels of anxiety-like behavior (i.e. spending little time in the center of the open field, open arms of the EPM or light compartment of the LDB), there may be a floor-effect that obscures our ability to see a possible exacerbation of bLRs’ anxiety-like behavior following MS stress exposure (Aisa et al., Citation2009; Franklin et al., Citation2010; Huot et al., Citation2001; Ladd et al., Citation2000; Roth et al., Citation2009). Modification of our test conditions (or perhaps selection of another anxiety test) may have offered a milder anxiogenic stimulus that could potentially enable us to see an effect of MS on bLRs’ behavior.

Nevertheless, the fecal boli data from both the Open Field Test and Elevated Plus maze indicate that MS enhances bLRs’ physiological response to stress above and beyond the already high levels of stress-induced defecation exhibited by bLR controls.

MS enhanced stress-induced defecation in bLR offspring

The MS paradigm offers a tool to explore functional interactions between the brain, HPA axis, and gut, and has therefore become a popular model for gastric disorders like Irritable Bowel Syndrome (Schmidt et al., Citation2011). Some previous studies (Daniels et al., Citation2004; Georgel et al., Citation2003), but not all (Savignac et al., Citation2011; Young et al., Citation2005), report exaggerated stress-induced defecation in adult animals subjected to MS. Our findings support this notion, showing that MS enhances defecation particularly in individuals genetically predisposed to high anxiety (i.e. bLRs). Indeed, even at baseline, control bLRs exhibited far greater stress-induced defecation than bHRs, suggesting that relative to bHR, they already have aberrant gut activity in the absence of MS stress. These findings are consistent with a fairly robust literature showing that exaggerated stress-induced defecation is a common feature in animal models of high anxiety/depression (e.g. the Maudsley Reactive rat (Abel et al., Citation1992; Berrettini et al., Citation1994; Fournier et al., Citation2002; Paterson et al., Citation2001); Roman High Avoidance Rat (Ferre et al., Citation1995); the WKY rat (Broide et al., Citation2007); and others (Beneyto & Meador-Woodruff, Citation2008; Beneyto et al., Citation2007; Hashimoto et al., Citation2009)). Furthermore, this work is further supported by a long-standing theory that defecation in the OF and other behavioral tests is an important indicator of emotionality, with greater defecation correlating with higher levels of emotional distress (Archer, Citation1973; Walsh & Cummins, Citation1976). Descending pre-autonomic circuits control visceral motor outflow, including gastrointestinal responses to stress, and a recent study showed that daily 15-min MS enhances gastric preautonomic circuits originating in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), a key node in the HPA stress axis (Banihashemi & Rinaman, Citation2010). This suggests that 180-min daily MS may also re-wire brain-gut circuits to elicit gastric dysfunction (Coutinho et al., Citation2002; Gareau et al., Citation2007; Schwetz et al., Citation2005; Soderholm et al., Citation2002). It would be interesting to interrogate these circuits in bHR/bLR rats at baseline and following 3-h MS stress given that the groups differ in both conditions.

