2,250
Views
29
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Mini Review

Chemical communication and mother-infant recognition

Pages 279-281 | Received 16 Feb 2009, Accepted 17 Feb 2009, Published online: 15 Jun 2009

Abstract

Fifty years after the term ‘pheromone’ was coined by Peter Karlson and Martin Lüsher, the search for these semiochemicals is still an elusive goal of chemical ecology and communication studies. Contrary to what appears in the popular press, the race is still on to capture and define human scents. Over the last several years, it became increasingly clear that pheromone-like chemical signals probably play a role in offspring identification and mother recognition. Recently, we analyzed the volatile compounds in sweat patch samples collected from the para-axillary and nipple-areola regions of women during pregnancy and after childbirth. We hypothesized that, at the time of birth and during the first weeks of life, the distinctive olfactory pattern of the para-axillary area is probably useful to newborns for recognizing and distinguishing their own mother, whereas the characteristic pattern of the nipple-areola region is probably useful as a guide to nourishment.

Pheromones or Signature Odors?

Throughout the lives of most mammalian species, the sense of smell plays an important role in response to chemical messengers involved in different behaviors. Within the overall olfactory communication pheromones are very important actors. The term ‘pheromone’—referred to the chemical compounds used to communicate between individuals of the same species—was coined by Peter Karlson and Martin Lüsher in 1959.Citation1 Over the last 50 years, pheromones, sending messages between individuals, have been found in many species across the animal kingdom.

The so-called “individual odors”, learned for recognition, do not seem to fit Karlson and Lüsher's definition. In the past, some researchers even doubted that humans could have their behavior altered by something as simple as an instinctive reaction to smell. Now, after years of debate, it seems clear that these variable odors are not pheromones and instead are better referred to as ‘signature odors’.

But species-specific small molecules, that fit the classic pheromone definition, have now been identified for mammals. It appears clear that signature odors and pheromones can be mixed for effect. Some mammals, including elephants and mice, present their small-molecule pheromones in the cleft of highly variable lipocalin proteins. Pheromone signals can also be overlaid and improved with individual signature odors, the proteins release the small molecules slowly, making them last longer.

The consensus now is that the human use of pheromones, as in all mammals, appears likely. The armpits are prime source candidates, as their odor emanation develops along with other changes at puberty. However, both human behavior and chemical emissions are so complex that at present the research is challenging. Up to now, no pheromones have been conclusively identified, but a strong contender for the first identified human pheromone is some elusive compound in the armpit of women. Apparently this unidentified pheromone causes menstrual synchrony in females living in close quarters. It has been speculated that its identification could potentially open the door to sniffable contraceptives.Citation2

Chemical Basis for Olfactory Communication

In animal species, recognition between individuals is an essential requirement for any kind of further interaction. Recognition between mother and newborn is a fundamental behavioral interaction that is worth systematic investigation. The emotional relationship between a mother and her newborn begins with mutual recognition, which starts during gestation and continues through birth, augmented by body contact and lactation. Imprinting takes place through visual, auditory and olfactory learning, which occurs very early during the so-called “critical period”. Consequently, from beginning of pregnancy olfaction seems to represent an Ariadne's thread that permits the infant after birth to find its mother.Citation3

Pheromones regulate reproductive behavior in many animal species. Once released in the environment, through urine or glandular secretions, these volatile substances reach other individuals of the same species, signaling, for instance, mating availability and strengthening ties between mother and offspring, as well as regulating social relationships.Citation4 In non-human vertebrate species, pheromones are detected by a specific sensory apparatus, the vomeronasal system, composed of a peripheral organ located at the base of the nasal septum, the vomeronasal nerve and a nerve center, the accessory olfactory bulb. The vomeronasal system is completely separated and independent from the main olfactory system.Citation5 It is triggered by a different class of volatile substances and is present in many reptiles and in almost all mammals, but absent in fishes and birds, even if they possess a main olfactory system.

Primates, long considered functionally non-microsomatic, were previously thought to show complex olfactory communication only in prosimians and in some New World monkeys. Now it is well established that even higher primates use pheromones to recognize con-specific individuals and for territorial marking.Citation6Citation8 Furthermore, protein-pheromone complexes—present in the secretions released by scent-marking of some non-human primates—were recently shown to activate vomeronasal receptors particularly for sexually related behaviors and intra-specific identification of individuals.Citation9Citation12

Formerly, it was widely held that the human vomeronasal organ was vestigial and even the existence of pheromonal communication in humans was contested. However, support for a role of pheromones in human behavior came from several observational studies (i.e., synchronization of the menstrual cycleCitation13), but this role was often denied because in the absence of a sufficient neuro-anatomic basis for such a complex behavior.

