392
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Mini Review

The breath of a flower

CO2 adds another channel - and then some - to plant-pollinator interactions

Pages 66-68 | Received 04 Apr 2008, Accepted 16 Apr 2008, Published online: 30 Sep 2008

Abstract

n this article I comment on our findings that floral carbon dioxide (CO2) can be used by Manduca sexta hawkmoths in a scale- and context-dependent fashion. We firstly found, in wind tunnel assays, that diffusing floral CO2 is used as long-distance cue (e.g. meters). Moths track CO2 plumes up-wind in the same manner they track floral odors. Nevertheless, CO2 did not appear to function as a local stimulus for flower probing, evidencing a scale-dependent role in nectar foraging. These results were further enriched by a second finding. In dual choice assays, where moths were offered two scented artificial flowers of which only one emitted above-ambient CO2-levels, female Manduca sexta chose to feed on the CO2 emitting flower only when host-plant volatiles were added to the background. We discuss this apparent measurement of oviposition obligations during foraging in the context of the life histories of both insect and plant species. These findings seem to pinpoint the usually artificial nature of compartmentalizing herbivory and pollination as different, isolated aspects of insect-plant interactions. Insects do not seem to have a defined response to a certain stimulus; instead, motor programs appear to be in response to composite arrangements of external stimuli and inner states. If animal-plant interactions have evolved under these premises, I believe it may prove beneficial to include a non-linear, integrative view of plant multi-signaling and life history aspects into the study of pollination biology.

Pollination Biology: Plant Signals and Animal Responses

Since KölreuterCitation1 and SprengelCitation2, the study of the behavioral responses of animal pollinators to plant signaling has become a central theme in pollination biology. The diversity and abundance of the class InsectaCitation3 have made them principal actors amongst the cast of animal pollinators.Citation4 Plants advertise their rewards with a wealth of floral cues, while nectivorous insects utilize their sensory and cognitive abilities to exploit such information while foraging.Citation4Citation6 Most angiosperms use odors, color displays, alluring shapes and probably other, unknown floral stimuli, to attract insects to their nectar reward (reviewed in ref. Citation7).

In our studies, we focus on the foraging behavior of the nocturnal hawkmoth Manduca sexta (hereafter: Manduca). In recent years Raguso and collaborators have experimentally manipulated floral signals to both dissect and reconstruct the constellation of stimuli used by Manduca during nectar foraging.Citation8Citation14 The results of these studies have lent support to the idea that pollinators make use of multiple sensory channels whose central integration in the brain has profound consequences for the insect's behavior. However, despite the insights that we have gained through the use of artificial flowers on the relative importance of olfaction, vision, mechanoreception and hygroreception in nectar foraging by Manduca, we remained skeptical. To what extent did our artificial flowers realistically offer the full complement of stimuli presented by natural flowers? Obviously, to address this question we needed to turn to the natural nectar sources of Manduca. In the Sonoran desert of Arizona, the night-blooming Datura wrightii (hereafter: Datura) is a preferred nectar source for Manduca moths.Citation15 Datura is also a preferred host-plant for Manduca larvae, which adds an unusual twist to the putatively mutualistic relationship between Datura flowers and adult Manduca pollinators, and made it worthwhile to study in the context of our interests.

The Breath of Datura: A Matter of Scale

The candle-like flower buds of Datura undergo impressive elongation, nectar secretion and scent biosynthesis prior to opening, suggesting high metabolic expenditures associated with flowering. Datura flowers open abruptly, releasing clouds of carbon dioxide (CO2) at concentrations up to 200 ppm above the ambient air. Our colleagues at the University of Arizona (Guerenstein et al.Citation30) had shown that CO2 levels at the corolla opening are initially correlated with floral nectar production, suggesting that the interests of foraging moths would be served by an ability to detect floral CO2. Indeed, Manduca is capable of detecting minute (0.5 ppm) differences in CO2 concentrations with its CO2-sensing labial-pit organ LPO;Citation16Citation18 and Thom et al.Citation19 showed that naïve male Manduca first probe on artificial flowers emitting levels of CO2 observed in freshly opened Datura flowers. These observations convinced us that CO2 was a candidate for a novel floral component that Manduca could use in its foraging bouts. But, at what scale might it function? Does CO2 elicit probing responses in the fashion that appetitively conditioned odors trigger Manduca's proboscis extension reflex PER?;Citation20 Does CO2 also mediate orientation behavior towards individual flowers? Continuing with our experimental strategy of dissecting and reconstructing floral stimuli to better understand insect foraging behavior, we tackled these questions.

