837
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Article Addendum

Asymmetry in olfactory generalization and the inclusion criterion in ants

Article: e29163 | Received 17 Apr 2014, Accepted 08 May 2014, Published online: 12 May 2014

Abstract

Animals constantly face the challenge of extracting important information out of their environment, and for many animals much of this information is chemical in nature. The ability to discriminate and generalize between chemical stimuli is extremely important and is commonly thought to depend mostly on the structural similarity between the different stimuli. However, we previously provided evidence that in the carpenter ant Camponotus aethiops, generalization not only depends on structural similarity, but also on the animal’s previous training experience. When individual ants were conditioned to substance A, they generalized toward a mixture of A and B. However, when trained to substance B, they did not generalize toward this mixture, resulting in asymmetrical generalization. This asymmetry followed an inclusion criterion, where the ants consistently generalized from a molecule with a long carbon chain to molecules with a shorter chain, but not the other way around. Here I will review the evidence for the inclusion criterion, describe possible proximate mechanisms underlying this phenomenon as well as discuss its potential adaptive significance.

Communication is vital for all types of social interaction, and is achieved by the interplay of three separate components, previously described for recognition systems:Citation1 the expression, perception and action components. The expression component occurs when an organism produces a signal or cue (e.g., chemical, visual, acoustic). After expression, a receiver individual detects this product (perception component) and could then alter its behavior, based on the newly acquired information (action component).

Chemical communication is perhaps the most widespread means of communication in the animal world.Citation2 In insects, the importance of odour detection is highlighted by the fact that olfactory receptor genes are one of the largest families of genes, and underwent rapid evolutionary change.Citation3 In the olfactory modality of insects, the perception component can be divided into multiple steps.Citation4,Citation5 First, odorant molecules are picked up by sensilla present on the antennae. These sensilla contain Odorant Binding Proteins (OBPs) and Chemosensory Proteins (CSPsCitation6,Citation7), both of which can bind specific odorant molecules and transport it through the lymph fluid to the Olfactory Receptors (ORs), located on the Olfactory Receptor Neurons (ORNs). These ORNs then transmit a signal to the antennal lobes, the first-order integration center of the insect central nervous system. The antennal lobe consists of many glomeruli, which are synaptic aggregations where ORNs are connected to local interneurons and projection neurons. These local interneurons modulate the response of other glomeruli through inhibition or stimulation. Afterwards, the projection neurons of the glomeruli transfer the modified, integrated information to the higher brain centers (mushroom bodies and lateral horn). These brain centers integrate all received information (olfactory, visual etc.), and regulate for example motor neurons, resulting in a behavioral response (action component).

A particularly fascinating area of study is that of olfactory generalization, which occurs when an individual treats two structurally different odorants as similar. Olfactory generalization can arise from either the perception or action component of communication. If it is due to the perception component, the animal can simply not distinguish between the two stimuli. If it arises from the action component, the animal actually does perceive a difference between the two stimuli, but treats them as being similar.

Asymmetry in Generalization and the Inclusion Criterion

We previously studied generalization of odorants in the carpenter ant Camponotus aethiops.Citation8,Citation9 By using an associative learning protocol known as the Maxilla-Labium Extension Response (MaLERCitation10), we conditioned individual ants by training them to for example substance A, and afterwards tested their response to a mixture of A and a novel substance B. Ants trained to substance A treated a mixture of A and B as similar, but surprisingly individuals trained to B did not generalize toward the mixture of A and B, indicating that generalization between stimuli is not only influenced by the similarity of molecular structure of the odorants involved, but also by the animal’s previous experience.

While asymmetry in generalization itself has previously been found in many organisms, especially in the olfactory modality (e.g.Citation11-Citation15), most of these studies either used very few substances, or substances that greatly differed in their molecular structure or function (e.g., floral odour vs. alarm pheromone), making it impossible to draw conclusions about the molecular basis of the asymmetry.

