960
Views
48
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Opinion Article

Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression?

, , &
Pages 285-291 | Received 08 Feb 2009, Accepted 08 Apr 2009, Published online: 15 Jun 2009

Abstract

The escalation of conflicts of interest into aggressive conflict can be costly in terms of increased post-conflict stress and damage to the opponents’ relationship. Some costs may be mitigated through post-conflict interactions. One such type of interaction is affiliative contact from a bystander to the recipient of aggression. This type of interaction has been suggested to have a number of functions, including stress reduction and opponent relationship repair. It may also protect bystanders from redirected aggression from the original recipient of aggression. Here we review the evidence for such functions and propose a framework within which the function and occurrence of post-conflict affiliation directed from a bystander to the recipient of aggression is related to the quality of the relationships between the individuals involved and the patterns of behavior expressed.

Introduction

Aggressive conflict can be very costly in terms of risk of injury, loss of time and energy, as well as loss of benefits afforded by the relationship between the opponentsCitation1Citation5 and increased post-conflict levels of stress (behavioralCitation6,Citation7 and hormonalCitation8,Citation9 measures). Post-conflict interactions mitigate some of the costs of aggressive conflict.Citation7,Citation10 The majority of research on post-conflict behavior has focused on reconciliation, the affiliative post-conflict interaction between former opponents,Citation11,Citation12 but important post-conflict interactions may also occur between the opponents and bystanders uninvolved in the previous conflict.Citation13,Citation14 In particular, a number of recent studies have demonstrated the phenomenon of post-conflict affiliation directed from a bystander to the recipient of aggression in a variety of species from primatesCitation15Citation25 to dogsCitation26 and rooks.Citation27 This review focuses on bystander to recipient interactions and examines the variations in their patterns, which are consistent with a number of different functional explanations.

When reconciliation was first documented in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Arnhem Zoo by de Waal & van Roosmalen,Citation28 it was also shown that the recipient of aggression was more likely to engage in an affiliative interaction with an uninvolved bystander than by chance. Such interactions were labeled ‘consolation’, assuming that the interaction had a calming function. De Waal and van Roosmalen,Citation28 however, did not test this assumption, nor did they differentiate between affiliative interactions initiated by bystanders and those initiated by the recipient of aggression. In offering post-conflict affiliation to the recipient of aggression, bystanders may provide a benefit in stress-reduction to the recipient, while putting himself or herself at risk of receiving aggression. In fact, they may become targets of aggression either directly, by receiving redirected aggression from the initial recipient of aggression, or indirectly, by becoming involved in renewed aggression between the former opponents while offering post-conflict affiliation. In responding to affiliation solicited by the recipient of aggression, however, the bystander faces little or no risk of redirected aggression. Thus, post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards the recipient of aggression may be qualitatively different from affiliation by the recipient of aggression towards the bystander, and thus a differentiation is frequently made between ‘unsolicited affiliation’, initiated by the bystander, and ‘solicited affiliation’, initiated by the recipient of aggression.Citation29 Such a differentiation has been further supported by the fact that a number of studies have found evidence for the occurrence of post-conflict bystander affiliation solicited by the recipient of aggression but not for similar affiliation offered by the bystander,Citation29Citation31 or for the occurrence of unsolicited affiliation but not affiliation solicited by the recipient of aggression.Citation16,Citation17,Citation19,Citation20

The role of the conflict participant may also be important in discriminating between different post-conflict interactions involving bystanders.Citation13,Citation14 Whereas de Waal and van RoosmalenCitation28 focused on recipients of aggression only, many later studies did not differentiate between recipients of aggression and aggressors. The recipient of aggression is more likely to experience higher levels of post-conflict stress,Citation19,Citation32,Citation33 and thus the potentially stress-alleviating effect of bystander affiliation may offer higher benefits to the recipient of aggression than to the aggressor. Furthermore, the risks of aggression for the bystander on approaching the aggressor may differ from those on approaching the recipient of aggression after a conflict. Moreover, affiliating with the aggressor after a conflict may prove more successful or meaningful with respect to certain potential functions, such as providing encouragement or the strengthening of bonds between alliesCitation14 than affiliating with the recipient of aggression. Nevertheless, some, if not all, of the functions of bystander affiliation discussed in this review may be applicable to both post-conflict interactions directed from bystanders to aggressors and those directed towards recipients of aggression, but our focus is only on the latter.