MS enhanced HPA axis function in bLRs

Numerous studies show that MS stress produces lifelong changes in the HPA axis, including exaggerated neuroendocrine response to acute stress (Bird, Citation2002; Daniels et al., Citation2009; Huot et al., Citation2004; Plotsky & Meaney, Citation1993; Rosenfeld et al., Citation1992; Smith et al., Citation2011; Waterland, Citation2006), altered CRF and glucocorticoid receptor expression (Bravo et al., Citation2011; Meaney et al., Citation1996), and increased noradrenergic input to the PVN (Wang et al., Citation2010). Neuroendocrine responsivity in bHR rats was only subtly affected by MS, with MS-exposed bHRs showing diminished ACTH secretion following the LDB test compared to control bHRs and no differences in CORT secretion. On the other hand, MS-bLRs exhibited HPA axis abnormalities reminiscent of previously published results, with MS-bLRs showing exaggerated ACTH and CORT secretion following the LBD test. These disparate findings are not altogether surprising considering bHR/bLR baseline HPA axis reactivity differences (; Clinton et al., Citation2008; Kabbaj et al., Citation2000; Piazza et al., Citation1991, Citation1993; Rouge-Pont et al., Citation1998). Compared to bLRs, bHRs exhibit an exaggerated HPA axis response to acute stress (as in ). Furthermore, earlier work showed that high novelty-seeking rats find elevated corticosterone levels rewarding, which may contribute to their “thrill seeking”/risk-taking temperament (Piazza et al., Citation1993). Several neurobiological factors likely contribute to these effects, including different expression levels of glucocorticoid receptors and corticotrophic releasing hormone (CRH) (Kabbaj et al., Citation2000; Rouge-Pont et al., Citation1998), and altered mineralocorticoid/glucocorticoid receptor balance (Kerman et al., Citation2012). If bHR/bLR rats exhibit fundamental differences in structure and function of the HPA axis, it reasonably follows that MS may differentially impact the stress circuit in each phenotype (Biagini et al., Citation1998; Veenstra-Vanderweele & Cook, Citation2004).

Considering the effect of MS on maternal care in bHR/bLR dams

The quality and quantity of maternal care critically shapes rodent neurodevelopment, emotional behavior, and hormonal stress responsivity (Caldji et al., Citation1998; Francis et al., Citation1999; Liu et al., Citation1997, Citation2000; Meaney, Citation2001; Wang et al., Citation2010). Moreover, MS (like other postnatal manipulations) influences maternal care, suggesting that MS stress exerts its effect, in part, via subtle behavioral or stress hormone changes in dams when they reunite with litters (Huot et al., Citation2004; Macri et al., Citation2011). We previously reported that bLR mothers are more maternal (showing more licking, grooming, arched back nursing) with pups compared to bHR mothers. Although bHR dams exhibited less maternal behavior than bLRs during the dark/active phase, they were very attentive to pups during the light phase, spending greater time passive nursing and in pup contact compared to bLR dams (Clinton et al., Citation2007; Kristiansen et al., Citation2007). These findings concur with observed HAB/LAB maternal differences, where highly anxious HAB mothers attend more to pups compared to LAB dams (Bosch, Citation2011; Bosch & Neumann, Citation2008). One caveat of the present study was that we did not evaluate bHR/bLR maternal behavior in MS and control litters. Neumann and colleagues reported that HAB/LAB maternal differences persisted throughout the MS process, and that the two lines acted similarly upon reunion with their litters (Neumann et al., Citation2005). It would be quite interesting to know (a) whether bHR and bLR mothers differentially react to the MS procedure; and (b) to what extent their reactions may contribute to MS effects in adult offspring (which could potentially be explored in a cross-fostering experiment). Future experiments are required to explore these new questions.

Conclusions

We believe that the bLR rat represents a useful new rodent model of co-morbid anxiety- and depression given their naturally high levels of anxiety- and depressive-like behavior (Clinton et al., Citation2008, Citation2011a; Flagel et al., Citation2010; Garcia-Fuster et al., Citation2012; Kabbaj et al., Citation2000; Perez et al., Citation2009; Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011; White et al., Citation2007), diminished aggression (Kerman et al., Citation2011) and sexual motivation (McCullumsmith et al., Citation2007), and reduced responsivity to reward (Cummings et al., Citation2011; Davis et al., Citation2008; Flagel et al., Citation2011; Garcia-Fuster et al., Citation2010; Kabbaj, Citation2004; Piazza et al., Citation1989). The present study together with our earlier work (Clinton et al., Citation2008; Stedenfeld et al., Citation2011) also demonstrates high vulnerability of bLRs to chronic stress, whereas bHRs are fairly resilient. MS during the first two postnatal weeks appears to exacerbate adult bLRs’ physiological response to stress, increasing their already high levels of stress-induced defecation and also enhancing HPA axis responsivity. Our previous studies point to differences in the ontogeny of hippocampal circuits in bLR versus bHR rats as a possible neurobiological underpinning for their marked behavioral and neuroendocrine phenotypes (Clinton et al., Citation2011b). Ongoing studies will determine whether the deleterious effects of MS on bLR offspring are associated with changes within the hippocampus and other limbic regions.

