1,109
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Article Addendum

The role of dopamine and serotonin in conditioned food aversion learning in the honeybee

Pages 318-320 | Received 13 Jan 2011, Accepted 13 Jan 2011, Published online: 01 May 2011

Abstract

For most animals, eating entails the risk of being poisoned. Learning how to identify foods with toxins is an important mechanism that reduces the risk of poisoning. While conditioned food aversions have been studied in vertebrates for over 50 years, the neural circuits underlying this form of learning have been difficult to elucidate because of their complexity. Insects, such as fruit flies and honeybees, are important models for the study of the neural mechanisms of learning and memory, but conditioned food aversions have not yet been reported from either species. My collaborators and I recently established that the honeybee has the ability to learn to avoid odours associated with toxins in food using two independent neural pathways. In these experiments, we found that honeybees can learn to associate scents with toxins that they can pre-ingestively detect using their proboscis. This form of learning is primarily mediated by the neurotransmitter, dopamine. We also found a second mechanism: bees can learn to avoid odours associated with the malaise caused by ingesting toxins. This form of learning is mediated by serotonin. Our data are the first to show that two different mechanisms account for conditioned food aversions in insects. 

This article refers to:

Learning to avoid eating toxic food is as important as learning to find and obtain nutrition. Central to this form of learning is the ability to integrate sensory information with changes in state caused by intoxication. Although conditioned food aversions have been studied for the past 50 years in vertebrates, the molecular mechanisms of the neural circuits underpinning this form of learning have proven difficult to pinpoint.Citation1 Several neuromodulators such as dopamine (DA) and serotonin (5HT), as well as glutamate and endocannabinoids, participate in the acquisition and consolidation of a conditioned food aversionCitation1Citation3 but their exact roles as pre- or post-ingestive signaling mechanisms in these neural circuits remain unclear. Determining whether these pathways are functionally independent and mediated by different neuromodulators is fundamental to our understanding of the way that the brain evaluates the quality of ingested food.

Invertebrates such as nematodes, sea slugs, fruit flies and honeybees have simpler brains—using them as model systems has made it possible to dissect the way that neuromodulators function during learning.Citation4Citation7 In flies and bees, octopamine (OA) is the primary neuromodulator of circuits involved in appetitive learning,Citation8,Citation9 whereas learned avoidance of odors signaling electric shock is mediated by DA.Citation9Citation11 A recent study of food aversion learning in the nematode C. elegans demonstrated that nematodes without 5HT receptors failed to learn to avoid odors associated with the ingestion of pathogenic bacteria.Citation12 Considerably less is known about the role of 5HT in appetitive and aversive learning in insects, however. Although conditioned food aversions have been demonstrated in slugs,Citation13 cuttlefish,Citation14 mantidsCitation15 and grasshoppers,Citation16 they have not yet been reported in important insect models such as fruit flies or honeybees.

The honeybee, an olfactory-learner par excellence, is a mutualist with its food source and as such, rarely encounters food containing toxins. In fact, the honeybee possesses only 10 genes encoding gustatory receptors, perhaps indicating that it is limited in its ability to use bitter taste to pre-ingestively detect toxins.Citation17 Free-flying honeybees will, however, avoid consuming sucrose solutions containing caffeine, nicotine and amygdalin.Citation18,Citation19 How they perform this, and whether or not they can learn to avoid cues associated with the presence of toxins, has not yet been tested.

Using a well-characterized assay for olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension reflex (PER) in the honeybee,Citation20 we found that honeybees can detect bitter compounds such as quinine and learn to avoid odors associated with its presence in sucrose solution.Citation21 Our data are the first to establish that the bee has gustatory neurons that respond to toxins such as quinine and amygdalin on the proboscis. Furthermore, by mixing these compounds with sucrose, we found that the detection of toxins requires the activity of several classes of neurons, including those that respond most to sucrose alone. This suggests that the ability to detect toxins, at least in insects, is not a labeled line code in specific ‘bitter’ sensing neurons. Instead, our data support the hypothesis that the perception of bitter requires input from several classes of neurons simultaneously.

By presenting two toxins—the strongly bitter toxin, quinine and the difficult-todetect toxin, amygdalin—in a rewarding sucrose solution during olfactory conditioning, we established that learned olfactory avoidances that depend on bitter gustatory input occur rapidly whereas learning that relies on the malaise that occurs after toxin ingestion occurs on a slower time course. Learning to avoid odors associated with bitter substances develops within 5 min of the first learning trial and relies on input gustatory neurons located on the proboscis. This is in contrast to learned olfactory aversions arising after toxins are ingested: in this case, the post-ingestive pathway for learned inhibition of PER is expressed 20–40 min after initial toxin ingestion. The slow nature of this learned avoidance presumably arises because of physiological stress caused by toxin ingestion that could be signaled by the gut. Alternatively, if the toxin itself passed via the midgut into the hemolymph, it could be detected by neural tissues elsewhere in the body. This occurs in vertebrates, where lithium chloride, a salt often used to produce conditioned malaise by injection, activates the vagus nerve.Citation22 Whether malaise is signaled by afferents projecting from the gut to the brain in insects is unknown.

These data clearly show that at least two pathways for learned suppression of PER exist in the honeybee: a pre-ingestive pathway that relies on gustatory input and a post-ingestive one arising after food ingestion. Importantly, our work also establishes that these two pathways are mediated by different neurotransmitters. Recent studies in crickets and in larval fruit flies have demonstrated that DA mediates learned avoidances of visual or olfactory cues associated with salt or quinine applied to the mouthparts.Citation23Citation25 To see if this was also the case in the honeybee, we blocked DA, OA and 5HT receptors during differential conditioning with quinine and also observed that learned avoidances of bitter substances are primarily modulated by DA. In contrast, learning to associate odors with the malaise caused by ingesting amygdalin is mediated by 5HT. Our data are the first to identify a role for 5HT during food avoidance learning in an insect. We speculate that 5HT modulates the circuit that controls conditioned proboscis extension in honeybees. Identifying the mechanism that links the gut and brain during malaise learning remains to be determined.