In order to address this question, our experimental setup allowed us to describe how molecular structure systematically plays a role in predicting whether the ants would generalize or not. The ants consistently generalized from molecules with a long carbon chain to mixtures containing this trained substance and a molecule with a shorter chain. However, when trained to a molecule with a short chain, no generalization to a mixture containing this trained substance and a molecule with a longer chain was found. Additionally, we elucidated a hierarchy of stimuli regarding functional groups, where ants trained to alcohols generalized to aldehydes and ketones of similar or shorter length. When trained to aldehydes, ants would generalize to ketones of similar or shorter length, but not to alcohols. Lastly, when trained to ketones ants would not generalize to molecules with other functional groups at all. This asymmetry thus follows an inclusion criterion (a term adapted from Guerrieri et al.Citation16), where substances with shorter chain lengths or certain functional groups are treated as if similar to the trained substance.

This raises the question about whether the inclusion criterion is a general phenomenon in the animal world, or whether it is exclusive to ants. Two pioneering studies by Smith and MenzelCitation11 and Guerrieri et al.Citation15 in honeybees allowed for extensive analysis of asymmetry in generalization with regards to molecular structure, as both studies used different volatile floral odours varying in either chain length or functional group. Although asymmetry in generalization was found in these studies, it could not systematically be predicted by molecular structure, thus not supporting the inclusion criterion. However, in these studies the test stimulus was always a single substance instead of a mixture of the conditioned stimulus and the test stimulus. In these conditions, ants also show no evidence of the inclusion criterion.Citation9 As generalization to a mixture after training to a single substance has, to my knowledge, not been studied in a systematic way in other animals, it is currently impossible to draw conclusions on how widespread the inclusion criterion is.

In ants, a similar phenomenon of inclusion appears to come into play in nestmate recognition, where non-nestmates (enemies) are aggressed only if their cuticular chemical profile contains additional substances in comparison to the profile of the discriminating individual, but are not recognized when they have less substances.Citation16,Citation17 Indeed, some social parasites use this to their advantage, and are known to be chemically insignificant, having only a very small amount of substances on their cuticle, which ensures that they are not recognized by the host colony.Citation18 This implies that the inclusion criterion might be a general phenomenon, at least in ants.

Does it Arise from the Perception or Action Component?

Now that we established that, at least in the carpenter ant C. aethiops, asymmetry in generalisation follows an inclusion criterion, the question remains about whether it arises from the perception or action component of communication.

If inclusion arises at the perception component, the receiving individual does not notice the difference between the trained odorant and the substance or mixture that it generalizes toward. However, this does not necessarily mean the odour is not detected by the sensilla, as inclusion could potentially result from processes at different levels of the perception component.

The first level is that of the OBPs and CSPs in the sensilla. These sensory proteins are normally tuned to specific substances. Intuitively, one could predict that these proteins might also bind smaller substances due to the size of their binding pocket (while not binding larger molecules than the one they are tuned to). In turn, generalization toward a molecule smaller than the conditioned stimulus might be expected, as these proteins would bind to both to the trained substance, as well as smaller ones. However, evidence points in the other direction, where sensory proteins tuned to a certain substance bind, next to the molecule they are tuned to, larger molecules better than smaller ones.19,20,but see Citation21 If larger molecules indeed activate proteins tuned to smaller molecules but not the other way around, a binary mixture containing a large and small molecule could activate the same sensory proteins as the large molecule alone, potentially explaining our results. Alternatively, not the binding pocket, but the chemical attributes of the odorants might cause asymmetry in generalization. Nearly all ligand interactions rely, to some extent, on hydrogen bonding. For example, there is a difference in the capability of alcohols and aldehydes/ketones to form hydrogen bonds. The oxygen in all of these classes of molecules can build a hydrogen bond with the hydrogen atoms of the sensory protein, but only alcohols can also build a hydrogen bond with the protein’s oxygen through the hydrogen in the alcohol’s -OH group.Citation22 This might cause alcohols to bind better than aldehydes and ketones, possibly explaining the hierarchy of stimuli found in our study, where generalization occurred from alcohols to aldehydes and ketones, but not the other way around.

