8,394
Views
39
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Clinical Research Articles

Losing a child: finding meaning in bereavement

&
Article: 22910 | Received 25 Sep 2013, Accepted 01 Mar 2014, Published online: 31 Mar 2014

Abstract

Background

Confronting the loss of a loved one leads us to the core questions of human existence. Bereaved parents have to deal with the rupture of a widely shared concept of what is perceived to be the natural course of life and are forced into meaning reconstruction.

Objective

This study aims to expand upon existing work concerning specific themes of meaning reconstruction in a sample of bereaved parents. More specifically, the relationship between meaning reconstruction, complicated grief, and posttraumatic growth was analyzed, with special attention focused on traumatic and unexpected losses.

Method

In a mixed methods approach, themes of meaning reconstruction (sense-making and benefit-finding) were assessed in in-depth interviews with a total of 30 bereaved parents. Posttraumatic growth and complicated grief were assessed using standardized questionnaires, and qualitative and quantitative results were then merged using data transformation methods.

Results

In total 42 themes of meaning reconstruction were abstracted from oral material. It was shown that sense-making themes ranged from causal explanations to complex philosophical beliefs about life and death. Benefit-finding themes contained thoughts about personal improvement as well as descriptions about social actions. Significant correlations were found between the extent of sense-making and posttraumatic growth scores (r s=0.54, r s=0.49; p<0.01), especially when the death was traumatic or unexpected (r s=0.67, r s=0.63; p<0.01). However, analysis revealed no significant correlation with complicated grief. Overall results corroborate meaning reconstruction themes and the importance of meaning reconstruction for posttraumatic growth.

For the abstract or full text in other languages, please see Supplementary files under Article Tools online

The loss of a child is one of the most devastating experiences for a parent and is associated with diverse maladaptive developments (Znoj, Citation2004). Bereaved parents are at risk of anxiety disorders, depression (Kreicbergs, Valdimarsdóttir, Onelöv, Henter, & Steineck, Citation2004; Rogers, Floyd, Seltzer, Greenberg, & Hong, Citation2008), complicated grief (Kersting, Brähler, Glaesmer, & Wagner, Citation2011), psychiatric hospitalization (Li, Laursen, Precht, Olsen, & Mortensen, Citation2005), and mortality (Li, Precht, Mortensen, & Olsen, Citation2003). Despite these alarming results, research dealing with bereaved parents is still rare (Rogers et al., Citation2008).

Losing a child is recognized as especially disruptive and challenging to the expected order of life events: The young ought to outlive the old. This inconsistency of the loss with the pre-loss worldview brings upon fundamental questions of meaning in life (Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Larson, Citation1998; Keesee, Currier, & Neimeyer, Citation2008; Lichtenthal, Currier, Neimeyer, & Keesee, Citation2010; Murphy, Johnson, & Lohan, Citation2003; Polatinsky & Esprey, Citation2000; Wheeler, Citation2001). Hence, a process of finding and integrating meaning of the loss and its consequences into one's worldview takes place. Neimeyer (Citation2000) defines this “meaning reconstruction,” as sense-making, benefit-finding, and identity-change (Gillies & Neimeyer, Citation2006).

Sense-making refers to those thinking processes engaged in to understand the loss by incorporating it into a personal worldview, for example, by understanding the cause of death. Benefit-finding refers to the process of discovering positive consequences in the face of adversity, like increased compassion (Davis et al., Citation1998). In the current study, identity-change is conceptualized as an outcome of meaning reconstruction rather than a process itself. This goes along with the majority of authors who conceptualize the positive changes in one's identity in the aftermath of adversity as posttraumatic growth (Tedeschi & Calhoun, Citation1996).

Similar to the concept of meaning reconstruction, the “Model of Growth in the Context of Grief” (Calhoun, Tedeschi, Cann, & Hanks, Citation2010) states that inconsistency of the loss with pre-loss worldviews triggers intense cognitive work to reconstruct a new worldview incorporating the loss in a meaningful way (Calhoun et al., Citation2010; Cann et al., Citation2010). Thus, meaning reconstruction describes a process and posttraumatic growth describes an outcome, both concepts concern the integration of the loss (Calhoun et al., Citation2010; Gillies & Neimeyer, Citation2006). Therefore, a positive relationship between meaning reconstruction and posttraumatic growth would be expected; however studies show heterogeneous results: Some studies showed that benefit-finding is correlated with posttraumatic growth, whereas thoughts about the meaning of life (part of the sense-making process) are not necessarily (Cadell, Regehr, & Hemsworth, Citation2003; Engelkemeyer & Marwit, Citation2008; Hogan & Schmidt, Citation2002). More recent research suggests that while the presence of meaning in life is associated with posttraumatic growth (Linley & Joseph, Citation2011; Triplett, Tedeschi, Cann, Calhoun, & Reeve, Citation2012), the search for meaning is associated with negative change (Linley & Joseph, Citation2011). However, to date research analyzing the relations of the two concepts is scarce, and often only parts of meaning reconstruction are analyzed.