We have recently shown, using an experimental wind tunnel, that Manduca adults can utilize CO2 while nectar foraging from artificial flowers emitting CO2-rich plumes at concentrations produced by Datura flowers.Citation13 In a first series of experiments we found CO2 to act only at a distance: moths track CO2 up-wind in the same fashion they track floral odors, but CO2 shows no effect on the flower probing response. Once a moth reaches the flower's threshold, fragrance seems to be a reliable local indicator of a feeding site. The effect of CO2 proved to be scale-dependent; from a distance (3 m) it acted as an attractant, or orientation cue, but within mere cm of the flower it seemed to have no effect as a local feeding stimulus. However, if moths can track floral odors from both a distance and close range to evaluate nectar sources, why would they need to track CO2? A possible answer to that question might be rooted in the moths' olfactory physiology. Because CO2-receptor cells can respond to both increments (excitation) and decrements (inhibition) of the CO2 concentration, they may detect fluctuations with higher temporal resolution than the olfactory receptor cells and neural circuitry that process floral odors, which typically respond to increments in concentration.Citation17 Both at the periphery (LPO) and at the central nervous system level, fluctuations in [CO2] can be monitored at a temporal resolution of at least 10 Hz,Citation17 whereas fluctuations in typical floral odors (e.g., linalool) are usually monitored at lower frequencies.Citation21,Citation22 This could allow moths to more efficiently search for high-CO2 emitting flowers from a distance, potentially facilitating more accurate predictions of flower profitability “on the wing”.

The Breath of Datura: A Matter of Context

A second experiment, involving dual-choice assays, suggested additional reasons why floral CO2 might be important to the Datura-Manduca relationship. When given the choice between a scented paper flower and an identical second flower with above-ambient levels of CO2, we discovered an unexpected sexually dimorphic behavior. Even though both sexes responded similarly to the floral stimuli in the first experiment, in the dual choice assays of the second experiment, males unequivocally probed the CO2-emitting flower first, while female first choices were random. This result was puzzling, and suggested that foraging and oviposition tasks might be conflated. However, previous investigations had not explicitly addressed this possibility. Experiments conducted to evaluate the effect of CO2 on female Manduca oviposition behaviorCitation23 showed that moths did not have any strong preference between Datura plants surrounded by above-ambient vs. near-to-ambient [CO2]. However, in these experiments the Datura plants did not have flowers.

It has long been known that Datura is both a nectar source and a host plant for Manduca, whose active periods for foraging and ovipositon overlap considerably.Citation24 Despite this pattern, historical research on these two tasks in Manduca—and on pollination and herbivory in general—has tended to separate them intentionally. Thus, we omitted host plant odors in our previous experiments with floral stimuli.Citation8,Citation11 Thom et al.Citation19 used only males to study CO2 effects on foraging behavior and Abrell et al.Citation23 used plants with no flowers to study oviposition. However, female M. sexta often mix feeding and ovipositing boutsCitation25 and their reproductive fitness benefits directly from a 10-fold increase in mature egg production when they feed as adults.Citation26 With this in mind we designed a third experiment, in which we presented moths with the exact same dual-choice as before, but now with host-plant (tomato) odors in the background see.Citation13,Citation27 This time, males and females responded identically, with unambiguous preference for the flower with high [CO2]. Now, in addition to the scale-dependence of CO2 found in our first experiment, we identified a context-dependent role for CO2. Host-plant odors provided the context in which females chose flowers with high CO2. How could we explain these results?

Looking into Herbivory to Understand Pollination?