Following de Waal & Roosmalen'sCitation28 study, the occurrence of post-conflict affiliation initiated by the bystander received further attention when de Waal & AureliCitation34 compared its patterns in chimpanzees with previously published findings in four macaque species.Citation35Citation39 Post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards the recipient of aggression was found to occur in chimpanzees but not in macaques. One possible interpretation of this result is that monkeys may not have the requisite cognitive capacity for this interaction, known as the ‘cognitive constraints hypothesis’.Citation34 In order for bystanders to ‘console’ a recipient of aggression, they are expected to recognize that the recipient is distressed and be able to respond appropriately to alleviate that distress, and thus the absence of ‘consolation’ in monkeys may be due to a difference in empathy attribution in monkeys and apes.Citation40 Japanese macaque mothers do not shows signs of distress or increase affiliative contacts with their offspring when their offspring are targets of aggression,Citation41 suggesting that the mothers may not perceive their offspring's need for distress alleviation and supporting the hypothesis that monkeys are not cognitively capable of ‘consolation’. The degree to which apes are capable of empathy, however, is under debate.Citation40,Citation42Citation46 While differences in cognitive ability may explain differences in the relative occurrence of bystander affiliation in different species when a stress-reduction function is applicable, this should not impact the occurrence of bystander affiliation if and when other functions apply, for which such cognitive abilities are not required.

While simply demonstrating the occurrence of post-conflict interactions may not in itself tell us about their functions, their relative occurrence across species may help us to understand their functional significance. Although some other studies did find evidence for post-conflict affiliation between conflict opponents and bystanders in monkeys, those studies either made no distinction between the aggressor and recipient of aggressionCitation47Citation49 or did not differentiate between solicited and unsolicited bystander affiliation.Citation39 The stress reduction function was not tested in those studies, but a different function for post-conflict affiliation between bystanders and conflict participants was proposed. Such affiliation was proposed to function as ‘triadic reconciliation’Citation39 given that the bystander involved was the opponent's kin. In such cases, the bystander was proposed to act as a proxy for their kin, thus contacts between a conflict participant and their opponent's kin could potentially fulfill the same functions as reconciliation, namely relationship repair and distress alleviation. Although no firm conclusions can be drawn from these studies about the function of post-conflict affiliation directed from the bystander to the recipient of aggression as distinctions between the roles of the conflict participant were not made and/or the initiator of the contact was not identified, it seems possible that a relationship repair function could apply to such affiliation.

The first, and indeed the only, study to definitively demonstrate the occurrence of post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards the recipient of aggression in primate species other than apes (although such contacts have been recently demonstrated in domestic dogsCitation26 and rooksCitation27), did so only by analyzing different types of affiliative behaviors separately.Citation25 Stumptailed macaque (Macaca arctoides) victims were found to receive an increased level of post-conflict affiliation from bystanders in the form of sociosexual behaviors, but not allo-grooming. The authors argued that if the function of such contacts were stress reduction, as is implied by the term ‘consolation’, allo-grooming would be expected to increase, as this behavior has been shown to reduce heart rate and tension-related activities.Citation50,Citation51 Furthermore, as kin are likely to have a valuable relationship and valuable partners are more likely to be responsive to each others' distress, kin would be expected to be most likely to provide consolation. However, kin of the recipient of aggression did not provide the post-conflict affiliation. The authors instead proposed another function for post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards the recipient of aggression: self-protection. One way for the recipient of aggression to reduce post-conflict stress is to redirect aggression towards other group members.Citation38,Citation52 Thus, by affiliating with the recipient of aggression, the bystander may be attempting to appease them and reduce the likelihood of becoming a target of redirected aggression.

Post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards the recipient of aggression may therefore have a number of different functions. Hence, the functional label ‘consolation’ first attributed to this type of post-conflict interaction may not always be appropriate, and should only be used where a stress-alleviating function has been shown. More neutral terms such as post-conflict triadic or third-party affiliation have been used in recent studies.Citation17,Citation19,Citation25,Citation53 As these terms do not specify the direction of the interaction or the role of the conflict participant, we have chosen to use the term ‘post-conflict bystander affiliation directed towards recipients of aggression’ (hereafter referred to as ‘bystander affiliation’, with the implicit assumption that we are only referring to affiliation initiated by the bystander and directed towards the recipient of aggression). In order for the occurrence of bystander affiliation to be determined in a population, the distribution and latency of affiliative contacts during post-conflict periods and during matched control observations must be compared separately according to the initiator of the interaction and the role of the conflict participant. Bystander affiliation is demonstrated in a population when it can be shown that bystanders are likely to initiate affiliative contact with the recipient of aggression earlier in the post-conflict periods than in control periods. Using this definition, bystander affiliation has thus far only been demonstrated in chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas, stump-tailed macaques, rooks and dogs (see ), although its occurrence has been investigated, but not found, in a number of other species.Citation13 Although studies of bystander affiliation initially focused primarily on the demonstration of the interaction, the attention has now shifted more towards the functional aspects of bystander affiliation, and therefore the importance of distinguishing the role of the conflict opponent and the initiator of bystander affiliation has only relatively recently become apparent. Thus, comparisons among different ‘types’ of bystander affiliation, i.e., among instances of bystander affiliation fulfilling (at least potentially) different functions, have not yet been made. Below, we review evidence for the different functions of bystander affiliation in light of recent developments in the field, and propose a predictive framework for the occurrence of these functions.