As stated above, bereaved parents have a high risk of complicated grief, especially those bereaved by traumatic causes (Keesee et al., Citation2008; Wijngaards-de Meij et al., Citation2005). Complicated grief is characterized by symptoms of intense grief persisting for more than 6 months (Prigerson et al., Citation1995). In contrast, individuals with non-complicated grief gradually adapt to the reality of the loss and over time the intensity of grief declines (Znoj, Citation2004). Adaptation is possible through integration of the loss and its consequences into the autobiographic memory and the worldview. That is meaning reconstruction. But then the intense emotional distress of individuals with complicated grief hinders the cognitive work required for meaning reconstruction (Boelen, van den Hout, & van den Bout, Citation2006). Recent empirical work supports the notion that the inability to find any meaning in a loss is associated with distress and complicated grief (Bonanno et al., Citation2007; Davis et al., Citation1998; Keesee et al., Citation2008; Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010), whereas positive meaning reconstruction is negatively correlated with complicated grief (Currier, Holland, & Neimeyer, Citation2006).

With a mixed methods study, Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) identified specific themes of meaning reconstruction in a sample of bereaved parents with qualitative methods. The present study aims to scrutinize and expand these. Further, associations of meaning reconstruction with complicated grief and posttraumatic growth are analyzed, drawing special attention to violent and unexpected losses.

Method

Procedures and participants

Bereaved parents were invited to participate in our study via bereavement support groups and psychotherapists. Study aims and procedures were described, and contact details were submitted in the form of a letter. Study inclusion required that the time since the loss occurred exceeded a year, given that meaning reconstruction (Murphy et al., Citation2003) as well as posttraumatic growth (Taku, Calhoun, Cann, & Tedeschi, Citation2008; Taku, Cann, Tedeschi, & Calhoun, Citation2009) and the development of complicated grief (Prigerson et al., Citation2009) take time. Furthermore, to account for the fact that perinatal bereavement poses a different challenge in meaning reconstruction (Uren & Wastell, Citation2002), the deceased child had to have been born rather than died in the womb. A total sample comprised N=31 bereaved parents. Two participants decided to answer the interview questions in writing instead of orally. Among these two, one participant had to be excluded due to missing data. Another participant was box-plot identified as an outlier and therefore remained excluded from quantitative analysis exclusively.

The mean age was 51.0 years (SD=11.5 years; range=29–72 years). There were 21 (70%) women and nine (30%) men. Of the 30 participants, most were married (66.7%), some divorced (13.3%), 6.7% were single, 13.3% in a relationship and none were widowed. More than half of the participants (N=18) stemmed from the same parental dyad. Fifty percent of the parents reported that their child died of an illness, 30% had lost their child through an accident, 16.7% were bereaved due to suicide and 3.3% by homicide. Consequently, 50% of the deaths were coded as traumatic, and 50% as non-traumatic (objective measure). Nonetheless, only 16.7% of the parents reported having subjectively anticipated the loss; most (83.3%) did not (subjective measure). The mean length of time since the loss had occurred was 9.73 years (SD=7.8; range=1–26). Participants from the same (M=12, SD=8.06) and participants from different parental dyads (M=6.09, SD=6.55) differentiated between the number of years since occurrence of the loss. The mean age of the child at the time of death was 10.2 years (SD=9.4; range=4 days to 40 years).

The study was conducted according to specific ethical advice for clinical research with bereaved parents (Dyregrov, Citation2004; Hynson, Aroni, Bauld, & Sawyer, Citation2006). All participants provided written informed consent.