It has recently been shown that female Manduca oviposit more eggs on Datura plants whose floral nectar volume has been experimentally increased.Citation28 In that study, the authors suggest that nectar volume could measure into the oviposition behavior of female Manduca, probably as a way to “evaluate” host-plant physiological state. Thus, nectar volume would be a local proxy for host-plant quality see.Citation29 In view of the positive correlation between nectar production and CO2 emission in D. wrightii,Citation30 we have proposed that CO2 could be used by female moths as a long distance indicator of host-plant quality. In other words, by orienting towards a plume of CO2, Manduca sexta females might increase the probability of finding nectar-filled flowers belonging to a suitable host-plant. Subsequent successful feeding would then, at the local scale, lower thresholds for oviposition.

Herbivory versus Pollination: Why Different Parcels?

Our study adds carbon dioxide to the list of plant primary metabolites having a “secondary function”, but also depicts an interesting, contextual function of respiratory CO2 in the relationship between Manduca sexta and Datura wrightii. In the first place, our findings compel us to consider that plant signals of diverse physical natures, acquired through different sensory channels, are integrated as an assemblage of stimuli, where scale and context acquire crucial relevance. In second place, our study suggests it is a mistake to consider a priori that pollination and herbivory are separate, isolated categories of plant-animal interactions. Perhaps this assumption reflects that a large proportion of our knowledge on pollination biology comes from studies on social insects with division of labor i.e., honeybees, bumblebees, wasps (see ref. Citation31). Lepidopterans and numerous other solitary pollinators often perform tasks such as foraging, mating and oviposition on limited spatial and temporal scales. The context-dependence of “foraging” responses by Manduca females to floral CO2 suggests that this plant-insect interaction has been molded by selective pressures extending beyond the strict imperatives of foraging and cross-pollination for insect and plant, respectively. We suspect that this phenomenon might be more common than is currently appreciated (see ref. Citation28), especially considering the relative neglect with which solitary pollinators have been studied.Citation31 Comprehensive, integrated studies that consider life histories and their vicissitudes may yield more profound insights in the study of insect-plant interactions, as revealed by the breath of the sacred Datura.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Pablo Guerenstein for his useful suggestions and Robert Raguso for his efforts to interpret my thoughts and suggest intelligible ways to communicate them. I am deeply indebt with Professor Thomas Eisner for his editorial work on the article I comment on and his constant encouragement. The author was funded by the CALS excellence fellowship, awarded by the Department of Neurobiology and Behavior of Cornell University.