Stress Reduction

Consolation, the original term used for bystander affiliation, implies a stress reduction function, but for almost 30 years it was not formally tested. The first study to actually investigate this function found no evidence that bystander affiliation reduces behavioral indicators of stress in chimpanzees.Citation19 More recently, however, another study on chimpanzees found that bystander affiliation did reduce levels of behavioral indicators of stress,Citation54 suggesting that bystander affiliation does have a stress reduction function, at least in some cases. Both of these studies used levels of self-directed behavior (self-scratching and/or self-grooming) to measure levels of stress. While direct evidence that these behaviors are linked to stress-related physiological changes is lacking, there is a growing body of circumstantial evidence that suggests that self-directed behaviors are reliable indicators of stress and anxiety in primates.Citation55,Citation56 Indirect physiological evidence is provided by the fact that allo-grooming reduces both self-directed behaviorsCitation50 and heart rate.Citation51,Citation57 There is also pharmacological evidence to support the link, as anxiogenic and anxiolytic drugs selectively increase and decrease levels of self-directed behavior respectively.Citation58,Citation59 Furthermore, levels of self-grooming and self-scratching have been used successfully in a number of studies across primate species to investigate the stress-inducing and stress-alleviating effects of aggressive conflict and post-conflict interactions and variations in stress levels therein.Citation32,Citation33,Citation60Citation65 Moreover, the stress-alleviating effect of reconciliation in human children assessed using levels of self-directed behaviorCitation64 has now been confirmed based on analysis of salivary cortisol levels.Citation8 As the evidence for the stress alleviation effect is still limited, more studies are needed to address the link between post-conflict bystander affiliation and stress reduction, preferably using both behavioral and physiological measures of stress in the same study.

Self Protection

Call et al.Citation25 suggested that bystander affiliation in stumptailed macaques might serve a self-protective rather than stress-reducing function on the basis that such contacts consisted of socio-sexual behaviors, which are not likely to alleviate stress, and were not provided by the recipient of aggression's kin, which are expected to be the partners most likely to provide stress alleviation. No evidence was found, however, that the recipients of aggression redirected aggression towards bystanders after conflicts. In contrast, Koski and SterckCitation66 found that in chimpanzees, bystanders were less likely to become targets of redirected aggression if they affiliated with the recipient of aggression after a conflict. Original recipients of aggression were equally likely to redirect aggression after receiving affiliation from bystanders, but directed aggression towards group members other than the bystander from whom they received post-conflict affiliation.Citation66 As bystander affiliation was not shown to reduce behavioral indicators of stress in this group of chimpanzees,Citation19 it seems unlikely that it simultaneously serves a self-protection and stress-reduction function, but rather that these functions represent separate post-conflict strategies. Moreover, when bystander affiliation was shown to have a self-protective function, it was unlikely to be provided by kin,Citation66 whereas, in another group of chimpanzees, bystander affiliation with a stress-reducing function was offered by valuable partners.Citation54

The social constraints hypothesisCitation34 predicts, as an alternative to the cognitive constraints hypothesis, that inter-species differences in the occurrence of bystander affiliation are due to the differential risk of aggression for potential bystanders on approaching the original recipient of aggression. Following this hypothesis, bystander affiliation is more likely to occur in species with more tolerant social systems, where the risks of further aggression involving the recipient are lower. The apparent intra-specific differences in functions of bystander affiliation in chimpanzees may provide support for the social constraints hypothesis. Bystander affiliation provided by valuable partners was shown to reduce levels of behavioral indicators of stress in one group of chimpanzees, where redirected aggression was virtually absent,Citation54 whereas a stress-alleviating effect was not found in a group of chimpanzees with a relatively higher rate of redirected aggression,Citation19 and bystander affiliation in that group was likely to be offered by those most at risk of becoming targets of redirected aggression.Citation66 Thus, differences in the function of bystander affiliation may stem from a differential risk of aggressive conflict for the bystanders.