Measures

Meaning reconstruction was assessed with a guided interview developed for this study, extending the approach of earlier research (e.g., Lehman, Wortman, & Williams, Citation1987; Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010; McIntosh, Silver, & Wortman, Citation1993) and building on the definitions from Davis et al. (Citation1998). The resulting questions assessing sense-making are the following: (1) Have you been able to make sense of the loss?; (2) Have you questioned the meaning of life as a result of the loss?; and (3) Has your worldview changed since the loss? Benefit-finding was assessed with the following questions: (1) What does the death of your child mean for you and your life?; (2) Has your aim in life changed since the loss?; and (3) Do you think that it is possible to discover positive consequences even through loss? Please describe your personal experiences.

Posttraumatic growth was assessed with the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI; Tedeschi & Calhoun, Citation1996), in German translation (Maercker & Langer, Citation2001). The PTGI measured positive changes among individuals who have experienced a highly stressful event. With 21 items, the PTGI measured five domain sub-scales of positive changes: Relating to Others, New Possibilities, Personal Strength, Spiritual Change, and Appreciation of Life on a scale ranging from 0 (did not experience) to 5 (experienced to a very great degree). Internal consistency was estimated at 0.90; test–retest reliability at a 2-month interval is 0.71 (Tedeschi & Calhoun, Citation1996).

Complicated grief was assessed with the shortened version (Maercker & Langner, Citation2002) of the Complicated Grief Module (CGM; Horowitz et al., Citation1997), which assesses symptoms of complicated grief noticed in the last month. It measures intrusion (α=0.88), avoidance (α=0.82), and failure to adapt (α=0.84) on 26 items, which could be evaluated on a four-point Likert-scale ranging from 0 (not true) to 3 (total agreement).

Data analysis

Qualitative analysis

Oral material from conducted interviews has been transcribed using special software (f5 for mac) and subsequently content analyzed (without using specific software) (Mayring, Citation2010). The written material was organized into meaning units [segments of responses with either sense-making or benefit-finding as defined by Davis et al. (Citation1998)]. We identified N=334 independent meaning units across participants’ responses. Analyzing and comparing these meaning units built the basis to: (1) assign meaning units to existing themes (Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010) when definition criteria ( and , “coding definition”) were met; and (2) develop new themes to accurately picture the broad content of participants’ answers. It was determined that a new topic required more than one participant talking about it. Themes which were only mentioned by one participant and not already captured by Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) were classified as “Other theme.” By doing it this way, one coder abstracted 39 content themes. The coder (first author) was double checked and supervised during the whole coding process by the second author. In order to enhance study reliability, Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) were contacted to discuss ambiguities.

Quantitative analysis

All analyses were conducted with SPSS 19.0. Gender differences as well as differences between participants from the same and from different parental dyads were evaluated using χ 2-tests, t-tests, and Mann-Whitney-U-tests. Pearson's r-correlations were computed when all requirements, assessed with Lilliefors-corrected Kolmogorov-Smirnov-test and Levene-statistics, were met, and in all other cases, Spearman-correlations were used. Correlations were examined as one- or two-tailed tests depending on the hypotheses. All non-specified correlation analyses were two-tailed. To account for problems of non-independence, Mann-Whitney-U-tests, t-tests, and χ 2-tests were used to calculate possible differences between participants coming from the same and from different parental dyads.

After analyzing both data-sets independently, the results were merged by means of data transformation (Creswell & Zhang, Citation2009). Qualitative data was, therefore, numerically scaled, exhibiting the degree of sense-making and benefit-finding in numerical order (see ), and establishing a continuous variable.

Table 1 Data transformation

Results

We abstracted 20 sense-making themes, each characterizing ways in which the death was made comprehensible for the parents. The most common responses were thoughts encompassing the purpose of life and death, and were given by 50% of the participants. Answers described the early death in the context of the fulfillment of the child's purpose in life, such as, “He was very early mature, and so I think he was done.” Or the purpose was given as a result of the experience with the child. For example, a mother who lost her disabled daughter stated, “The purpose of why she was here became clear to me. Now, I have the experience to work with disabled children.” Other common responses were biological explanations of the death, stated by 43.3% of the participants, and beliefs about the existence of an afterlife, which were stated by 40% of the participants.

A total of 36.7% of the parents stated that they could not make any sense of the loss; however, most of these participants (86.2%) also mentioned other sense-making themes in the course of the interview. This finding was included in the development of two additional categories, one describing the difficulty of being able, and willing, to understand such a traumatic event despite having some explanation (“Little sense of meaning” 10%). The other category includes the strong will to believe in a sense of meaning, without being able to know or describe it in words (“Belief in a sense of meaning” 6.7%).