Declaration of interest

The study was funded by NIMH R00 MH085859-03 (SMC), Office of Naval Research ONR- N00014-09-1-0598 (HA and SJW), NIDA PPG 5P01DA021633-02 (HA and SJW) and a Hope for Depression Research Foundation (HDRF) grant (HA). There are no biomedical financial interests or conflicts of interest for any of the authors.

Supplemental material

Supplementary Material

Download PDF (29.4 KB)

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Sue Miller, Antony Abraham, and Tracy Bedrosian for excellent technical assistance.

References

  • Abel EL, Altman HJ, Commissaris RL. (1992). Maudsley reactive and nonreactive rats in the forced swim test: comparison in fresh water and soiled water. Physiol Behav 52:1117–19
  • Aisa B, Elizalde N, Tordera R, Lasheras B, Del Rio J, Ramirez MJ. (2009). Effects of neonatal stress on markers of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus: implications for spatial memory. Hippocampus 19:1222–31
  • Archer J. (1973). Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: a review. Anim Behav 21:205–35
  • Banihashemi L, Rinaman L. (2010). Repeated brief postnatal maternal separation enhances hypothalamic gastric autonomic circuits in juvenile rats. Neuroscience 165(1):265--77
  • Beneyto M, Kristiansen LV, Oni-Orisan A, McCullumsmith RE, Meador-Woodruff JH. (2007). Abnormal glutamate receptor expression in the medial temporal lobe in schizophrenia and mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:1888–902
  • Beneyto M, Meador-Woodruff JH. (2008). Lamina-specific abnormalities of NMDA receptor-associated postsynaptic protein transcripts in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:2175–86
  • Berrettini WH, Harris N, Ferraro TN, Vogel WH. (1994). Maudsley reactive and non-reactive rats differ in exploratory behavior but not in learning. Psychiatric Genetics 4:91–4
  • Biagini G, Pich EM, Carani C, Marrama P, Agnati LF. (1998). Postnatal maternal separation during the stress hyporesponsive period enhances the adrenocortical response to novelty in adult rats by affecting feedback regulation in the CA1 hippocampal field. Int J Dev Neurosci 16:187–97
  • Bird A. (2002). DNA methylation patterns and epigenetic memory. Genes Dev 16:6–21
  • Bos K, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Drury SS, Mclaughlin KA, Nelson CA. (2011). Psychiatric outcomes in young children with a history of institutionalization. Harvard Rev Psychiatry 19:15–24
  • Bosch OJ. (2011). Maternal nurturing is dependent on her innate anxiety: the behavioral roles of brain oxytocin and vasopressin. Hormones Behav 59:202–12
  • Bosch OJ, Neumann ID. (2008). Brain vasopressin is an important regulator of maternal behavior independent of dams' trait anxiety. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:17139–44
  • Bravo JA, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. (2011). Alterations in the central CRF system of two different rat models of comorbid depression and functional gastrointestinal disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 14:666–83
  • Braw Y, Malkesman O, Merlender A, Bercovich A, Dagan M, Maayan R, Weizman A, Weller A. (2006). Stress hormones and emotion-regulation in two genetic animal models of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31:1105–16
  • Broide RS, Redwine JM, Aftahi N, Young W, Bloom FE, Winrow CJ. (2007). Distribution of histone deacetylases 1–11 in the rat brain. J Mol Neurosci 31:47–58
  • Caldji C, Tannenbaum B, Sharma S, Francis D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:5335–40
  • Card JP, Levitt P, Gluhovsky M, Rinaman L. (2005). Early experience modifies the postnatal assembly of autonomic emotional motor circuits in rats. J Neurosci 25:9102–11
  • Caspi A, Mcclay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297:851–4
  • Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, Mcclay J, et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–9