Recently, a new class of olfactory receptors (trace amino-associated receptors, TAAR) was discovered in the olfactory epithelium of mice.Citation14 Genes similar to those responsible for the control of these receptors in mice have been identified in humans and fishes. These data suggest a great evolutionary conservation of these genes and lend support to the hypothesis that the putative human pheromonal response is mediated by receptors located in the main olfactory system.

The putative human pheromones are steroids present in the secretions of exocrine glands.Citation15Citation20 Estrogen derivatives are present in females (the so-called “copulins”—mixtures of aliphatic acids such as acetic, propionic, butyric, isovaleric and isocaproic acid with estratetraenol), and androgen derivatives are present in males (androstenol, androstenone and androstadienone). Recent studies concerning the most volatile compounds of human sweatCitation21Citation23 have shown that the characteristic odor produced by the para-axillary region is due to the presence of volatile C6–C11 acids, the most abundant being E-3-methyl-2-hexenoic acid (E-3M2H).

Mother-Child Identification

Human chemical signals may also play a role in offspring identification. Odor cues from newborns are absolutely salient to their mothers.Citation24 Mothers are able to distinguish the odor of their own newborn baby from that of other newborns.Citation25 Experiments also have demonstrated that adults can even recognize gender and individuality of non-related children.Citation26 Thus, body odors can provide humans with important information about the individual identity of their offspring.Citation27Citation32

On the other hand, children usually prefer parts of clothes that were in contact with the axilla and worn by their own mothers to clothes worn by other mothers.Citation25 Therefore, chemical signals seem to have a fundamental role in the mechanism of mother-child identification.Citation30,Citation33 Breast-feed versus bottle-feed infants show different reactions to maternal odors. Breast-feed infants are exposed to salient maternal odors and rapidly become familiarized with their mothers' unique olfactory signature.Citation34 Apparently, orientation to lactating-breast odors is an inborn adaptive response of newborns.Citation35

It seems an inescapable conclusion that naturally occurring odors play an important role in mediating infant behavior. Even fetal olfactory learning seems to occur and breast odors from the mother exert a pheromone-like effect at the newborn's first attempt to locate the nipple. Newborns are generally responsive to breast odors produced by lactating women.Citation33 Olfactory recognition may be implicated in the early stages of the mother-infant attachment process, when the newborns learn to recognize their own mother's unique odor signature: this process is possibly made easier by the high norepinephrine release and the arousal of the locus coeruleus at birth.Citation36 Human infants are responsive to maternal odors beginning shortly after birth. They show an attraction to amniotic fluid odor that may reflect fetal exposure to that substance (i.e., prenatal olfactory learningCitation37). Moreover, human amniotic fluid seems to carry individualized odor properties, which are hypothesized to initiate parent-infant interactions.Citation38

Not only Chemical Signals: Some Socio-Biological Remarks

It is clear that, as any other organism, humans are subject to invisible but potentially irresistible influences of metabolic materials on our muscle, motive and motor actions, both directly and indirectly.Citation39,Citation40

Obviously, mutual recognition among organisms is related to many aspects of the personal profile, both of metabolic-material biological similarity and morphologic-motor ecological familiarity.Citation41

Mothers recognize the sounds of their own babies' chortles and cries, and may differentiate among their causes, from tiredness to hunger, from pain to scare. Yet mothers may pause for a moment to listen harder to cries from an unseen child that are similar enough to their own babies' cries to require more attention before discounting and dismissing them as indeed being from someone else baby.Citation42

Yet it is appreciated that the cry from any newborn can cause agitation and distress in any mother. Perhaps this reflects the process of acquiring maternal memories: that is, at the time a mother has a newborn, she does not yet know her babies cry well enough to discriminate with certainty. Moreover, mothers of newborns are in states of hormonally heightened metabolic arousal, and are thus readily responsive to both biologically and ecologically conditioned cues.Citation43

Immediacy of recognition of a simple similar-enough sound in a noisy place, such as a familiar name, is well enough known (the “cocktail party syndrome”), even if that name was not in fact said, and so what was heard was only ‘similar enough’ to seem ‘familiar enough’.

The connection among all aspects of recognition is surely not sets of discrete ‘signals’ but, instead, a composition of several sorts of similarities and familiarities, including biochemical features. Probably these features serve to substantiate and sustain mutual recognition.Citation44

Volatile Compounds Behind Mother Recognition

In our recent studyCitation3 we hypothesized that women probably develop a volatile profile, through pregnancy and childbirth that enables identification of the mother by the newborn. The aim was to understand, through an analytical approach, how the volatile pattern of pregnant women changes during pregnancy and, consequently, to verify the effective role played by volatile chemical signals in the mechanism of mother-infant recognition.