Another developed theme encompassed the understanding that one's identity as a parent could not be destroyed by death (“Identity,” 20%), for example, “The fact that I am a mother has not changed, my child is just somewhere else” (see ).

Table 2 Sense-making themes and frequencies (participants often gave multiple answers)

In total, 19 benefit-finding themes emerged in parent's responses, of which the theme of personal improvement was most frequently named (46.7%). Responses concerning personal improvement were multifaceted and included, for example, describing personal growth, having a greater trust in life, being more tolerant, or developing personal potential. As one father stated, “I've learned to live, or rather, discovered a potential in me that I did not have before.” Other frequent themes were the description of changed priorities (43.3%) such as, “It is then no longer work, money, and material things that count, but concentrating on the essentials.”

A total of 36.7% of participants stated that they developed a heightened appreciation of life. A third of the participants (33.3%) described either a desire or concrete activities to help others in need, for example, “As a result of my experiences, I aim to build up a self-help group for parents and siblings.” Thirty percent of the participants also described feeling stronger, less afraid or better able to cope with difficult situations. For example, “I am now less afraid in life, especially concerning my own death.”

This feeling of acquiring a stronger ability to cope might also be a consequence of thinking that nothing worse could possibly happen. Such thoughts were included in the theme “Bitter benefit,” describing positive aspects that cannot be seen as a positive gain (13.3%). Another added theme (“Experience”), emphasized experiences with or following the loss (20% of the parents). For example, “In principle, it is a gift being able to have such an experience.” Some of the participants (10%) described a gain in freedom or time as a consequence of the loss.

A quarter of the participants (24.1%) stated explicitly “No” to the question of benefits or no further positive implications of their loss. At the same time, 57.1% of these parents also mentioned other themes determined as benefit-finding themes. This seeming ambivalence is illustrated by a mother who lost her 19-year-old son, “I think that the price is much too high. I don't want to learn, or mature. Maybe yes, one matures facing such an adversity. But it doesn't justify the death of someone” (see ).

Table 3 Benefit-finding themes and frequencies (participants often gave multiple answers)

Analysis revealed that more women were rating high on the CGM (t=−3.23; p<0.01). There were no gender differences in terms of posttraumatic growth (t=0.14; p=0.89), sense-making (χ 2=0.99; p=0.61), and benefit-finding (χ 2=0.08; p=0.96). A significant negative correlation between age and posttraumatic growth (r=−0.42; p<0.05), unaltered when controlled for the degree of benefit-finding (r=−0.33; p<0.05), but disappearing when controlled for sense-making (r=−0.19; p=0.32), was shown. A significant correlation was also found between age and complicated grief (r (one-tailed)=−0.33; p<0.05), unaltered when controlled for the degree of sense-making (r=−0.32; p=0.049) or benefit-finding (r=−0.34; p<0.05). There was no significant age difference between participants below and above the clinical cut-off (t=1.19; p=0.24).

Correlation analysis revealed a significant relationship between age and the degree of sense-making (r s =−0.53; p<0.01), whereas age and the degree of benefit-finding did not correlate significantly (r s =−0.27; p=0.15).

Participants from the same and from different parental dyads differentiated according to the number of years having elapsed since the loss, with a greater number of years having elapsed for participants from the same parental dyads (M=12, SD=8.06), compared to participants from different parental dyads (M=6.09, SD=6.55). Analysis revealed that the higher the number of years having elapsed since the loss, the lower the score in the CGM (r (one-tailed)=−0.49; p<0.01), and for participants above the clinical cut-off of complicated grief, less time had elapsed since the loss (r pb=−0.37; p=0.03). However, time had no significant relationship with posttraumatic growth (r (one-tailed)=−0.25; p=0.09). No relationship was found between either the degree of sense-making (r s(one-tailed)=−0.23; p=0.11) or the degree of benefit-finding (r (one-tailed)=−0.05; p=0.39) and time since the loss. Moreover, a negative correlation was found between posttraumatic growth and complicated grief for bereaved participants of fewer than 7 years (r=−0.52, p<0.05) (see ).

Table 4 Measures of posttraumatic growth and complicated grief

Results revealed significant correlations between sense-making and posttraumatic growth (r s=0.54, p=0.00), as well as between benefit-finding and posttraumatic growth (r s=0.49, p=0.00). There was no statistically significant correlation between either sense-making (r s=0.08, p=0.34) or benefit-finding (r s =−0.07, p=0.35) with complicated grief. No significant correlation between posttraumatic growth and complicated grief was found (r=0.00, p=0.48) (see ).