  • Clinton S, Miller S, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2008). Prenatal stress does not alter innate novelty-seeking behavioral traits, but differentially affects individual differences in neuroendocrine stress responsivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33:162–77
  • Clinton SM, Kerman IA, Orr HR, Bedrosian TA, Abraham AD, Simpson DN, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2011a). Pattern of forebrain activation in high novelty-seeking rats following aggressive encounter. Brain Res 1422:20–31
  • Clinton SM, Stead JD, Miller S, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2011b). Developmental underpinnings of differences in rodent novelty-seeking and emotional reactivity. Eur J Neurosci 34:994–1005
  • Clinton SM, Vazquez DM, Kabbaj M, Kabbaj MH, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2007). Individual differences in novelty-seeking and emotional reactivity correlate with variation in maternal behavior. Horm Behav 51:655–64
  • Coutinho SV, Plotsky PM, Sablad M, Miller JC, Zhou H, Bayati AI, Mcroberts JA, Mayer EA. (2002). Neonatal maternal separation alters stress-induced responses to viscerosomatic nociceptive stimuli in rat. Am J Physiol Gastrointestinal Liver Physiol 282:G307–16
  • Cryan JF, Leonard BE. (2010). Depression: from psychopathology to pharmacotherapy. Basel, New York: Karger
  • Cryan JF, Valentino RJ, Lucki I. (2005). Assessing substrates underlying the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neurosci Biobehav Revs 29:547–69
  • Cummings JA, Gowl BA, Westenbroek C, Clinton SM, Akil H, Becker JB. (2011). Effects of a selectively bred novelty-seeking phenotype on the motivation to take cocaine in male and female rats. Biol Sex Differ 2:3
  • Daniels WM, Fairbairn LR, Van Tilburg G, Mcevoy CR, Zigmond MJ, Russell VA, Stein DJ. (2009). Maternal separation alters nerve growth factor and corticosterone levels but not the DNA methylation status of the exon 1(7) glucocorticoid receptor promoter region. Metab Brain Dis 24:615–27
  • Daniels WM, Pietersen CY, Carstens ME, Stein DJ. (2004). Maternal separation in rats leads to anxiety-like behavior and a blunted ACTH response and altered neurotransmitter levels in response to a subsequent stressor. Metab Brain Dis 19:3–14
  • Davis BA, Clinton SM, Akil H, Becker JB. (2008). The effects of novelty-seeking phenotypes and sex differences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in selectively bred High-Responder and Low-Responder rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 90:331–8
  • Dugovic C, Solberg LC, Redei E, Van Reeth O, Turek FW. (2000). Sleep in the Wistar-Kyoto rat, a putative genetic animal model for depression. Neuroreport 11:627–31
  • Everett JW, Sawyer CH. (1949). A neural timing factor in the mechanism by which progesterone advances ovulation in the cyclic rat. Endocrinology 45:581–95, illust
  • Ferre P, Fernandez-Teruel A, Escorihuela RM, Driscoll P, Corda MG, Giorgi O, Tobena A. (1995). Behavior of the Roman/Verh high- and low-avoidance rat lines in anxiety tests: relationship with defecation and self-grooming. Physiol Behav 58:1209–13
  • Flagel SB, Clark JJ, Robinson TE, Mayo L, Czuj A, Willuhn I, Akers CA, et al. (2011). A selective role for dopamine in stimulus-reward learning. Nature 469:53–7
  • Flagel SB, Robinson TE, Clark JJ, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, Seeman P, Phillips PE, Akil H. (2010). An animal model of genetic vulnerability to behavioral disinhibition and responsiveness to reward-related cues: implications for addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:388–400
  • Fournier I, Ploye F, Cottet-Emard JM, Brun J, Claustrat B. (2002). Folate deficiency alters melatonin secretion in rats. J Nutr 132:2781–4
  • Francis DD, Champagne FA, Liu D, Meaney MJ. (1999). Maternal care, gene expression, and the development of individual differences in stress reactivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 896:66–84