We identified the volatiles compounds in sweat patch samples collected from the para-axillary and nipple-areola regions of women during pregnancy and after childbirth. Results showed that during pregnancy women developed a distinctive pattern of five volatile compounds common to the para-axillary and nipple-areola regions (1-dodecanol, 1-1′-oxybis octane, isocurcumenol, α-hexyl-cinnamic aldehyde and isopropyl myristate).

Hypothetically, the differentiation of the olfactory pattern among pregnant women helps newborns to recognize their own mother and distinguish her from other individuals. At the time of birth and during the first weeks of life, the distinctive olfactory pattern of the para-axillary area might be useful to newborns to recognize and distinguish their mother, whereas the characteristic pattern of the nipple-areola region is probably useful as a guide to nourishment.

Future Challenges

These recent results show the effectiveness of the methodology used. Through the collection and analysis of the secretion released at the level of the para-axillary area and in the nipple areola region, it is possible to investigate the volatile compounds in a proper way. Up to now these phenomena have often been investigated with inadequate methodologies and, as a consequence, the role of volatile compounds as regulators of mother-infant recognition has been underestimated. Moreover, recently developed technical instruments and procedures—as Solid Phase Micro-Extraction (SPME), Dynamic Head-Space Extraction (DHE), and Maldi TOF/TOF—in addition to the classic ones—as Gas Chromatography-Mass Spectrometry (GC-MS) and Liquid Chromatography-Mass Spectrometry (LC-MS)—now allow investigators to characterize volatile and non-volatile compounds with high reliability.Citation45

The study of the mechanism of mother-infant recognition is important not only for the acquisition of new knowledge concerning the emission of signal molecules essential for mother-child identification, but also for its clear practical consequences. This information can be helpful for setting up the proper conditions to establish solid mother-child bonding. It can indicate the behavior and conduct to maintain during gestation and the initiation period of life of the newborn. Therefore an understanding of the mechanisms of newborns recognition of their mother could have practical health implications.

Acknowledgements

I am deeply in debt to Professor Roscoe Stanyon for his editorial work on the article. Moreover, I would like to thank Leaf Lovejoy for useful suggestions. I also thank Professor Brunetto Chiarelli and Professor Giorgio Mello for encouragement.