Table 5 Relations of meaning reconstruction, complicated grief and posttraumatic growth

Analysis revealed that the correlation between sense-making and posttraumatic growth was especially high for objectively traumatic deaths (r s=0.67, p=0.00) and subjectively traumatic deaths (r s=0.63, p=0.00).

Discussion

The primary goal of the present study was to scrutinize and expand meaning reconstruction themes identified by Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) in a sample of bereaved parents with qualitative methods. Results revealed qualitative evidence of the comprehensive ways bereaved parents try to reconstruct meaning: 30 of 32 existing themes (Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010) were reproduced and 10 additional themes were identified.

Results showed that 82.2% of the participants noted at least one way they had made sense of their loss, whereas only 52.6% in the study of Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) did so. This inconsistency may lie in the fact that meaning reconstruction requires time (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich, Citation2006; Keesee et al., Citation2008; Murphy et al., Citation2003), which in contrast to this work, was not accounted for in the study of Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010).

The most common way of sense-making was the parent's preoccupation with the child's purpose in life, which often evoked the theme of their incontestable identity as a parent (Meert, Briller, Schim, Thurston, & Kabel, Citation2009). Current research increasingly recognizes this process of identification and retaining bonds with the deceased. For example, Ronen et al. (Citation2009–2010) noted that identification with the deceased child differentiated between grieving bereaved parents and complicated grieving bereaved parents. However, under certain conditions continuing bonds to the deceased may also be maladaptive (Field, Citation2006), and being able to make sense of a loss might foster the adaptive nature of retaining bonds (Neimeyer, Baldwin, & Gillies, Citation2006) underlining the clinical importance of sense-making.

None of the 30 (German and Austrian) participants in the present study described understanding the child's death in the context of God's will, whereas this was the most frequently mentioned (17.9%) sense-making theme by predominantly US participants of Lichtenthal et al. study (Citation2010). Other research in the Anglo-American area supports the importance of religious coping (e.g., Davis et al., Citation1998; McIntosh et al., Citation1993). The conflicting results might display a cultural difference between Europe and the US concerning the power of religious beliefs in explaining life and death (Walter, Citation2012): According to Pally (Citation2005), religion plays an essential role in the US, whereas Western Europe is experiencing increasing criticism of institutional religion.

With respect to benefit-finding, comparisons showed that 73.7% of the participants in the study of Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) offered at least one benefit in relation to their loss; that is, 89.7% of the participants in the present study did so. As for sense-making, these differences could be explained by the stricter time criteria imposed by this study.

Inconsistent with Lichtenthal et al. (Citation2010) study, no one mentioned “benefits for other people” as consequence of the loss. Drawing on the theme definition (Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010), it is strongly related to the circumstances of the death, that is, to objective positive consequences such as organ donation. In this study, no such positive consequences for others arose, although some parents are engaged in processes to make that happen, which was included in the theme “helping others.” For example, one mother of a child dying as a result of substance abuse is engaging in the prevention of addiction.

The qualitative approach allowed revealing the struggle some parents experience with benefit-finding, such as the feeling of being forced into justifying their loss. A thought one mother describes as helping her to resolve this struggle speaks for itself:

No father or mother would ever say, “My child has died so that I could learn something.” Therefore, this idea of a heritage is also a proof that my child was here and things have changed. Children leave marks: If a child has been in a room, you notice—because they usually leave a mess (laugh). You know it, and it's the same with children who have died.

Generally, the qualitative data of the current work is an important contribution to existing research on meaning reconstruction, as the assessment was accomplished with elaborated in-depth interviews, assuring guidance and support in the discussion of such a sensitive topic (Dyregrov, Citation2004; Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010) as well as rich results. In contrast, most of the research to date assessed meaning reconstruction in writing and with fewer questions (e.g., Currier et al., Citation2006; Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010; Wheeler, Citation2001).

A secondary aim was to look for associations of meaning reconstruction with complicated grief and posttraumatic growth, with special attention focused on traumatic and unexpected losses. Referring to complicated grief the negative relationship to meaning reconstruction could not be supported either in general (Currier et al., Citation2006; Keesee et al., Citation2008; Lichtenthal et al., Citation2010, Neimeyer et al., Citation2006) or for traumatic loss (Murphy et al., Citation2003; Neimeyer et al., Citation2006). The reason for this discrepancy may lie in the smaller sample size of the current study, which is however comparable to other studies with bereaved parents involving oral interviews. (Barrera et al., Citation2009; Brabant, Forsyth, & McFarlain, Citation1997; Dyregrov & Dyregrov, Citation1999; Meert et al., Citation2009; Possick, Sadeh, & Shamai, Citation2008; Riches & Dawson, Citation1998).