Opponent Relationship Repair

Affiliative post-conflict interactions between a conflict participant and their opponent's kin, or triadic reconciliations, have been well documented.Citation13,Citation14,Citation39,Citation47Citation49 They have been suggested to function as a substitute for reconciliation when the risk of renewed aggression between the opponents is too high.Citation16 Thus, relatives of a conflict participant may approach and affiliate with its former opponent and in doing so act on its behalf, potentially repairing the former opponents' relationship and reducing post-conflict stress. In most studies it was unclear whether such interactions were initiated by bystanders or conflict participants, and directed towards the recipient or initiator of aggression. The only study that distinguished between the aggressor and recipient of aggression found post-conflict affiliative interactions only between aggressors and their opponents' kin.Citation39 A recent study, however, has shown that bystander affiliation directed from the aggressor's kin to the recipient of aggression may indeed repair the opponents' relationship.Citation24 Using an experimental set-up that exploited baboons' (Papio hamadryas ursinus) use of affiliative vocalizations for reconciliation, playback recordings of affiliative vocalizations from the aggressor's kin after a conflict lead to the recipient of aggression displaying increased tolerance for the aggressor's presence. Thus, bystander affiliation may function not only as a stress-alleviating or self-protecting mechanism, but may also function to repair the opponents' relationship.Citation24

Such a function may not be limited to bystander affiliation from the aggressor's kin. In species in which high-value relationships exist between unrelated individuals, valuable partners may also be able to act on the aggressor's behalf. In chimpanzees, bystander affiliation has been shown to occur between aggressors' valuable partners and recipients of aggression.Citation67

Other Functions

While the stress-reduction, self-protection and relationship-repair functions of bystander affiliation have received the most attention, they are almost certainly not its only functions. That aggressive conflict affects the behavior of individuals other than those directly involved in the conflict is well known, but post-conflict interactions may also have a wider effect than is immediately apparent. Bystander affiliation in chimpanzees has been found to reduce the likelihood of further aggression among group members,Citation18 and thus might function to prevent the diffusion of conflict throughout the group.

Research on corvids has lead to the suggestion that bystander affiliation may also function to show support for valuable partners or to strengthen bonds between allies and advertise that bond to others,Citation27 although in these cases affiliation was initiated by either the bystander or the recipient of aggression.

Interdependency of Bystander Affiliation & Reconciliation

Bystander affiliation is only one of a number of post-conflict interactions,Citation16,Citation68 and as such we may be able to understand more about the functions of bystander affiliation by examining it in the context of post-conflict strategies as a whole rather than in isolation. The stress-reduction and relationship-repair functions of bystander affiliation, in particular, overlap with those of reconciliation, and thus it seems likely that the occurrence of reconciliation and bystander affiliation (at least when those functions are applicable) are linked. Reconciliation is expected to be the first priority for opponents, as it is likely to be most effective in mitigating the negative consequences of aggressive conflict, by repairing the relationship between opponents and reducing post-conflict stress. Reconciliation does, however, entail risks of renewed aggression,Citation7 and thus may not occur when the value of the opponents' relationship is low or the risk of renewed aggression is high, as the costs would outweigh the benefits.Citation7,Citation69 Bystander affiliation may thus, under some circumstances, function as an alternative to reconciliation in situations when approaching the former opponent is likely to result in renewed aggression.Citation16 Accordingly, interdependency of post-conflict interactions is expected when the functions of bystander affiliation and reconciliation overlap. Indeed bystander affiliation was found to be more likely to occur in the absence of reconciliation and vice-versa when a stress-reduction functionCitation20 and a relationship-repair functionCitation16 were likely, suggesting that bystander affiliation under those circumstances occurs when reconciliation is too risky. A similar interdependence between bystander affiliation and reconciliation was found when bystander affiliation was shown to reduce further aggression in the whole group,Citation18 and when a function was not determined.Citation23

As the self-protection function of bystander affiliation primarily benefits the bystander rather than the recipient of aggression, the effects of this form of bystander affiliation and reconciliation might not be linked. Indeed, no interdependency between the two events was found when a self-protection function was shown in chimpanzees.Citation68 However, a more subtle form of interdependency may be present. As self-protective bystander affiliation functions to reduce the chances of redirected aggression, the patterns of interdependency between self-protective bystander affiliation and reconciliation may be mediated by the occurrence of redirected aggression. Thus, if the occurrence of redirected aggression and reconciliation are interdependent, as has been found in some species,Citation38 then bystander affiliation occurring when the risks of redirected aggression are high may be indirectly linked to the occurrence of reconciliation. When multiple post-conflict interactions occur after a single conflict, however, the likelihood that once a post-conflict interaction takes place, any subsequent interactions occur as a result of the first post-conflict interaction, rather than as a direct result of the preceding conflict, should be considered.

Bystander Affiliation and Relationship Quality

Our review has highlighted that post-conflict affiliation directed from a bystander to a recipient of aggression can have multiple functions. These functions are not species-specific, but rather this behavior may have different functions within the same species and even within the same group. Relationship quality may be the key to understanding the function of each occurrence of bystander affiliation. The quality of the relationship between the opponents determines the costs and benefits of reconciliation, which in turn may determine the costs and benefits of bystander affiliation from the recipient's point of view. Similarly, the relationship between the bystander and the recipient may determine the costs and benefits of affiliation from the bystander's point of view. Thus, in order to understand the functions and determinants of bystander affiliation, it is important to investigate the quality of the relationships involved. In this context it is useful to view relationship quality as consisting of three separate components, relationship value, compatibility and security.Citation69 Value refers to the benefits afforded by the relationship. Compatibility is a measure of the general tenor of social interactions, or the tolerance of the dyad. Security refers to the predictability of a partner's behavior, or is a measure of the consistency of interactions between partners over time.

