157
Views
26
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

When Instructional Guidance is Needed

, &
Pages 149-162 | Received 05 Jul 2016, Accepted 11 Sep 2016, Published online: 19 Oct 2020

References

  • Atkinson, DerryS.J., RenklA., & WorthamD. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.
  • AusubelD.P. (1964). Education and the structure of knowledge. Chicago: Rand McNally.
  • ChenO., KalyugaS., & SwellerJ. (2015). The worked example effect, the generation effect, and element interactivity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 689–704.
  • ChenO., KalyugaS., & SwellerJ. (2016a). Relations between the worked example and generation effects on immediate and delayed tests. Learning and Instruction, 45, 20–30.
  • ChenO., KalyugaS., & SwellerJ. (2016b). The expertise reversal effect is a variant of the more general element interactivity effect. Educational Psychology Review. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10648-016-9359-1
  • CooperG., & SwellerJ. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 347–362.
  • CowanN. (2001). Metatheory of storage capacity limits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 154–176.
  • DonaldsonW., & BassM. (1980). Relational information and memory for problem solutions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 26–35.
  • EricssonK.A., & KintschW. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.
  • GriffithD. (1976). The attentional demands of mnemonic control processes. Memory & Cognition, 4, 103–108.
  • KalyugaS. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509–539.
  • KalyugaS. (2011). Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 1–19.
  • KarpickeJ.D., & ZarombF.M. (2010). Retrieval mode distinguishes the testing effect from the generation effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 227–239.
  • KirschnerP., SwellerJ., & ClarkR. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.
  • KyunS., KalyugaS., & SwellerJ. (2013). The effect of worked examples when learning to write essays in English literature. Journal of Experimental Education, 81, 385–408. doi:10.1080/00220973.2012.727884
  • LeahyW., HanhamJ., & SwellerJ. (2015). High element interactivity information during problem solving may lead to failure to obtain the testing effect. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 291–304.
  • MayerR.E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? American Psychologist, 59, 14–19.
  • McFarlandC.E., FreyT.J., & RhodesD.D. (1980). Retrieval of internally versus externally generated words in episodic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 210–225.
  • MillerG.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
  • PaasF. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.
  • PaasF., & Van MerriënboerJ.J. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.
  • PeynircioğluZ.F. (1989). The generation effect with pictures and nonsense figures. Acta Psychologica, 70, 153–160.
  • ReissleinJ., AtkinsonR.K., SeelingP., & ReissleinM. (2006). Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis. Learning and Instruction, 16, 92–103.
  • RenklA. (2014a). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 1–37.
  • RenklA. (2014b). The worked-out examples principle in multimedia learning. In R.E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • RourkeA., & SwellerJ. (2009). The worked-example effect using ill-defined problems: Learning to recognise designers’ styles. Learning and Instruction, 19, 185–199.
  • SchweickertR., McDanielM.A., & RieglerG. (1994). Effects of generation on immediate memory span and delayed unexpected free recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 781–804.
  • SchwonkeR., RenklA., KriegC., WittwerJ., AlevenV., & SaldenR. (2009). The worked-example effect: Not an artefact of lousy control conditions. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 258–266.
  • SlameckaN.J., & GrafP. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.
  • SwellerJ. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.
  • SwellerJ. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.
  • SwellerJ., AyresP., & KalyugaS. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.
  • SwellerJ., & ChandlerP. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 185–233.
  • SwellerJ., & CooperG.A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89.
  • SwellerJ., Van MerrienboerJ.J., & PaasF.G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.
  • Tindall-FordS., ChandlerP., & SwellerJ. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 257–287.
  • Van GogT., KesterL., & PaasF. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 212–218.
  • Van GogT., PaasF., & van MerriënboerJ.J. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16, 154–164.
  • Van GogT., & SwellerJ. (2015). Not new but nearly forgotten: The testing effect decreases or even disappears as the complexity of learning materials increases. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 247–264.
  • Van MerriënboerJ.J., KesterL., & PaasF. (2006). Teaching complex rather than simple tasks: Balancing intrinsic and germane load to enhance transfer of learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 343–352.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.