Referring to posttraumatic growth, this study supports the notion that a positive relationship to meaning reconstruction exists (Calhoun et al., Citation2010, Gillies & Neimeyer, Citation2006; Triplett et al., Citation2012). Following the argument of Linley and Joseph (Citation2011), we conclude that study inclusion criteria related to the time since the loss, ensured that the process of meaning reconstruction could already have led to find meaning associated with positive changes, in contrast to the searching phase. Along with Neimeyer, Prigerson and Davies (Citation2002), it was shown that in the case of traumatic loss, sense-making was highly correlated with posttraumatic growth and at the same time seems to be more challenging for parents bereaved by traumatic loss (Lichtenthal, Neimeyer, Currier, Roberts, & Jordan, Citation2013).

Limitations

Due to the small sample size, the self-selection of participants and the fact that most of the participants were female, the generalizability of the findings may be limited. Moreover, many participants came from a parental dyad, thus grieving for the common child, which could create a problem with non-independence (see “nested data” Wijngaards-de Meij et al., Citation2005).

Conclusion

This work provides support for theoretical plurality regarding bereavement research and practice (Rosner, Kruse, & Hagl, Citation2010; Rosner, Pfoh, & Kotoučová, Citation2011) and underlines the value of modern models of grief (e.g., Gillies & Neimeyer, Citation2006; Janoff-Bulman, Citation1992; Klass, Citation1999).

Further, it underlines the importance of meaning-centered therapeutic techniques and the need for clinicians to address this process with an open mind, given the individuality of every loss experience; that is, to be prepared for spiritual thoughts as well as the need to function as interpreter between the client and a medical team, in order to enhance understanding.

Drawing on mentioned limitations, an essential next-step would be extensive qualitative research in order to develop a reliable instrument measuring meaning reconstruction. This would consequently allow for longitudinal studies in larger samples as well as the use of analysis allowing causal interpretations of findings.

Conflict of interest and funding

There is no conflict of interest in the present study for any of the authors.

Supplemental material

Turkish abstract

Download PDF (119.9 KB)

Serbian abstract

Download PDF (74.9 KB)

Russian abstract

Download PDF (93 KB)

German abstract

Download PDF (7.4 KB)

Hungarian abstract

Download PDF (66.7 KB)

Bosnian abstract

Download PDF (75 KB)

Bosnian_Croatian abstract

Download PDF (69.6 KB)

Notes

For the abstract or full text in other languages, please see Supplementary files under Article Tools online