  • Franklin TB, Russig H, Weiss IC, Graff J, Linder N, Michalon A, Vizi S, Mansuy IM. (2010). Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biol Psychiatry 68:408–15
  • Garcia-Fuster MJ, Parks GS, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, Akil H, Civelli O. (2012). The melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) system in an animal model of depression-like behavior. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 22:607–13
  • Garcia-Fuster MJ, Perez JA, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2010). Impact of cocaine on adult hippocampal neurogenesis in an animal model of differential propensity to drug abuse. Eur J Neurosci 31:79–89
  • Gareau MG, Jury J, Macqueen G, Sherman PM, Perdue MH. (2007). Probiotic treatment of rat pups normalises corticosterone release and ameliorates colonic dysfunction induced by maternal separation. Gut 56:1522–8
  • Georgel PT, Horowitz-Scherer RA, Adkins N, Woodcock CL, Wade PA, Hansen JC. (2003). Chromatin compaction by human MeCP2. Assembly of novel secondary chromatin structures in the absence of DNA methylation. J Biol Chem 278:32181–8
  • Hashimoto T, Nguyen QL, Rotaru D, Keenan T, Arion D, Beneyto M, Gonzalez-Burgos G, Lewis DA. (2009). Protracted developmental trajectories of GABAA receptor alpha1 and alpha2 subunit expression in primate prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry 65:1015–23
  • Heim C, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. (2004). Importance of studying the contributions of early adverse experience to neurobiological findings in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:641–8
  • Holmes A, Le Guisquet AM, Vogel E, Millstein RA, Leman S, Belzung C. (2005). Early life genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors shaping emotionality in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:1335–46
  • Hulshof HJ, Novati A, Sgoifo A, Luiten PG, Den Boer JA, Meerlo P. (2011). Maternal separation decreases adult hippocampal cell proliferation and impairs cognitive performance but has little effect on stress sensitivity and anxiety in adult Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res 216:552–60
  • Huot RL, Gonzalez ME, Ladd CO, Thrivikraman KV, Plotsky PM. (2004). Foster litters prevent hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis sensitization mediated by neonatal maternal separation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29:279–89
  • Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM. (2001). Development of adult ethanol preference and anxiety as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long Evans rats and reversal with antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 158:366–73
  • Kabbaj M. (2004). Neurobiological bases of individual differences in emotional and stress responsiveness: high responders-low responders model. Arch Neurol 61:1009–12
  • Kabbaj M, Devine DP, Savage VR, Akil H. (2000). Neurobiological correlates of individual differences in novelty-seeking behavior in the rat: differential expression of stress-related molecules. J Neurosci 20:6983–8
  • Kerman IA, Clinton SM, Bedrosian TA, Abraham AD, Rosenthal DT, Akil H, Watson SJ. (2011). High novelty-seeking predicts aggression and gene expression differences within defined serotonergic cell groups. Brain Res 1419:34–45
  • Kerman IA, Clinton SM, Simpson DN, Bedrosian TA, Bernard R, Akil H, Watson SJ. (2012). Inborn differences in environmental reactivity predict divergent diurnal behavioral, endocrine, and gene expression rhythms. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:256–69
  • Kristiansen LV, Huerta I, Beneyto M, Meador-Woodruff JH. (2007). NMDA receptors and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Pharmacol 7:48–55
  • Ladd CO, Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM. (2000). Long-term behavioral and neuroendocrine adaptations to adverse early experience. Progr Brain Res 122:81–103
  • Lehmann J, Feldon J. (2000). Long-term biobehavioral effects of maternal separation in the rat: consistent or confusing? Rev Neurosci 11:383–408