Bystander affiliation is likely to function as consolation, or have a stress reduction function, when conflict has lead to elevated stress levels and when reconciliation is less likely to occur. Thus, in these circumstances the relationship between opponents is likely to be of low value, and thus not worth repairing, and/or of low compatibility, leading to an increased risk of renewed aggression. Furthermore, the relationship between the bystander and the recipient of aggression is likely to be highly valuable when bystander affiliation has a stress reduction function, as valuable partners are expected to be more responsive to each other's distress.Citation70 Moreover, valuable partners are likely to exchange affiliation reciprocally, and as such may be more likely to offer stress-reducing bystander affiliation, as bystanders are unlikely to gain direct benefits from such interactions. Indeed, Fraser et al.Citation54 found that bystander affiliation was most likely to be offered by those who shared a valuable relationship with the recipient of aggression in chimpanzees, and bystander affiliation in rooks occurs only between mating partners.Citation27 If the stress reduction function of bystander affiliation applies mainly to valuable partners, it seems likely that it would be difficult to detect a stress reduction effect if such an effect were investigated across the group as a whole rather than within just those dyads who share valuable relationships. This may have happened in the case of Koski & Sterck'sCitation19 study in which a stress-reduction function for bystander affiliation was not found (see below), although a stress reduction effect was not found even when only bystander affiliation provided by kin was considered. That bystander affiliation is provided by valuable partners and reduces stress levelsCitation54 supports the hypothesis that ‘consolation’ may be an expression of empathy. The mechanism behind bystander affiliation, however, cannot be determined by examining its function, and more needs to be understood about the benefits afforded to the bystander by consoling the recipient of aggression for us to understand the bystander's motivation to console.

The self-protection function of bystander affiliation is likely to apply when the relationship between the bystander and the recipient of aggression is of low value and low compatibility, leading to a high probability that the bystander will become the target of redirected aggression. The relationship between the opponents is likely to depend on the functions and patterns of redirected aggression. If redirected aggression provides an alternative stress-alleviation mechanism to reconciliation,Citation60 then the relationship between opponents is likely to be of low compatibility as the risks of renewed aggression upon reconciliation may be too high. However, as redirected aggression may lead to an increased likelihood of reconciliation,Citation38 the value of the opponents' relationship may be high as the benefits of reconciliation appear to outweigh the risks once redirected aggression has occurred. Thus, the occurrence of self-protective bystander affiliation is likely linked to risks of aggressive conflict for both the original recipient of aggression and for the bystander. Interestingly, when a self-protection function was likely for most cases of bystander affiliation in a chimpanzee group, it seemed unlikely to apply to bystander affiliation offered by kin of the recipient of aggression, as the risks of redirected aggression towards those individuals was very low.Citation66 Thus, while bystander affiliation offered by those at risk of becoming targets of redirected aggression may function as a self-protective mechanism, bystander affiliation from kin may serve another function, such as distress alleviation.

When opponent relationship repair is the likely function of bystander affiliation, the relationship between the bystander and the aggressor is valuable enough to serve as a proxy for the recipient. It is also likely that the relationship between the opponents is of low compatibility, and thus the risks of renewed aggression are too high for direct reconciliation to take place. The relationship between opponents is nevertheless likely to be valuable enough for relationship repair to be worthwhile. The relationship between the bystander and the aggressor is also likely to be more valuable than the relationship between the bystander and the recipient of aggression, as was found when a relationship repair function was likely for bystander affiliation in chimpanzees.Citation67