The second level is that of the olfactory receptors and the antennal lobe. Every odour elicits a unique pattern (much like a barcode) in the antennal lobe by activating a specific subset of glomeruli.Citation23,Citation24 If higher chain length molecules elicit the same pattern as their lower chain length counterparts, but activate some additional glomeruli in the process, this could give rise to asymmetry in generalisation, as the activation of additional compounds seems to enable discrimination, whereas the deactivation of many glomeruli does not.Citation25 However, smaller molecules do not necessarily activate less glomeruli, at least in the honey bee.Citation24,Citation26 Furthermore, mixtures are not always represented in the antennal lobe as the sum of their parts due to mixture interactions. Because of this, odour blends can acquire a unique quality, where sometimes not all information about the individual components is perceptually available anymore.Citation27 A map of the antennal lobe, including activation patterns of many different odorants and their mixtures would be required to test this hypothesis in ants. Unfortunately, due to the complex morphology of the ant antennal lobe, full mapping remains extremely challenging.Citation28,Citation29

Another potential cause for the inclusion criterion arising at the perception component could be inhibition due to overshadowing. Overshadowing occurs when an individual encounters a mixture, but only perceives a subset of that mixture.Citation30 For example, if ants trained to e.g., 2-hexanone are tested with a mixture of 2-hexanone and octanol, reaction might be inhibited if octanol overshadows 2-hexanone (i.e., the ant mainly perceives octanol, even though 2-hexanone is also present in the mixture). However, this seems to be an unlikely cause of our results, as we also conducted an overshadowing experiment by training ants to a mixture and afterwards testing their reaction to the individual components, which suggested that overshadowing is rare in C. aethiops.Citation9

Instead of the perception component, the inclusion criterion could arise at the action component. This would imply that the animal receives all the information needed in order to discriminate between different substances, but follows certain decision rules resulting in asymmetrical generalisation. Our experiment showed that ants could perceive each tested substance,Citation9 making it likely that the action component is the cause for the inclusion criterion. However, as previous experience to a single substance can adjust processes in the perception component (through for example sensory adaptationCitation31), more experimental data are needed in order to tease apart the two components from each other.

Conclusions

The question remains on how widespread the inclusion criterion is. More species should be studied in order to find out whether our study species (Camponotus aethiops) is unique with respect to the inclusion criterion or not. As learning paradigms are well established for many different animals,Citation12,Citation32-Citation35 a systematic investigation, including many substances of different chain-lengths and functional groups, of the reaction of individuals to a mixture of the conditioned stimulus and a novel odorant would elucidate whether the inclusion criterion is widespread or not. If the ant is indeed the exception and one of the only groups of animals that uses the inclusion criterion, this would suggest (but not prove) that it arises from the action component, as the olfactory pathway (which is part of the perception component) is well preserved between different groups of animals.Citation36

Whether the inclusion criterion is adaptive or not is not clear. One potential benefit for ants could be during recognition of nestmates vs. non-nestmates. As their cuticular chemical profile consists of many different substances, the inclusion criterion could ‘compact’ this profile into something simpler to interpret by treating multiple different substances as similar. This hypothesis is supported by recent evidence in the Argentine ant (Linipithema humile).Citation37 Ants were exposed to a filter paper containing their own nestmate odour, to which they were not aggressive. When one of eight synthetic hydrocarbons (differing in branch position, chain length, or both) was added to this filter paper, the ants started to be aggressive. The ants displayed different levels of aggression depending on which hydrocarbon was added. The authors found functional homologs, where hydrocarbons differing in chain length (but not in the position of the functional group) received equal levels aggression, whereas this was not the case when comparing aggression levels against hydrocarbons differing in the position of the functional group. This could indicate that these functional homologs are ‘compacted’ into a single peak during the perception component.

Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest

No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Heli Havukainen, Heikki Helanterä, Patrizia d’Ettorre, Liselotte Sundström, as well as two anonymous referees for their useful comments and discussions. Thanks to the Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions for allowing allocation of working time and salary (provided by project grant 251337) on the writing of this manuscript.

References

  • Starks PT. Recognition systems : From components to conservation. Ann Zool Fenn 2004; 41:689 - 90
  • Stevens M. Sensory ecology, behaviour, & evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013.
  • de Bruyne M, Baker TC. Odor detection in insects: volatile codes. J Chem Ecol 2008; 34:882 - 97; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10886-008-9485-4; PMID: 18535862
  • Hildebrand JG, Shepherd GM. Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: converging evidence for common principles across phyla. Annu Rev Neurosci 1997; 20:595 - 631; http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.20.1.595; PMID: 9056726
  • Vosshall LB, Stocker RF. Molecular architecture of smell and taste in Drosophila. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007; 30:505 - 33; http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094306; PMID: 17506643
  • Kulmuni J, Havukainen H. Insights into the evolution of the CSP gene family through the integration of evolutionary analysis and comparative protein modeling. PLoS One 2013; 8:e63688; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063688; PMID: 23723994
  • Kulmuni J, Wurm Y, Pamilo P. Comparative genomics of chemosensory protein genes reveals rapid evolution and positive selection in ant-specific duplicates. Heredity (Edinb) 2013; 110:538 - 47; http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/hdy.2012.122; PMID: 23403962
  • Bos N, Dreier S, Jørgensen CG, Nielsen J, Guerrieri FJ, d’Ettorre P. Learning and perceptual similarity among cuticular hydrocarbons in ants. J Insect Physiol 2012; 58:138 - 46; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jinsphys.2011.10.010; PMID: 22067290
  • Bos N, d’Ettorre P, Guerrieri FJ. Chemical structure of odorants and perceptual similarity in ants. J Exp Biol 2013; 216:3314 - 20; http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.087007; PMID: 23685976
  • Guerrieri FJ, d’Ettorre P. Associative learning in ants: conditioning of the maxilla-labium extension response in Camponotus aethiops. J Insect Physiol 2010; 56:88 - 92; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jinsphys.2009.09.007; PMID: 19800887
  • Smith BH, Menzel R. The use of electromyogram recordings to quantify odourant discrimination in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. J Insect Physiol 1989; 35:369 - 75; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(89)90110-8
  • Linster C, Smith BH. Generalization between binary odor mixtures and their components in the rat. Physiol Behav 1999; 66:701 - 7; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00007-4; PMID: 10386917
  • Sandoz JC, Pham-Delègue MH, Renou M, Wadhams LJ. Asymmetrical generalisation between pheromonal and floral odours in appetitive olfactory conditioning of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A 2001; 187:559 - 68; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s003590100228; PMID: 11730303
  • Laloi D, Pham-Delège M. Bumble Bees Show Asymmetrical Discrimination Between Two Odors in a Classical. J Insect Behav 2004; 17:385 - 96; http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JOIR.0000031538.15346.e1
  • Guerrieri F, Schubert M, Sandoz J-C, Giurfa M. Perceptual and neural olfactory similarity in honeybees. PLoS Biol 2005; 3:e60; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030060; PMID: 15736975
  • Guerrieri FJ, Nehring V, Jørgensen CG, Nielsen J, Galizia CG, d’Ettorre P. Ants recognize foes and not friends. Proc Biol Sci 2009; 276:2461 - 8; http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1860; PMID: 19364750
  • Van Zweden J, d’Ettorre P. Nestmate recognition in social insects and the role of hydrocarbons. In: Bagneres AG, Blomquist GJ, editors. Insect Hydrocarbons: Biology, Biohemistry and Chemical Ecology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2010. page 222–43.
  • Lenoir A, D’Ettorre P, Errard C, Hefetz A. Chemical ecology and social parasitism in ants. Annu Rev Entomol 2001; 46:573 - 99; http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.573; PMID: 11112180