  • Lehmann J, Stohr T, Feldon J. (2000). Long-term effects of prenatal stress experiences and postnatal maternal separation on emotionality and attentional processes. Behav Brain Res 107:133–44
  • Liu D, Diorio J, Day JC, Francis DD, Meaney MJ. (2000). Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nat Neuroscie 3:799–806
  • Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Sharma S, et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 277:1659–62
  • Macri S, Zoratto F, Laviola G. (2011). Early-stress regulates resilience, vulnerability and experimental validity in laboratory rodents through mother-offspring hormonal transfer. Neurosci Biobehav Revi 35:1534–43
  • McCullumsmith RE, Gupta D, Beneyto M, Kreger E, Haroutunian V, Davis KL, Meador-Woodruff JH. (2007). Expression of transcripts for myelination-related genes in the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Resh 90:15–27
  • Meaney MJ. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Ann Rev Neurosci 24:1161–92
  • Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Widdowson J, Laplante P, Caldji C, Sharma S, et al. (1996). Early environmental regulation of forebrain glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: implications for adrenocortical responses to stress. Dev Neurosci 18:49–72
  • Neumann ID, Wigger A, Kromer S, Frank E, Landgraf R, Bosch OJ. (2005). Differential effects of periodic maternal separation on adult stress coping in a rat model of extremes in trait anxiety. Neuroscience 132:867–77
  • Pardon MC, Gould GG, Garcia A, Phillips L, Cook MC, Miller SA, Mason PA, Morilak DA. (2002). Stress reactivity of the brain noradrenergic system in three rat strains differing in their neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress: implications for susceptibility to stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Neuroscience 115:229–42
  • Pare WP. (1989). Stress ulcer susceptibility and depression in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. Physiol Behav 46:993–8
  • Paterson A, Whiting PJ, Gray JA, Flint J, Dawson GR. (2001). Lack of consistent behavioural effects of Maudsley reactive and non-reactive rats in a number of animal tests of anxiety and activity. Psychopharmacology 154:336–42
  • Perez JA, Clinton SM, Turner CA, Watson SJ, Akil H. (2009). A new role for FGF2 as an endogenous inhibitor of anxiety. J Neurosci 29:6379–87
  • Petersen IT, Bates JE, Goodnight JA, Dodge KA, Lansford JE, Pettit GS, Latendresse SJ, Dick DM. (2012). Interaction between serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and stressful life events in adolescents' trajectories of anxious/depressed symptoms. Dev Psychol 48:1463–75
  • Piazza PV, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Simon H. (1989). Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science 245:1511–13
  • Piazza PV, Deroche V, Deminiere JM, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H. (1993). Corticosterone in the range of stress-induced levels possesses reinforcing properties: implications for sensation-seeking behaviors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:11738–42
  • Piazza PV, Maccari S, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Mormede P, Simon H. (1991). Corticosterone levels determine individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88:2088–92
  • Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. (1993). Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 18:195–200
  • Plotsky PM, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB. (1998). Psychoneuroendocrinology of depression. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychiatric Clin North Am 21:293–307
  • Redei E, Pare WP, Aird F, Kluczynski J. (1994). Strain differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity and stress ulcer. Am J Physiol 266:R353–60
  • Rosenfeld P, Wetmore JB, Levine S. (1992). Effects of repeated maternal separations on the adrenocortical response to stress of preweanling rats. Physiol Behavr 52:787–91