Measuring Relationship Quality

The importance of each of the components of relationship quality, especially between opponents, in shaping post-conflict behavior is well established,Citation69,Citation71 yet the methods used for measuring relationship quality components are varied and, for the most part, offer an incomplete view of the true quality of the relationship between partners.Citation72 Common methods include assigning dyads to relationship quality classifications according to their sex- and/or age-combinationCitation17,Citation73Citation75 or according to whether they are kin or non-kin.Citation39,Citation76Citation79 While this can be effective, this method does not take into account the considerable variation in the quality of relationships that may exist within these broad categories. Furthermore, the class of relationship quality assigned to each category (such as classifying a particular sex-combination as having valuable relationships) is frequently chosen without adequate justification in the study group (e.g., on what basis are all dyads with that sex-combination thought to have more valuable relationships than other sex-combinations). Levels of certain behaviors deemed to represent certain components of relationship quality are also frequently employed, such as using agonistic support as a measure of relationship value.Citation71 Single behaviors, however, may not provide enough information for a true evaluation of the quality of the relationship between individuals, and the assignment of behaviors to particular components can be contentious, as for example, grooming has be used to represent either relationship valueCitation80 or compatibility.Citation53,Citation68 Thus, in order to gain more accurate, quantitative estimate of each of the relationship quality components, which is vital if the functions of bystander affiliation are to be fully understood, relationship quality must be measured using improved techniques. Wittig & BoeschCitation81 suggested that relationship value is best reflected by a combination of several benefits that emerge directly from the value of the relationship and not from the frequency of affiliative interactions and based their measure on levels of both food-sharing and agonistic support. Fraser et al.Citation72 went a step further towards obtaining composite, quantitative, behavioral measures for each component of relationship quality using principal component analysis. Their study showed that indeed relationship value, compatibility and security each describes a separate aspect of relationship quality and can be measured using different behavioral indicators.

Bystander Affiliation Form and Function

Examining the behaviors used for bystander affiliation may also provide clues as to its function, particularly for those species showing rich behavioral variation in the form of their post-conflict interactions. Call et al.Citation25 were only able to demonstrate the occurrence of bystander affiliation in stumptailed macaques when grooming and socio-sexual behaviors were analyzed separately. Only socio-sexual behaviors were used by the macaques for bystander affiliation, possibly because these behaviors would be more effective in achieving the function of self-protection, while minimizing the risk of aggression, as these are brief behaviors that require the partners to spend only a very short period of time in contact with each other. Valuable partners can afford to use potentially more risky behaviors (i.e., those that involve close contact, exposure of sensitive parts of the body and those that make it difficult to flee), as the risk of being attacked is minimal. Thus, for bystander affiliation with a stress reduction function, reassuring, close affiliative contacts are likely to be used.Citation20

Conclusions

Post-conflict bystander affiliation directed at the recipient of aggression may have a number of functions, including, but not exclusively, stress-reduction, self-protection and opponent relationship repair. In addition to studying the consequences of the interaction, a key aspect to understanding these functions is the quality of the relationship within the triad (i.e., the former aggressor, the former recipient of aggression and the bystander). Variation in relationship quality, even within a population, seems to be strongly associated with different functions for what outwardly appear to be very similar behaviors. Thus far, very few studies have investigated the function of post-conflict bystander affiliation, and the occurrence of bystander affiliation in non-primate species is largely unknown. Many more studies, across a wide range of species, are needed before we can fully understand how and why bystanders affiliate with recipients of aggression after a conflict. Future studies should, where possible, investigate the functions of bystander affiliation separately according to qualities of the relationships between opponents and between the bystander and each opponent, as well as according to the forms of affiliative behaviors. Terminology used should reflect the potential for a number of different functions, and thus functional terminology, such as the use of the term ‘consolation’, should only be used where such a function has been demonstrated. Bystander affiliation is not a single post-conflict interaction, but represents a number of different strategies for the management of conflict, and the functions proposed here may also apply to post-conflict affiliation from bystanders to aggressors. As such, it is important that bystander affiliation is not investigated in isolation but in the context of post-conflict behavior as a whole.

Figures and Tables

Table 1 Species in which post-conflict affiliation directed from a bystander to the recipient of aggression has been demonstrated and the suggested function of such interactions

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for their useful comments on this manuscript.