Pollinators, such as honeybees, provide a vital service to plants by transporting pollen between the flowers of conspecifics. Plants generally produce toxins as a means of preventing herbivory; it is, therefore, counterintuitive that nectar and pollen contain toxins as they are the rewards offered to pollinators for visiting. However, many species do have toxins in their nectar such as the almond tree which produces nectar containing amygdalin.Citation18,Citation26,Citation27 Our data show that the honeybee's ability to pre-ingestively detect toxins in sucrose solution depends on the chemical nature of the toxin, as some toxins are much more difficult to detect. Toxins in nectar could afford plants a selective advantage if they repelled nectar thieves but not pollinators. Even if pollinators post-ingestively learned that they had been poisoned, the time course of this form of learning is slower. A pollinator could, therefore, transfer pollen between plants before it was aware that it had been poisoned and so facilitate selection for this trait in a population of plants.

Abbreviations

OA=

octopamine

DA=

dopamine

5HT=

serotonin

PER=

proboscis extension reflex

Acknowledgements

The work was funded in part by small grants from Association for the Study of Animal Behavior and the Wellcome Trust.

Addendum to:

References

  • Reilly SS. Reilly SS, Schactmann TR. Central gustatory system lesions and conditioned taste aversion. Conditioned Taste Aversion: Behavioral and Neural Processes 2009; Oxford OUP 309 - 327
  • Bures J, Bermudez-Rattoni F, Yamamoto T. Conditioned taste aversion: memory of a special kind 1998; Oxford Oxford University Press
  • Mosher TM, Smith JG, Greenshaw AJ. Aversive stimulus properties of the 5-HT2c receptor agonist WAY 161503 in rats. Neuropharmacology 2006; 51:641 - 650
  • Clarac F, Pearlstein E. Invertebrate preparations and their contribution to neurobiology in the second half of the 20th century. Brain Res Rev 2007; 54:113 - 161
  • Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Bailey CH. Molecular mechanisms of memory storage in Aplysia. Biol Bull 2006; 210:174 - 191
  • Davis RL. Olfactory learning. Neuron 2004; 44:31 - 44
  • Menzel R. Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee. Learn Mem 2001; 8:53 - 62
  • Hammer M. An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees. Nature 1993; 366:59 - 63
  • Schwaerzel M, Monastirioti M, Scholz H, Friggi-Grelin F, Birman S, Heisenberg M. Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci 2003; 23:10495 - 10502
  • Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell 2009; 139:416 - 427
  • Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Aversive learning in honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex. PLoS One 2007; 2:288
  • Zhang Y, Lu H, Bargmann CI. Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 2005; 438:179 - 184
  • Gelperin A. Rapid food-aversion learning by a terrestrial mollusk. Science 1975; 189:567 - 570
  • Darmaillacq AS, Chichery R, Dickel L. Food imprinting, new evidence from the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Biol Lett 2006; 2:345 - 347
  • Berenbaum MR, Miliczky E. Mantids and milkweed bugs—efficacy of aposematic coloration against invertebrate predators. Am Midland Nat 1984; 111:64 - 68
  • Bernays EA, Lee JC. Food aversion learning in the polyphagous grasshopper Schistocerca-americana. Physiol Ent 1988; 13:131 - 137
  • Robertson HM, Wanner KM. The chemoreceptor superfamily in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: Expansion of the odorant, but not gustatory, receptor family. Genome Res 2006; 16:1395 - 1403
  • London-Shafir I, Shafir S, Eisikowitch D. Amygdalin in almond nectar and pollen—facts and possible roles. Plant Syst Evol 2003; 238:87 - 95
  • Singaravelan N, Nee'man G, Inbar M, Izhaki I. Feeding responses of free-flying honeybees to secondary compounds mimicking floral nectars. J Chem Ecol 2005; 31:2791 - 2804
  • Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schafer S. Classical-conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psych 1983; 97:107 - 119
  • Wright GA, Mustard JA, Simcock N, Ross-Taylor AAR, McNicholas LD, Popescu A, et al. Parallel reinforcement pathways for conditioned food aversions in the honeybee. Curr Biol 2010; 20:2234 - 2240
  • Niijima A, Yamamoto T. The effects of lithium chloride on the activity of the afferent nerve fibers from the abdominal visceral organs in the rat. Brain ResBull 1994; 35:141 - 145
  • Honjo K, Furukubo-Tokunaga K. Distinctive neuronal networks and biochemical pathways for appetitive and aversive memory in Drosophila larvae. J Neurosci 2009; 29:852 - 862
  • Selcho M, Pauls D, Han KA, Stocker RF, Thum AS. The role of dopamine in Drosophila larval classical olfactory conditioning. PLoS One 2009; 4:5897
  • Unoki S, Matsumoto Y, Mizunami M. Participation of octopaminergic reward system and dopaminergic punishment system in insect olfactory learning revealed by pharmacological study. Eur J Neurosci 2005; 22:1409 - 1416
  • Adler LS. The ecological significance of toxic nectar. Oikos 2000; 91:409 - 420
  • Detzel A, Wink M. Attraction, deterrence or intoxication of bees (Apis mellifera) by plant allelochemicals. Chemoecology 1992; 4:8 - 18