References

  • Karlson P, Lüscher M. Pheromones: a new term for a class of biologically active substances. Nature 1959; 183:55 - 56
  • Wyatt TD. Fifty years of pheromones. Nature 2009; 457:262 - 263
  • Vaglio S, Minicozzi P, Bonometti E, Mello G, Chiarelli B. Volatile signals during pregnancy: a possible chemical basis for mother-infant recognition. J Chem Ecol 2009; 35:131 - 139
  • Vaglio S, Chiarelli B, Ligabue Stricker F. Analysis on pheromonal communication in primates: a research proposal. Folia Primatol 2006; 77:295 - 296
  • Døving KB, Trotier D. Structure and function of the vomeronasal organ. J Exp Biol 1998; 201:2913 - 2925
  • Michael RP, Keverne EB. Pheromones in the communication of sexual status in primates. Nature 1968; 218:746 - 749
  • Michael RP, Keverne EB, Bonsail RW. Isolation of male sex attractants from a female primate. Science 1971; 172:964 - 966
  • Keverne EB. Pheromonal influences on the endocrine regulation of reproduction. Trends Neurosci 1983; 6:381 - 384
  • Smith TE, Tomlinson AJ, Mlotkiewicz JA, Abbot DH. Female marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) can be identified from the chemical composition of their scent marks. Chem Senses 2001; 26:449 - 458
  • Hayes RA, Morelli TL, Wright PC. Anogenital gland secretions of Lemur catta and Propithecus verreauxi coquereli: a preliminary chemical examination. Am J Primatol 2004; 63:49 - 62
  • Palagi E, Telara S, Curreli R, De Santi L, Garbusi P, Vaglio S, et al. Ethological and chemical investigation of olfactory communication in Lemur catta: research proposal and preliminary reports. Folia Primatol 2004; 75:403
  • Vaglio S, Palagi E, Telara S, Boscaro F, Moneti G, Borgognini Tarli S. Male scent-marking in Lemur catta: an investigation by chemical and behavioural approaches. Folia Primatol 2004; 75:400 - 401
  • Mc Clintock MK. Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature 1971; 229:244 - 245
  • Liberles SD, Buck LB. A second class of chemosensory receptors in the olfactory epithelium. Nature 2006; 442:645 - 650
  • Taylor R. Brave new nose: sniffing out human sexual chemistry. J Nat Inst Health Res 1994; 6:47 - 51
  • Stern K, Mc Clintock MK. Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones. Nature 1998; 392:177 - 179
  • Grosser BI, Monti-Block L, Jenings-White C, Berliner DL. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of androstadienone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000; 25:289 - 299
  • Pause BM. Are androgen steroids acting as pheromones in humans?. Physiol Behav 2004; 83:21 - 29
  • Grammer K, Fink B, Neave N. Human pheromones and sexual attraction. Eur J Obstet Gyn R B 2005; 118:135 - 142
  • Hauser R, Wiergowski M, Gos T, Marczak M, Karaszewski B, Wodniak-Ochocinska L. Alarm pheromones as an exponent of emotional state shortly before death. Science fiction or a new challenge?. Forensic Sci Int 2005; 155:226 - 230
  • Zeng XN, Leyden JJ, Lawley HJ, Sawano K, Nohara I, Preti G. Analysis of characteristic odors from human male axillae. J Chem Ecol 1991; 17:1469 - 1492
  • Zeng XN, Leyden JJ, Spielman AI, Preti G. Analysis of characteristic human female axillary odors: qualitative comparison to males. J Chem Ecol 1996; 22:237 - 257
  • Bernier UR, Kline DL, Barnard DR, Schreck CE, Yost RA. Analysis of human skin emanations by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. Anal Chem 2000; 72:747 - 756
  • Kaitz M, Good A, Rokem AM, Eidelman AI. Mothers' recognition of their newborns by olfactory cues. Dev Psychobiol 1987; 20:587 - 591
  • Schaal B, Motagner H, Hertling E, Bolzoni D, Moyse R, Quinchon R. Les stimulations olfactives dans les relations entre l'enfant et la mere. Reprod Nutr Dev 1980; 20:843 - 858
  • Ligabue Stricker F. I feromoni: conoscenze attuali e brevi cenni sugli esperimenti in corso. Antropologia Contemporanea 1991; 14:305 - 314
  • Doty RL. Olfactory communication in humans. Chem Senses 1981; 6:351 - 376
  • Olsson SB, Barnard J, Turri L. Olfaction and identification of unrelated individuals: examination of the mysteries of human odor recognition. J Chem Ecol 2006; 32:1635 - 1645
  • Porter R, Moore JD. Human kin recognition by olfactory cues. Physiol Behav 1981; 27:493 - 495
  • Porter RH, Cernoch JM, Mc Laughlin FJ. Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiol Behav 1983; 30:151 - 154
  • Porter RH, Cernoch JM, Baogh RD. Odour signatures and kin recognition. Physiol Behav 1985; 34:445 - 448
  • Russell MJ, Mendelsson T, Peeke H. Mother's identification of their infant's odors. Ethol Sociobiol 1983; 4:29 - 31
  • Porter RH, Winberg J. Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neurosci Biobehav R 1999; 23:439 - 449
  • Cernoch JM, Porter RH. Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants. Child Devel 1985; 56:1593 - 1598
  • Porter RH, Makin JW, Davis LD, Christensen KM. An assessment of the salient olfactory environment of formula-fed infants. Physiol Behav 1991; 50:907 - 911
  • Winberg J, Porter RH. Olfaction and human neonatal behavior: clinical implications. Acta Paediatr 1998; 87:6 - 10
  • Varendi H, Porter RH, Winberg J. Attractiveness of amniotic fluid odor: evidence of prenatal olfactory learning. Acta Paediatr 1996; 85:1223 - 1227
  • Schaal B, Marlier L. Maternal and paternal perception of individual odor signatures in human amniotic fluid—Potential role in early bonding?. Biol Neonate 1998; 74:266 - 273
  • Morowitz HJ. Bacteriohopanetetrol and the sociology of science. Complexity 2008; 13:8 - 9
  • Srinivasan V, Morowitz HJ, Smith E. Essential amino acids, from LUCA to LUCY. Complexity 2008; 13:8 - 9
  • Lyon P. The biogenic approach to cognition. Cogn Process 2006; 7:11 - 29
  • Leavitt LA. Mothers' sensitivity to infant signals. Pediatrics 1998; 102:1247 - 1249
  • Bath R. “Reading-your-baby lesson” for parents of excessively crying infants: the concept of “guided parent-infant training sessions”. Prax Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatr 2000; 49:537 - 549
  • Lier L. Mother-infant relationship in the first year of life. Acta Paediatr Scand Suppl 1988; 344:31 - 42
  • Curran AM, Rabin SI, Prada PA, Furton KG. Comparison of the volatile organic compounds present in human odor using SPME-GC/MS. J Chem Ecol 2005; 31:1607 - 1619