References

  • Barrera M, O'Connor K, D'Agostino N. M, Spencer L, Nicholas D, Jovcevska V, etal. Early parental adjustment and bereavement after childhood cancer death. Death Studies. 2009; 33: 497–520.
  • Boelen P. A, van den Hout M. A, van den Bout J. A cognitive–behavioral conceptualisation of complicated grief. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2006; 13: 109–128.
  • Bonanno G, Neria Y, Mancini A, Coifman K, Litz B, Insel B. Is there more to complicated grief than depression and posttraumatic stress disorder? A test of incremental validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007; 116: 342–351.
  • Brabant S, Forsyth C. J, McFarlain G. The impact of the death of a child on meaning and purpose in life. Journal of Loss and Trauma. 1997; 2(3): 255–266.
  • Cadell S, Regehr C, Hemsworth D. Factors contributing to posttraumatic growth: A proposed structural equitation model. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2003; 73: 279–287.
  • Calhoun L. G, Tedeschi R. G, Cann A, Hanks E. A. Positive outcomes following bereavement: Paths to posttraumatic growth. Psychologica Belgica. 2010; 50: 125–143.
  • Cann A, Calhoun L. G, Tedeschi R. G, Kilmer R. P, Gil-Rivas V, Vishnevsky T, etal. The Core Beliefs Inventory: A brief measure of disruption in the assumptive world. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping. 2010; 23: 19–34.
  • Currier J. M, Holland J. M, Neimeier R. A. Sense-making, grief, and the experience of violent loss: Toward a mediational model. Death Studies. 2006; 30: 403–428.
  • Creswell J. W, Zhang W. The application of mixed methods designs to trauma research. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2009; 22(6): 612–621.
  • Davis C. G, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Larson J. Making sense of loss and benefiting from the experience: Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998; 75(2): 561–574.
  • Dyregrov A, Dyregrov K. Long-term impact of a sudden infant death: A 12- to 15-Year follow-up. Death Studies. 1999; 23: 635–661.
  • Dyregrov K. Bereaved parents’ experience of research participation. Social Science and Medicine. 2004; 58: 391–400.
  • Engelkemeyer S. M, Marwit S. J. Posttraumatic growth in bereaved parents. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008; 21: 344–346.
  • Field N. P. Continuing bonds in adaptation to bereavement: Introduction. Death Studies. 2006; 30: 709–714.
  • Gillies J, Neimeyer R. A. Loss, grief and the search for significance: Toward a model of meaning reconstruction in bereavement. Journal of Constructivist Psychology. 2006; 19: 31–65.
  • Helgeson V. S, Reynolds K. A, Tomich P. L. A meta-analytic review of benefit-finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006; 74: 797–816.
  • Hogan N. S, Schmidt L. A. Testing the grief to personal growth model using structural equitation modeling. Death Studies. 2002; 26: 615–634.
  • Horowitz M. J, Siegel B, Holen A, Bonanno G. A, Milbrath C, Stinson C. H. Diagnostic criteria for complicated grief disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997; 154: 904–910.
  • Hynson J. L, Aroni R, Bauld C, Sawyer S. M. Research with bereaved parents: A question of how not why. Palliative Medicine. 2006; 20: 805–811.
  • Janoff-Bulman R. Shattered Assumptions: Towards a New Psychology of Trauma. 1992; New York: The Free Press.
  • Keesee N. J, Currier J. M, Neimeyer R. A. Predictors of grief following the death of one's child: The contribution of finding meaning. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008; 64(10): 1145–1163.
  • Kersting A, Brähler E, Glaesmer H, Wagner B. Prevalence of complicated grief in a representative population-based sample. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011; 131: 339–343.
  • Klass D. The spiritual lives of bereaved parents. 1999; New York: Brunner/Mazel.
  • Kreicbergs U, Valdimarsdóttir U, Onelöv E, Henter J. I, Steineck G. Anxiety and Depression in Parents 4–9 Years after the Loss of a Child owing to a Malignancy: A population-based follow-up. Psychological Medicine. 2004; 34: 1431–1441.
  • Lehman D. R, Wortman C. B, Williams A. F. Long-term effects of losing a spouse or child in a motor vehicle crash. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987; 52(1): 218–231.
  • Li J, Laursen T. M, Precht D. H, Olsen J, Mortensen P. B. Hospitalization for mental illness among parents after the death of a child. New England Journal of Medicine. 2005; 352: 1190–1196.
  • Li J, Precht D. H, Mortenson P. B, Olson J. Mortality in parents after death of a child in Denmark: A nation-wide follow-up study. Lancet. 2003; 361: 363–367.
  • Lichtenthal W. G, Currier J. M, Neimeyer R. A, Keesee N. J. Sense and significance: A mixed methods examination of meaning making after the loss of one's child. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2010; 66(7): 791–812.
  • Lichtenthal W. G, Neimeyer R. A, Currier J. M, Roberts K, Jordan N. Cause of death and the quest for meaning after the loss of a child. Death Studies. 2013; 37(4): 311–342.
  • Linley P. A, Joseph S. Meaning in life and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Loss and Trauma: International Perspectives on Stress & Coping. 2011; 16(2): 150–159.