  • Löbel D, Marchese S, Krieger J, Pelosi P, Breer H. Subtypes of odorant-binding proteins--heterologous expression and ligand binding. Eur J Biochem 1998; 254:318 - 24; http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1432-1327.1998.2540318.x; PMID: 9660186
  • Jiang Q-Y, Wang W-X, Zhang Z, Zhang L. Binding specificity of locust odorant binding protein and its key binding site for initial recognition of alcohols. Insect Biochem Mol Biol 2009; 39:440 - 7; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ibmb.2009.04.004; PMID: 19376226
  • Dani FR, Iovinella I, Felicioli A, Niccolini A, Calvello MA, Carucci MG, Qiao H, Pieraccini G, Turillazzi S, Moneti G, et al. Mapping the expression of soluble olfactory proteins in the honeybee. J Proteome Res 2010; 9:1822 - 33; http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/pr900969k; PMID: 20155982
  • Dwyer DS, Bradley RJ. Chemical properties of alcohols and their protein binding sites. Cell Mol Life Sci 2000; 57:265 - 75; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/PL00000689; PMID: 10766022
  • Joerges J, Küttner A, Galizia CG, Menzer R. Representations of odours and odour mixtures visualized in the honeybee brain. Nature 1997; 387:285 - 8; http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/387285a0
  • Sachse S, Rappert A, Galizia CG. The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: steps towards the olfactory code. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11:3970 - 82; http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00826.x; PMID: 10583486
  • Semmelhack JL, Wang JW. Select Drosophila glomeruli mediate innate olfactory attraction and aversion. Nature 2009; 459:218 - 23; http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature07983; PMID: 19396157
  • Galizia CG, Menzel R. The role of glomeruli in the neural representation of odours: results from optical recording studies. J Insect Physiol 2001; 47:115 - 30; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1910(00)00106-2; PMID: 11064019
  • Silbering AF, Galizia CG. Processing of odor mixtures in the Drosophila antennal lobe reveals both global inhibition and glomerulus-specific interactions. J Neurosci 2007; 27:11966 - 77; http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3099-07.2007; PMID: 17978037
  • Galizia CG, Menzel R, Holldobler B. Optical imaging of odor-evoked glomerular activity patterns in the antennal lobes of the ant camponotus rufipes. Naturwissenschaften 1999; 86:533 - 7; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s001140050669; PMID: 10551948
  • Brandstaetter AS, Rössler W, Kleineidam CJ. Friends and foes from an ant brain’s point of view--neuronal correlates of colony odors in a social insect. PLoS One 2011; 6:e21383; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021383; PMID: 21731724
  • Kay LM, Crk T, Thorngate J. A redefinition of odor mixture quality. Behav Neurosci 2005; 119:726 - 33; http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.726; PMID: 15998193
  • Bhagavan S, Smith BH. Olfactory conditioning in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: effects of odor intensity. Physiol Behav 1997; 61:107 - 17; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00357-5; PMID: 8976540
  • Davis RL. Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. Annu Rev Neurosci 2005; 28:275 - 302; http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135651; PMID: 16022597
  • Giurfa M. Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the honeybee: a taste from the magic well. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2007; 193:801 - 24; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00359-007-0235-9; PMID: 17639413
  • Hoedjes KM, Kruidhof HM, Huigens ME, Dicke M, Vet LEM, Smid HM. Natural variation in learning rate and memory dynamics in parasitoid wasps: opportunities for converging ecology and neuroscience. Proc Biol Sci 2011; 278:889 - 97; http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2199; PMID: 21106587
  • Ghirlanda S, Enquist M. One century of generalization. Anim Behav 2003; 66:15 - 36; http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2174
  • Chittka L, Brockmann A. Perception space--the final frontier. PLoS Biol 2005; 3:e137; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030137; PMID: 15819608
  • van Wilgenburg E, Sulc R, Shea KJ, Tsutsui ND. Deciphering the chemical basis of nestmate recognition. J Chem Ecol 2010; 36:751 - 8; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10886-010-9812-4; PMID: 20556636