References

  • Kölreuter JG. Vorläufige Nachricht von einigen das Geschlecht der Pflanzen betreffenden Versuchen und Beobachtungen 1761; Leipzig
  • Sprengel CK. Das entdeckte Geheimniss der Natur im Bau und in der Befruchtung der Blumen 1793; Berlin Vieweg
  • Wilson EO. The diversity of life 1992; Cambridge, MA Belknap Press of Harvard University Press
  • Waser N, Ollerton J. Plant-Pollinator Interactions: From Specialization to Generalization 2006; Chicago University of Chicago Press
  • Barth FG. Insects and flowers: the biology of a partnership 1991; Princeton, NJ Princeton University Press
  • Chittka L, Thompson JD, et al. Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: animal behaviour and floral evolution 2001; Cambridge Cambridge University Press
  • Raguso RA. Flowers as sensory billboards: progress towards an integrated understanding of floral advertisement. Curr Op Plant Biol 2004; 7:434 - 440
  • Raguso RA, Willis MA. Synergy between visual and olfactory cues in nectar feeding by naive hawkmoths Manduca sexta. Animal Behav 2002; 64:685 - 695
  • Raguso RA, Willis MA. Synergy between visual and olfactory cues in nectar feeding by wild hawkmoths Manduca sexta. Animal Behav 2005; 69:407 - 418
  • Goyret J, Raguso RA. The role of mechanosensory input in flower handling efficiency and learning by Manduca sexta. J Exp Biol 2006; 209:1585 - 1593
  • Goyret J, Markwell PM, Raguso RA. The effect of decoupling olfactory and visual stimuli on the foraging behavior of Manduca sexta. J Exp Biol 2007; 210:1398 - 1405
  • Goyret J, Pfaff M, Raguso RA, Kelber A. Why do Manduca sexta feed from white flowers? Innate and learnt colour preferences in a hawkmoth. Naturwissenschaften 2008;
  • Goyret J, Markwell PM, Raguso RA. Context- and scale-dependent effects of floral CO2 on nectar forgaing by Manduca sexta. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; In press.
  • Raguso RA, LeClere AR, Schlumpberger BO. Sensory flexibility in hawkmoth foraging behavior: Lessons from Manduca sexta and other species. Chemical Senses 2005; 30:295 - 296
  • Raguso RA, Henzel C, Buchmann SL, Nabhan GP. Trumpet flowers of the Sonoran desert: Floral biology of Peniocereus cacti and sacred Datura. Intl J Plant Sci 2003; 164:877 - 892
  • Stange G. High-resolution measurement of atmospheric carbon dioxide concentration changes by the labial palp organ of the moth Heliothis armigera. J Comp Physiol A 1992; 171:317 - 324
  • Guerenstein PG, Christensen TA, Hildebrand JG. Sensory processing of ambient CO2 information in the brain of the moth Manduca sexta. J Comp Physiol A 2004; 190:707 - 725
  • Kent KS, Harrow ID, Quartararo P, Hildebrand JG. An accessory olfactory pathway in Lepidoptera: The labial pit organ and its central projections in Manduca sexta and certain other sphinx moths and silk moths. Cell Tissue Research 1986; 245:237 - 245
  • Thom C, Guerenstein PG, Mechaber WL, Hildebrand JG. Floral CO2 reveals flower profitability to moths. J Chem Ecol 2004; 30:1285 - 1288
  • Daly KC, Durtschi ML, Smith BH. Olfactory-based discrimination learning in the moth Manduca sexta. J Insect Physiol 2001; 47:375 - 384
  • Christensen TA, Heinbockel T, Hildebrand JG. Olfactory information processing in the brain: Encoding chemical and temporal features of odors. J Neurobiol 1996; 30:82 - 91
  • King JR, Christensen TA, Hildebrand JG. Response characteristics of an identified, sexually dimorphic olfactory glomerulus. J Neurosci 2000; 20:2391 - 2399
  • Abrell L, Guerenstein PG, Mechaber WL, Stange G, Christensen TA, Nakanishi K, Hildebrand JG. Effect of elevated atmospheric CO2 on oviposition behavior in Manduca sexta moths. Global Change Biol 2005; 11:1272 - 1282
  • Madden AH, Chamberlin FS. Biology of the tobacco hornworm in the southern cigar tobacco district. US Department of Agriculture Technical Bulletin 1945;
  • Adler LS, Bronstein JL. Attracting antagonists: does floral nectar increase leaf hervibory?. Ecol 2004; 85:1519 - 1526
  • Ziegler R, Schulz M. Regulation of lipid metabolism during flight in Manduca sexta. J Insect Physiol 1986; 32:903 - 908
  • Mechauber WL, Capaldo CT, Hildebrand JG. Behavioral responses of adult female tobacco hornworms, Manduca sexta, to hostplant volatiles change with age and mating status. Journal of Insect Science 2002; 2:1 - 8
  • Adler LS, Bronstein JL. Attracting antagonists: Does floral nectar increase leaf herbivory?. Ecol 2004; 85:1519 - 1526
  • Kessler D, Baldwin IT. Making sense of nectar scents: the effects of nectar secondary metabolites on floral visitors of Nicotiana attenuata. Plant J 2006; 49:840 - 854
  • Guerenstein PG, Yepez AE, Van Haren J, Williams DG, Hildebrand JG. Floral CO2 emission may indicate food abundance to nectar-feeding moths. Naturwissenschaften 2004; 91:329 - 333
  • Weiss M. Chittka L, Thompson JD. Vision and learning in some neglected pollinators: beetles, flies, moths and butterflies. Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: Animal Behaviour and Floral Evolution 2001; Cambridge Cambridge University Press