  • Maercker A, Langner R. Persönliche Reifung durch Belastungen und Traumata: Validierung zweier deutschsprachiger Fragebogenversionen [Personal growth through stress and trauma: validation of two german-language instruments]. Diagnostica. 2001; 47: 153–162.
  • Maercker A, Langner R. KTM. Komplizierte Trauer Modul [Complicated Grief Module]. 2002. Retrieved June 2, 2011, from http://www.zpid.de/index.php?wahl=products&uwahl=frei&uuwahl=testarchiveintro#k.
  • Mayring P. Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse. Qualitative Content-analysis 2010; Hemsbach: Beltz Verlag. (11. Auflage).
  • McIntosh D. N, Silver R. C, Wortman C. B. Religion's role in adjustment to a negative life event: Coping with the loss of a child. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993; 65(4): 812–821.
  • Meert K. L, Briller S. H, Schim S. M, Thurston C, Kabel A. Examining the needs of bereaved parents in the pediatric intensive care unit: A qualitative study. Death Studies. 2009; 33: 712–740.
  • Murphy S. A, Johnson L. C, Lohan J. Finding meaning in a child's violent death: A five-year prospective analysis of parents personal narratives and empirical data. Death Studies. 2003; 27: 381–404.
  • Neimeyer R. A. Searching for the meaning of meaning: Grief therapy and the process of reconstruction. Death Studies. 2000; 24: 541–558.
  • Neimeyer R. A, Baldwin S. A, Gillies J. Continuing bonds and reconstructing meaning: Mitigating complications in bereavement. Death Studies. 2006; 30: 715–738.
  • Neimeyer R. A, Prigerson H. G, Davies B. Mourning and meaning. American Behavioral Scientist. 2002; 46: 235–251.
  • Pally M. Duell der Paradoxien: Anmerkungen zum Thema Religion in den USA und Europa. Duel of paradox: remarks about religion in the U.S. and Europe Internationale Politik. 2005; 60(4): 6–17.
  • Polatinsky S, Esprey Y. An assessment of gender differences in the perception of benefit resulting from the loss of a child. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2000; 13: 709–718.
  • Possick C, Sadeh R. A, Shamai M. Parents’ experience and meaning construction of the loss of a child in a national terror attack. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2008; 78(1): 93–102.
  • Prigerson H, Horowitz M, Jacobs S, Parkes C, Aslan M, Goodkin K, etal. Prolonged grief disorder: Psychometric validation of criteria proposed for DSM-V and ICD-11. 2009. Retrieved May 12, 2011, from http://www.plosmedicine.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pmed.1000121 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000121.
  • Prigerson H, Maciejewski P, Newsom J, Reynolds C, Frank E, Bierhals E, etal. The inventory of complicated grief: A scale to measure certain maladaptive symptoms of loss. Psychiatry Research. 1995; 59: 65–79.
  • Riches G, Dawson P. Lost children, living memories: The role of photographs in processes of grief and adjustment among bereaved parents. Death Studies. 1998; 22(2): 121–140.
  • Rogers C. H, Floyd F. J, Seltzer M. M, Greenberg J, Hong J. Long-term effects of the death of a child on parents’ adjustment in midlife. Journal of Family Psychology. 2008; 22: 203–211.
  • Ronen R, Packman W, Field N. P, Davies B, Kramer R, Long J. K. The relationship between grief adjustment and continuing bonds for parents who have lost a child. Omega. 2009–2010; 60(1): 1–31.
  • Rosner R, Kruse J, Hagl M. A meta-analysis of interventions for bereaved children and adolescents. Death Studies. 2010; 34: 99–136.
  • Rosner R, Pfoh G, Kotoučová M. Treatment of complicated grief. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2011; 2 7995, doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v2i0.7995.
  • Taku K, Calhoun L. G, Cann A, Tedeschi R. G. The role of rumination in the coexistence of distress and posttraumatic growth among bereaved Japanese University students. Death Studies. 2008; 32: 428–444.
  • Taku K, Cann A, Tedeschi R. G, Calhoun L. G. Intrusive versus deliberate rumination in posttraumatic growth across US and Japanese samples, Anxiety. Stress & Coping. 2009; 22: 129–136.
  • Tedeschi R. G, Calhoun L. G. The posttraumatic growth inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1996; 9: 455–471.
  • Triplett K. N, Tedeschi R. G, Cann A, Calhoun L. G, Reeve C. L. Posttraumatic growth, meaning in life, and life satisfaction in response to trauma, Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice. and Policy. 2012; 4(4): 400–410.
  • Uren T. H, Wastell C. A. Attachment and meaning making in perinatal bereavement. Death Studies. 2002; 26(4): 279–308.
  • Walter T. Why different countries manage death differently: A comparative analysis of modern urban societies. The British Journal of Sociology. 2012; 63(1): 123–145.
  • Wheeler I. Parental bereavement: The crisis of meaning. Death Studies. 2001; 25: 51–66.
  • Wijngaards-de Meij L, Stroebe M, Schut H, Stroebe W, van den Bout J, van der Heijden P, etal. Couples at risk following the death of their child: Predictors of grief versus depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005; 73(4): 617–623.
  • Znoj H. Komplizierte Trauer. Complicated Grief 2004; Göttingen: Hogrefe Verlag.