References

  • de Waal FBM. Aureli F, de Waal FBM. The first kiss: foundations of conflict resolution research in animals. Natural Conflict Resolution 2000; Berkeley University of California Press 15 - 33
  • Cords M. Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques. Anim Behav 1992; 44:57 - 61
  • Koyama NF. The long-term effects of reconciliation in Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata. Ethology 2001; 107:975 - 987
  • Wittig RM, Boesch C. How to repair relationships—reconciliation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 2005; 111:736 - 763
  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Reconciliatory grunts by dominant female baboons influence victims' behaviour. Anim Behav 1997; 54:409 - 418
  • Aureli F, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Am J Primatol 1989; 19:39 - 51
  • Aureli F, Cords M, van Schaik CP. Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim Behav 2002; 64:325 - 343
  • Butovskaya M. Reconciliation, dominance and cortisol levels in children and adolescents (7–15-year-old boys). Behaviour 2008; 145:1557 - 1576
  • Crockford C, Wittig RM, Whitten PL, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Social stressors and coping mechanisms in wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Horm Behav 2008; 53:254 - 265
  • de Waal FBM. Primates—a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science 2000; 289:586 - 590
  • Aureli F, de Waal FBM. Natural Conflict Resolution 2000; Berkeley University of California Press
  • Arnold K, Aureli F. Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK. Postconflict reconciliation. Primates in Perspective 2007; Oxford Oxford University Press 592 - 608
  • Watts DP, Colmenares F, Arnold K. Aureli F, de Waal FBM. Redirection, consolation and male policing: how targets of aggression interact with bystanders. Natural Conflict Resolution 2000; Berkeley University of California Press 281 - 301
  • Das M. Aureli F, de Waal FBM. Conflict management via third parties: post-conflict affiliation of the aggressor. Natural Conflict Resolution 2000; Berkeley University of California Press 263 - 280
  • Fujisawa KK, Kutsukake N, Hasegawa T. Peacemaking and Consolation in Japanese Preschoolers Witnessing Peer Aggression. J Comp Psychol 2006; 120:48 - 57
  • Wittig RM, Boesch C. The choice of post-conflict interactions in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behaviour 2003; 140:1527 - 1559
  • Kutsukake N, Castles DL. Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates 2004; 45:157 - 165
  • Palagi E, Cordoni G, Borgognini Tarli S. Possible roles of consolation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am J Phys Anthropol 2006; 129:105 - 111
  • Koski SE, Sterck EHM. Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation console?. Anim Behav 2007; 73:133 - 142
  • Fraser ON, Aureli F. Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 2008; 70:1114 - 1123
  • Cordoni G, Palagi E, Tarli S. Reconciliation and consolation in captive western gorillas. Int J Primatol 2006; 27:1365 - 1382
  • Mallavarapu S, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL. Postconflict behavior in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Am J Primatol 2006; 68:789 - 801
  • Palagi E, Paoli T, Borgognini Tarli SM. Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Am J Primatol 2004; 62:15 - 30
  • Wittig RM, Crockford C, Wikberg E, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Kin-mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2007; 274:1109 - 1115
  • Call J, Aureli F, de Waal FBM. Postconflict third party affiliation in stumptailed macaques. Anim Behav 2002; 63:209 - 216
  • Cools AKA, van Hout AJM, Nelissen MHJ. Canine reconciliation and third-party-initiated postconflict affiliation: do peacemaking social mechanisms in dogs rival those of higher primates?. Ethology 2008; 114:53 - 63
  • Seed AM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ. Postconflict third-party affiliation in rooks, Corvus frugilegus. Curr Biol 2007; 17:152 - 158
  • de Waal FBM, van Roosmalen A. Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1979; 5:55 - 66
  • Verbeek P, de Waal FBM. Postconflict behavior of captive brown capuchins in the presence and absence of attractive food. Int J Primatol 1997; 18:703 - 725
  • Petit O, Thierry B. Roeder JJ, Thierry B, Anderson JR, Herrenschmidt N. Reconciliation in Guinea baboons (Papio papio). Current Biology 1994; 2:Strasbourg Université Louis Pasteur 137 - 145
  • Arnold K, Barton R. Postconflict behavior of spectacled leaf monkeys (Trachypithecus obscurus) II. Contact with third parties. Int J Primatol 2001; 22:267 - 286
  • Cooper M, Aureli F, Singh M. Sex differences in reconciliation and post-conflict anxiety in bonnet macaques. Ethology 2007; 113:26 - 38
  • Schino G, Rosati L, Geminiani S, Aureli F. Post-conflict anxiety in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): aggressor's and victim's perspectives. Ethology 2007; 113:1081 - 1088
  • de Waal FBM, Aureli F. Russon AE, Bard KA, Taylor Parker S. Consolation, reconciliation and a possible cognitive difference between macaques and chimpanzees. Reaching into thought: The minds of great apes 1996; Cambridge Cambridge University Press 80 - 110
  • Aureli F, Das M, van Eck CJV, Veenema HC, Verleur D, van Hooff JARAM. Post-conflict affiliative interactions among Japanese and Barbary macaques. Aggress Behav 1993; 19:49 - 50
  • Aureli F. Post-conflict behavior among wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 1992; 31:329 - 337
  • Aureli F, Das M, Verleur D, van Hooff JARAM. Postconflict social interactions among Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Int J Primatol 1994; 15:471 - 485
  • Aureli F, van Schaik CP. Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis): I. The social events. Ethology 1991; 89:89 - 100
  • Judge PG. Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Am J Primatol 1991; 23:225 - 237
  • de Waal FBM. Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Ann Rev Psychol 2008; 59:1 - 22