  • Roth TL, Lubin FD, Funk AJ, Sweatt JD. (2009). Lasting epigenetic influence of early-life adversity on the BDNF gene. Biol Psychiatry 65:760–9
  • Rouge-Pont F, Deroche V, Le Moal M, Piazza PV. (1998). Individual differences in stress-induced dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens are influenced by corticosterone. Eur J Neurosci 10:3903–7
  • Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev Psychopathol 13:419–49
  • Savignac HM, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. (2011). Resistance to early-life stress in mice: effects of genetic background and stress duration. Frontiers Behav Neurosci 5:13
  • Schmidt RJ, Hansen RL, Hartiala J, Allayee H, Schmidt LC, Tancredi DJ, Tassone F, Hertz-Picciotto I. (2011). Prenatal vitamins, one-carbon metabolism gene variants, and risk for autism. Epidemiology 22:476–85
  • Schwetz I, Mcroberts JA, Coutinho SV, Bradesi S, Gale G, Fanselow M, Million M, et al. (2005). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 mediates acute and delayed stress-induced visceral hyperalgesia in maternally separated Long-Evans rats. Am J Physiol. Gastrointestinal Liver Physiol 289:G704–12
  • Sheridan MA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Mclaughlin KA, Nelson CA 3rd. (2012). Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:12927–32
  • Smith GC, Konycheva G, Dziadek MA, Ravelich SR, Patel S, Reddy S, Breier BH, et al. (2011). Pre- and postnatal methyl deficiency in the rat differentially alters glucose homeostasis. J Nutrigenet Nutrigenom 4:175–91
  • Soderholm JD, Yates DA, Gareau MG, Yang PC, Macqueen G, Perdue MH. (2002). Neonatal maternal separation predisposes adult rats to colonic barrier dysfunction in response to mild stress. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 283:G1257–63
  • Stead JD, Clinton S, Neal C, Schneider J, Jama A, Miller S, Vazquez DM, et al. (2006). Selective breeding for divergence in novelty-seeking traits: heritability and enrichment in spontaneous anxiety-related behaviors. Behav Genet 36:697–712
  • Stedenfeld KA, Clinton SM, Kerman IA, Akil H, Watson SJ, Sved AF. (2011). Novelty-seeking behavior predicts vulnerability in a rodent model of depression. Physiol Behav 103:210–16
  • Sterley TL, Howells FM, Russell VA. (2011). Effects of early life trauma are dependent on genetic predisposition: a rat study. Behav Brain Funct 7:11
  • Turner CA, Clinton SM, Thompson RC, Watson SJ, Jr Akil H. (2011). Fibroblast growth factor-2 (FGF2) augmentation early in life alters hippocampal development and rescues the anxiety phenotype in vulnerable animals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:8021–5
  • Veenstra-Vanderweele J, Christian SL, Cook EH Jr. (2004). Autism as a paradigmatic complex genetic disorder. Annual Rev Genom Human Genets 5:379–405
  • Veenstra-Vanderweele J, Cook EH Jr. (2004). Molecular genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry 9:819–32
  • Walsh RN, Cummins RA. (1976). The open-field test: a critical review. Psychol Bull 83:482–504
  • Wang Z, Xu L, Zhu X, Cui W, Sun Y, Nishijo H, Peng Y, Li R. (2010). Demethylation of specific Wnt/beta-catenin pathway genes and its upregulation in rat brain induced by prenatal valproate exposure. Anatomical Record 293:1947–53
  • Waterland RA. (2006). Assessing the effects of high methionine intake on DNA methylation. J Nutr 136:1706S–10S
  • White DA, Kalinichev M, Holtzman SG. (2007). Locomotor response to novelty as a predictor of reactivity to aversive stimuli in the rat. Brain Res 1149:141–8
  • Ying SY, Gove S, Fang VS, Greep RO. (1973). Ovulation in postpartum rats. Endocrinology 92:108–16
  • Young JI, Hong EP, Castle JC, Crespo-Barreto J, Bowman AB, Rose MF, Kang D, et al. (2005). Regulation of RNA splicing by the methylation-dependent transcriptional repressor methyl-CpG binding protein 2. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:17551–8

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.