  • Schino G, Geminiani S, Rosati L, Aureli F. Behavioral and emotional response of Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) mothers after their offspring receive an aggression. J Comp Psychol 2004; 118:340 - 346
  • Preston SD, de Waal FBM. Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci 2002; 25:1 - 72
  • Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, et al. Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature 2005; 437:1357 - 1359
  • Jensen K, Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M. What's in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2006; 273:1013 - 1021
  • Warneken F, Hare B, Melis AP, Hanus D, Tomasello M. Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biol 2007; 5:1414 - 1420
  • Penn DC, Povinelli DJ. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely ressembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2007; 362:731 - 744
  • York AD, Rowell TE. Reconciliation following aggression in patas monkeys, Erythrocebus patas. Anim Behav 1988; 36:502
  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Redirected aggression and reconciliation among vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops. Behaviour 1989; 110:258 - 275
  • Castles DL, Whiten A. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons I. Reconciliation, redirection and consolation. Ethology 1998; 104:126 - 147
  • Schino G, Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Turillazzi PG. Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism—a behavioral approach. Am J Primatol 1988; 16:43 - 50
  • Aureli F, Preston SD, de Waal FBM. Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): a pilot study. J Comp Psychol 1999; 113:59 - 65
  • Bastock M, Morris D, Moynihan M. Some comments on conflict and frustration in animals. Behaviour 1953; 6:66 - 84
  • Arnold K, Whiten A. Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo forest, Uganda. Behaviour 2001; 138:649 - 690
  • Fraser ON, Stahl D, Aureli F. Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008; 105:8557 - 8562
  • Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. A modest proposal—displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Anim Behav 1992; 44:967 - 979
  • Troisi A. Displacement activities as a behavioural measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects. Stress 2002; 5:47 - 54
  • Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiol Behav 1989; 45:667 - 670
  • Barros M, Boere V, Huston JP, Tomaz C. Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset (Callithrix penicillata) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam. Behav Brain Res 2000; 108:205 - 211
  • Schino G, Perretta G, Taglioni A, Monaco V, Troisi A. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 1996; 2:186 - 191
  • Aureli F, van Schaik CP. Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis): II. Coping with uncertainty. Ethology 1991; 89:101 - 114
  • Castles DL, Whiten A. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons II: Stress & self- directed behaviour. Ethology 1998; 104:148 - 160
  • Das M, Penke Z, Van Hooff JARAM. Postconflict affiliation and stress-related behavior of long-tailed macaque aggressors. Int J Primatol 1998; 19:53 - 71
  • Kutsukake N, Castles DL. Reconciliation and variation in post-conflict stress in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata): testing the integrated hypothesis. Anim Cogn 2001; 4:259 - 268
  • Fujisawa KK, Kutskake N, Hasegawa T. Reconciliation pattern after aggression among Japanese school children. Aggress Behav 2005; 31:138 - 152
  • Koski SE, Koops K, Sterck EHM. Reconciliation, relationship quality and postconflict anxiety: testing the integrated hypothesis in captive chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 2007; 69:158 - 172
  • Koski SE, Sterck EH. Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: What's in it for the third party?. Am J Primatol 2009; 71:409 - 418
  • Wittig RM. Lonsdorf AV, Ross SR, Matsuzawa T. Function and cognitive underpinning of post-conflict affiliation in wild chimpanzees. The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives 2009; Chicago Chicago University Press
  • Koski SE, de Vries H, van den Tweel SW, Sterck EHM. What to do after a fight? The determinants and inter-dependency of post-conflict interactions in chimpanzees. Behaviour 2007; 144:529 - 555
  • Cords M, Aureli F. Aureli F, de Waal FBM. Reconciliation and relationship qualities. Natural Conflict Resolution 2000; Berkeley University of California Press 177 - 198
  • Aureli F, Schaffner C. Relationship assessment through emotional mediation. Behaviour 2002; 139:393 - 420
  • Watts DP. Conflict resolution in chimpanzees and the valuable-relationships hypothesis. Int J Primatol 2006; 27:1337 - 1364
  • Fraser ON, Schino G, Aureli F. Components of relationship quality in chimpanzees. Ethology 2008; 114:834 - 843
  • Manson JH, Perry S, Stahl D. Reconciliation in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol 2005; 65:205 - 219
  • Watts DP. Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas II. Redirection, side-direction and consolation. Ethology 1995; 100:158 - 174
  • de Waal FBM. The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. Q Rev Biol 1986; 61:459 - 479
  • de Waal FBM, Yoshihara D. Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour 1983; 85:224 - 241
  • Aureli F, Das M, Veenema HC. Differential kinship effect on reconciliation in three species of macaques (Macaca fascicularis, M. fuscata and M. sylvanus). J Comp Psychol 1997; 111:91 - 99
  • Mitani JC, Merriwether DA, Zhang C. Male affiliation, cooperation and kinship in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 2000; 59:885 - 893
  • Chapais B, Savard L, Gauthier C. Kin selection and the distribution of altruism in relation to degree of kinship in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2001; 49:493 - 502
  • Majolo B, Ventura R, Koyama NF. Postconflict behaviour among male Japanese macaques. Int J Primatol 2005; 26:321 - 336
  • Wittig RM, Boesch C. ‘Decision-making’ in conflicts of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): an extension of the Relational Model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2003; 54:491 - 504